Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-ndmmz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-16T08:30:55.063Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

12 - Variability of Language Aptitude and Working Memory in EFL Learners’ Listening Development

from Part III - Innovative Perspectives and Paradigms

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 May 2023

Zhisheng (Edward) Wen
Affiliation:
Hong Kong Shue Yan University
Peter Skehan
Affiliation:
Institute of Education, University of London
Richard L. Sparks
Affiliation:
Mount St Joseph University
Get access

Summary

Variability is a typical and inherent property of complex and dynamic systems. As a follow-up to Chang and Zhang’s (2021) study on inter- and intra-individual variability in EFL learners’ listening development, the present longitudinal study focused on exploring the variability of participants’ language aptitude and working memory, two prominent cognitive variables of learner individual differences (LIDs). Three EFL learners participated in 10 observations spanning 43 months and were requested to complete the IELTS, LLAMA, and a listening span test (LST). Time-series data of learners’ language aptitude and working memory were analyzed through CDST techniques such as moving min–max graphs and Monte Carlo Analysis. In our study, considerable and significant variability in language aptitude and working memory were identified not only between different learners but also within each learner’s dynamic learning process at different levels. The chapter concludes with a discussion on the importance of researching L2 learners’ inter- and intra-variability in understanding the dynamic nature of language aptitude and working memory from the CDST perspective.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2023

