Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-skm99 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T21:31:33.633Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

12 - Lizards – Measuring Cognition: Practical Challenges and the Influence of Ecology and Social Behaviour

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 July 2018

Nereida Bueno-Guerra
Affiliation:
Comillas Pontifical University
Federica Amici
Affiliation:
Universität Leipzig
Get access

Summary

In the past few decades, there has been a growing appreciation for the cognitive ability of lizards. Recent work has demonstrated that lizards show behavioural flexibility, social learning and may use imitation. Importantly, much of lizard ecology and behaviour is temperature dependent and the thermal environment during embryonic development can have a profound effect on phenotype. Lizards are generally relatively easy to maintain in captivity but, across the phylogeny, have been shown to differ fundamentally in their ecology and behaviour. For example, many clades are hard-wired for foraging mode (e.g. ambush vs active foragers) and with this, are differences in the ability to detect prey chemicals. These fundamental differences can affect the choice of a suitable cognitive test. Likewise, sex, reproductive state and social dominance may also affect learning ability. In this chapter, we will describe relevant aspects of lizard biology and then briefly review the literature on lizard cognition. Finally, we will describe practical approaches to studying cognition in the lab and field including how the data should be collected, managed and analysed.
Type
Chapter
Information
Field and Laboratory Methods in Animal Cognition
A Comparative Guide
, pp. 266 - 285
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2018

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

Begun, D., Kubie, J. L., Plough O’Keefe, M., and Halpern, M. (1988). Conditioned discrimination of airborne odorants by garter snakes. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 102, 3543.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crawford, F., and Bartlett, C. (1966). Runway behavior of the gray rat snake with food and water reinforcement. Psychonomic Science, 4, 99100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emer, S. A., Mora, C. V., Harvey, M. T., and Grace, M. S. (2015). Predators in training: operant conditioning of novel behavior in wild Burmese pythons (Python molurus bivitattus). Animal Cognition, 18, 269278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holtzman, D., Harris, T. W., Aranguren, G., and Bostock, E. (1999). Spatial learning of an escape task by young corn snakes (Elaphe guttata guttata). Animal Behaviour, 57, 5160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kellogg, W., and Pomeroy, W. (1936). Maze learning in water snakes. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 21, 275295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Takemasa, T., and Nakamura, K. (1935). A study of learning using snakes. Kyoiku Shinri Kenkyu, 10, 575581.Google Scholar
Wolfe, D., and Browne, C. (1940). A learning experiment with snakes. Copeia, 1940, 134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

References

Burghardt, G. M. (1977). Learning processes in reptiles. In Biology of the Reptilia: ecology and behaviour (pp. 555681). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Croston, R., Kozlovsky, D. Y., Branch, C. L., Parchman, T. L., Bridge, E. S., and Pravosudov, V. V. (2016). Individual variation in spatial memory performance in wild mountain chickadees from different elevations. Animal Behavior, 111, 225234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobs, L. F., and Menzel, R. (2014). Navigation outside of the box: what the lab can learn from the field and what the field can learn from the lab. Movement Ecology, 2, 122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leal, M., and Losos, J. B. (2015). A naturalist’s insight into the evolution of signal redundancy. American Naturalist, 186, 24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leal, M, and Powell, B. J. (2012). Behavioural flexibility and problem-solving in a tropical lizard. Biology Letters, 8, 2830.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morand-Ferron, J., Hamblin, S., Cole, E. F., Aplin, L. M., and Quinn, J. L. (2015). Taking the operant paradigm into the field: associative learning in wild great tits. PLoS ONE, 10(8), 16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shettleworth, S. (1998). Cognition, evolution, and behavior. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Toxopeus, I. B., Sterck, E. H. M., Hooff, J. A. R. A. M., Spruijt, B. M., and Heeren, T. J. (2005). Effects of trait anxiety on performance of socially housed monkeys in a learning test. Behaviour, 142, 12691287.Google Scholar

