Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-cnmwb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-19T22:18:23.902Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 7 - Explaining crosslinguistic preferences

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 February 2013

Edith A. Moravcsik
Affiliation:
University of Wisconsin, Milwaukee
Get access

Summary

Chapter outline

The concept of explanation is illustrated by explaining the structure of an English sentence. The three-step process is then applied to the explanation of crosslinguistically recurrent patterns. The postscript adds a few general comments about the study of language typology.

Introduction

Why are crosslinguistically recurrent patterns the way they are? In the preceding chapters, a few explanatory comments have already been made about some of them. The genesis of articles was explained by the general process of grammaticalization (Chapter 6, Section 6.2.1). The distribution of resumptive pronouns in relative clauses was accounted for by reference to the varying degrees of difficulty of relative clause constructions, which in turn stem from processing problems (Chapter 6, Section 6.3.2.2 and 6.4.1, Chapter 3, Section 3.2.1.1). The preference for homorganic consonant clusters was explained by auditory similarity among nasal consonants (Chapter 5, Section 5.2.1). The reason for the predominance of suffixing over prefixing was given by the history of affixation and by processing preferences (Chapter 4, Section 4.3). The predominance of the accusative and ergative alignment patterns over the other logical possibilities was explained by the need for clarity and economy (Chapter 3, Section 3.2.2.2). The crosslinguistic frequency of certain color terms was causally linked to cultural factors (Chapter 2, Section 2.5.2). And markedness was invoked for explaining a variety of patterns, such as asymmetries in body-part terms, kinship terminology, personal pronouns, number words, and antonymic adjectives (Chapter 2, Section 2.2–2.5), the distribution of zero forms (Chapter 4, Section 4.2.2.2), and some phonological patterns (Chapter 5, Section 5.3).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2012

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×