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(11), 417423.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 8, 4789.Google Scholar
Brunfaut, T., Kormos, J., Michel, M., & Ratajczak, M. (2021). Testing young foreign language learners’ reading comprehension: Exploring the effects of working memory, grade level, and reading task. Language Testing, 38(3), 356377.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. (1963). A model of school learning. Teachers College Record, 64, 723733.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B., & Sapon, S. M. (1959, 2002). Modern Language Aptitude Test. New York, NY: Psychological Corporation (reprinted in 2002).Google Scholar
Chan, H., Verspoor, M., & Vahtrick, L. (2015). Dynamic development in speaking versus writing in identical twins. Language Learning, 65, 298325.Google Scholar
Chang, P., & Zhang, L. J. (2020). Idiodynamic research into EFL listeners’ directed motivational currents. Modern Foreign Languages, 43(2), 200212.Google Scholar
Chang, P., & Zhang, L. J. (2021). A CDST perspective on variability in foreign language learners’ listening development. Frontiers in Psychology, 12(601962), 117.Google Scholar
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 19(4), 450466.Google Scholar
de Bot, K. (2015). Rates of change: Time scales in second language development. In Dörnyei, Z., MacIntyre, P. D., & Henry, A. (eds.), Motivational Dynamics in Language Learning. Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters, pp. 2937.Google Scholar
de Bot, K., Lowie, W., & Vespoor, M. (2005). Second Language Acquisition: An Advanced Resource Book. New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Dörnyei, Z. (2010). The relationship between language aptitude and language learning motivation: Individual differences from a dynamic systems perspective. In Macaro, E. (ed.), Continuum Companion to Second Language Acquisition. London, UK: Continuum, pp. 247267.Google Scholar
Dörnyei, Z., Ibrahim, Z., & Muir, C. (2015). Directed motivational currents: Regulating complex dynamic systems through motivational surges. In Dörnyei, Z., MacIntyre, P. D., & Henry, A. (eds.), Motivational Dynamics in Language Learning. Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters, pp. 95105.Google Scholar
Evans, D. R., & Larsen-Freeman, D. (2020). Bifurcations and the emergence of L2 syntactic structures in a complex dynamic system. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, (574603), 1–12.Google Scholar
Gass, S. M., & Mackey, A. (2017). Stimulated Recall Methodology in Applied Linguistics and L2 Research. New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Granena, G. (2013). Cognitive aptitudes for second language learning and the LLAMA language aptitude test. In Granena, G. & Long, M. (eds.), Sensitive Periods, Language Aptitude and Ultimate L2 Attainment. Philadelphia, PA: Benjamins, pp. 105129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hiver, P., & Al-Hoorie, A. H. (2020). Research Methods for Complexity Theory in Applied Linguistics. Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Hood, G. (2009). Poptools [Computer software]. Canberra, Australia: Pest Animal Control Co-operative Research Center (CSIRO).Google Scholar
Huang, T., Steinkrauss, R., & Verspoor, M. (2021). Variability as predictor in L2 writing proficiency. Journal of Second Language Writing, 52: 1–14.Google Scholar
Kormos, J. (2013). New conceptualizations of language aptitude in second language attainment. In Granena, G. & Long, M. (eds.), Sensitive Periods, Language Aptitude and Ultimate L2 Attainment. Philadelphia, PA: Benjamins, pp. 131152.Google Scholar
Kormos, J., & Sáfár, A. (2008). Phonological short-term memory, working memory and foreign language performance in intensive language learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11(2), 261271.Google Scholar
Larsen-Freeman, D. (2006). The emergence of complexity, fluency, and accuracy in the oral and written production of five Chinese learners of English. Applied Linguistics, 27 , 590619.Google Scholar
Larsen-Freeman, D. (2020). Complex dynamic systems theory. In van Pattern, W., Keating, G., & Wulf, S. (eds.), Theories of Second Language Acquisition: An Introduction. New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Larsen-Freeman, D., & Cameron, L. (2008). Research methodology on language development from a complex systems perspective. Modern Language Journal, 92(2), 200213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, K., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2002). Macro- and microdevelopmental research assumptions, research strategies, constraints and utilities. In Granott, N. & Parziale, J. (eds.), Microdevelopment: Transition Processes in Development and Learning. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 243265.Google Scholar
Li, S. (2015). The associations between language aptitude and second language grammar acquisition: A meta-analytic review of five decades of research. Applied Linguistics, 36 , 385408.Google Scholar
Lowie, W. (2017). Methodological considerations in complex dynamic theory approaches to second language development research. In Ortega, L. & Han, Z. (eds.), Complexity Theory and Language Development: In Celebration of Diane Larsen-Freeman. Philadelphia, PA: Benjamins, pp. 123141.Google Scholar
Lowie, W., & Verspoor, M. (2015). Variability and variation in second language acquisition orders: A dynamic re-evaluation. Language Learning, 65, 6388.Google Scholar
Lowie, W., & Verspoor, M. (2018). Individual differences and the ergodicity problem. Language Learning, 69 , 184206.Google Scholar
Meara, P. (2005). LLAMA Language Aptitude Test. Swansea, UK: Lognostics.Google Scholar
Niżegorodcew, A. (2012). L2 learners’ individual differences and the changing SLA perspective. In Pawlak, M. (ed.), New Perspectives on Individual Differences in Language Learning and Teaching. New York, NY: Springer, pp. 313.Google Scholar
Robinson, P. (2005). Aptitude and second language acquisition. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 25 , 4673.Google Scholar
Skehan, P. (1989). Individual Differences in Second-Language Learning. London, UK: Arnold.Google Scholar
Spoelman, M., & Verspoor, M. (2010). Dynamic patterns in development of accuracy and complexity: A longitudinal case study in the acquisition of Finnish. Applied Linguistics, 31(4), 532553.Google Scholar
Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (1994). A Dynamic Systems Approach to the Development of Cognition and Action. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
van Dijk, M., Verspoor, M., & Lowie, W. (2011). Variability and DST. In Verspoor, M., de Bot, K., & Lowie, W. (eds.), A Dynamic Approach to Second Language Development: Methods and Techniques. Philadelphia, PA: Benjamins, pp. 5584.Google Scholar
van Geert, P., Steenbeek, H., & Kunnen, S. (2012). Monte Carlo techniques: Statistical simulation for developmental data. In Kunnen, S. (ed.), A Dynamic Systems Approach to Adolescent Development (pp. 4353). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
van Geert, P., & van Dijk, M. (2002). Focus on variability: New tools to study intra-individual variability in developmental data. Infant Behavior and Development, 25, 340374.Google Scholar
Verspoor, M., de Bot, K., & Lowie, W. (eds.) (2011). A Dynamic Approach to Second Language Development: Methods and Techniques. Philadelphia, PA: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Verspoor, M., Lowie, W., Chan, H., & Vahtrick, L. (2017). Linguistic complexity in second language development: Variability and variation at advanced stages. Recherches En Didactique Des Langues et Des Cultures Les Cahiers de l’Acedle, 14(1), 127.Google Scholar
Verspoor, M., Lowie, W., & van Dijk, M. (2008). Variability in second language development from a dynamic systems perspective. Modern Language Journal, 92(2), 214231.Google Scholar
Wen, Z. (2016). Phonological and executive working memory in L2 task-based speech performance. Language Learning Journal, 44(4), 418435.Google Scholar
Wen, Z. (2019). Working memory as language aptitude: The Phonological/Executive Model. In Wen, Z., Skehan, P., Biedroń, A., Li, S., & Sparks, R. (eds.), Language Aptitude: Advancing Theory, Testing, Research and Practice. New York, NY: Routledge, pp. 187214.Google Scholar
Wen, Z., Biedroń, A., & Skehan, P. (2017). Foreign language aptitude theory: Yesterday, today and tomorrow. Language Teaching, 50(1), 131.Google Scholar
Wen, Z., & Juffs, A. (2020). Testing working memory in SLA. In Winke, P. & Brunfaut, T. (eds.), The Routledge Handbook of Second Language Acquisition and Testing. London, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Wen, Z., & Li, S. (2019). Working memory in L2 learning and processing. In Schwieter, J. W. & Benati, A. (eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Language Learning. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 365389.Google Scholar
Wen, Z., & Skehan, P. (2021). Stages of acquisition and the P/E model of working memory: Complementary or contrasting approaches to foreign language aptitude? Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 41 , 624.Google Scholar
Zhang, L. J. (2016). A dynamic metacognitive systems perspective on language learner autonomy. In Barnard, R. & Li, J. (eds.), Language Learner Autonomy: Teachers’ Beliefs and Practices in East Asian Contexts. Phnom Penh, Cambodia: IDP Education, pp. 150166.Google Scholar
Zhang, L. J., & Zhang, D. (2013). Thinking metacognitively about metacognition in second and foreign language learning, teaching, and research: Toward a dynamic metacognitive systems perspective. Contemporary Foreign Languages Studies, 396 , 111121.Google Scholar
Zhao, H., & Luo, S. (2020). A meta-analysis of the impact of working memory capacity on Chinese EFL learners’ L2 proficiency. Modern Foreign Languages, 43(4), 553564.Google Scholar
Zheng, Y. (2016). The complex, dynamic development of L2 lexical use: A longitudinal study on Chinese learners of English. System, 56, 4053.Google Scholar
Zheng, Y., & Feng, Y. (2017). A dynamic systems study on Chinese EFL learners’ syntactic and lexical complexity development. Modern Foreign Languages, 40(1), 5768.Google Scholar
Zheng, Y., Lu, X., & Ren, W. (2020). Tracking the evolution of Chinese learners’ multilingual motivation through a longitudinal Q methodology. Modern Language Journal, 104(4), 781803.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×