References

Alkov, R. A., and Crawford, F. T. (1965). Runaway behaviour of lizards to heat and light reinforcement. Psychological Reports, 16, 423426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amiel, J. J., and Shine, R. (2012). Hotter nests produce smarter young lizards. Biology Letters, 8, 372374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amiel, J. J., Lindström, T., and Shine, R. (2014). Egg incubation effects generate positive correlations between size, speed and learning ability in young lizards. Animal Cognition, 17, 337347.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amiel, J. J., Bao, S., and Shine, R. (2017). The effects of incubation temperature on the development of the cortical forebrain in a lizard. Animal Cognition, 20, 117125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bezzina, C. N., Amiel, J. J., and Shine, R. (2014). Does invasion success reflect superior cognitive ability? A case study of two congeneric lizard species (Lampropholis, Scincidae). PLoS ONE, 9, e86271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Birrell, J. M., and Brown, V. J. (2000). Medial frontal cortex mediates perceptual attentional set shifting in the rat. The Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 43204324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carazo, P., Font, E., and Desfilis, E. (2008). Beyond ‘nasty neighbours’ and ‘dear enemies’? Individual recognition by scent marks in a lizard (Podarcis hispanica). Animal Behaviour, 76, 19531963.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carazo, P., Noble, D. W. A., Chandrasoma, D., and Whiting, M. J. (2014). Sex and boldness explain individual differences in spatial learning in a lizard. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 281, 20133275.Google Scholar
Clark, B. F., Amiel, J. J., Noble, D. W. A., and Whiting, M. J. (2014). Colour discrimination and associative learning in hatchling lizards incubated at ‘hot’ and ‘cold’ temperatures. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 68, 239247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colacicco, G., Welzl, H., Lipp, H.-P., and Wuerbel, H. (2002). Attentional set-shifting in mice: modification of a rat paradigm, and evidence for strain-dependent variation. Behavioural Brain Research, 132, 95102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooper, W. E. (1994a). Chemical discrimination by tongue-flicking in lizards – a review with hypotheses on its origin and its ecological and phylogenetic relationships. Journal of Chemical Ecology, 20, 439487.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooper, W. E. J. (1994b). Prey chemical discrimination, foraging mode, and phylogeny. In Lizard ecology: historical and experimental perspectives (pp. 95116). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Croston, R., Branch, C. L., Kozlovsky, D. Y., Dukas, R., and Pravosudov, V. V. (2015). Heritability and the evolution of cognitive traits. Behavioral Ecology, 26, 14471459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, R. E., and Richardson, A. M. (1970). Classical conditioning of skeletal and autonomic responses in the lizard (Crotaphytus collaris). Physiology and Behavior, 5, 589594.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Day, L. B., Crews, D., and Wilczynski, W. (1999b). Relative medial and dorsal cortex volume in relation to foraging ecology in congeneric lizards. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 54, 314322.Google Scholar
Day, L. B., Crews, D., and Wilczynski, W. (1999a). Spatial and reversal learning in congeneric lizards with different foraging strategies. Animal Behaviour, 57, 393407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Day, L. B., Crews, D., and Wilczynski, W. (2001). Effects of medial and dorsal cortex lesions on spatial memory in lizards. Behavioural Brain Research, 118, 2742.Google ScholarPubMed
Day, L. B., Ismail, N., and Wilczynski, W. (2003). Use of position and feature cues in discrimination learning by the whiptail lizard (Cnemidophorus inornatus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 117, 440448.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dayananda, B., and Webb, J. K. (2017). Incubation under climate warming affects learning ability and survival in hatchling lizards. Biology Letters, 13(3), 20170002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunham, A. E., Miles, D. B., and Reznick, D. N. (1988). Life history patterns in squamate reptiles. In Biology of the Reptilia. Ecology B: defense and life history (pp. 441522). New York, NY: Alan R. Liss.Google Scholar
Fleishman, L. J., Loew, E. R., and Leal, M. (1993). Ultraviolet vision in lizards. Nature, 365, 397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fleishman, L. J., Loew, E. R., and Whiting, M. J. (2011). High sensitivity to short wavelengths in a lizard and implications for understanding the evolution of visual systems in lizards. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 278, 28912899.Google Scholar
Gaalema, D. E. (2011). Visual discrimination and reversal learning in rough-necked monitor lizards (Varanus rudicollis). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 125, 246249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallistel, C. R., Fairhurst, S., and Balsam, P. (2004). The learning curve: implications of a quantitative analysis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 101, 1312413131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garzanit, F. S., and Richardson, A. M. (1974). Black–white discrimination and orienting behavior in the desert iguana (Dipsosaurus dorsalis). Animal Learning and Behavior, 2, 126128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halpern, M. (1992). Nasal chemical senses in reptiles: structure and function. In Biology of the Reptilia: brain, hormones, and behavior (pp. 423522). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Herculano-Houzel, S., Kaas, J. H., Miller, D., and Von Bartheld, C. S. (2015). How to count cells: the advantages and disadvantages of the isotropic fractionator compared with stereology. Cell and Tissue Research, 360, 2942.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoops, D. (2015). A perfusion protocol for lizards, including a method for brain removal. MethodsX, 2, 165173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoops, D., Pullman, J. F. P., Janke, A. L., Vidal-Garcia, M., et al. (2017). Sexual selection predicts brain structure in dragon lizards. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 30, 244256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huang, W. S. (2006). Parental care in the long-tailed skink, Mabuya longicaudata, on a tropical Asian island. Animal Behaviour, 72, 791795.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Husak, J. F., Irschick, D. J., Henningsen, J. P., Kirkbride, K. S., Lailvaux, S. P., and Moore, I. T. (2009). Hormonal response of male green anole lizards (Anolis carolinensis) to GnRH challenge. Journal of Experimental Zoology A, 311, 105114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, S. M. (2011). Hormonal regulation of ovarian function in reptiles. In Hormones and reproduction of vertebrates (pp. 89115). London: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Kar, F., Whiting, M. J., and Noble, D. W. A. (2017). Dominance and social information use in a lizard. Animal Cognition, 20, 805812.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karsten, K. B., Andriamandimbiarisoa, L. N., Fox, S. F., and Raxworthy, C. J. (2008). A unique life history among tetrapods: an annual chameleon living mostly as an egg. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 89808984.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kemp, D. J., Herberstein, M. E., Fleishman, L. J., et al. (2015). An integrative framework for the appraisal of colouration in nature. The American Naturalist, 185, 705724.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirkish, P. M., Fobes, J. L., and Richardson, A. M. (1979). Spatial reversal learning in the lizard Coleonyx variegatus. Bulletin of Psychonomic Science, 13, 265267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kis, A., Huber, L., and Wilkinson, A. (2015). Social learning by imitation in a reptile (Pogona vitticeps). Animal Cognition, 18, 325331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krekorian, C. O., Vance, V. J., and Richardson, A. M. (1968). Temperature-dependent maze learning in the desert iguana Dipsosaurus dorsalis. Animal Behaviour, 16, 429436.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LaDage, L. D., Riggs, B. J., Sinervo, B., and Pravosudov, V. V. (2009). Dorsal cortex volume in male side-blotched lizards, Uta stansburiana, is associated with different space use strategies. Animal Behaviour, 78, 9196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LaDage, L. D., Roth, T. C., Cerjanic, A. M., Sinervo, B., and Pravosudov, V. V. (2012). Spatial memory: are lizards really deficient? Biology Letters, 8, 939941.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LaDage, L. D., Roth, T. C. II, Sinervo, B., and Pravosudov, V. V. (2013). Interaction between territoriality, spatial environment, and hippocampal neurogenesis in male side-blotched lizards. Behavioral Neuroscience, 127, 555565.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LaDage, L. D., Roth, T. C. II, Sinervo, B., and Pravosudov, V. V. (2016). Environmental experiences influence cortical volume in territorial and nonterritorial side-blotched lizards, Uta stansburiana. Animal Behaviour, 115, 1118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LaDage, L. D., Cobb Irvin, T. E., and Gould, V. A. (2017). Assessing spatial learning and memory in small squamate reptiles. Journal of Visual Experiments, 119, e55103.Google Scholar
Leal, M., and Powell, B. J. (2012). Behavioural flexibility and problem-solving in a tropical lizard. Biology Letters, 8, 2830.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loop, M. S. (1976). Autoshaping a simple technique for teaching a lizard to perform a visual discrimination task. Copeia, 3, 574576.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manrod, J. D., Hartdegen, R., and Burghardt, G. M. (2008). Rapid solving of a problem apparatus by juvenile black-throated monitor lizards (Varanus albigularis albigularis). Animal Cognition, 11, 267273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marcellini, D. (1977). Acoustic and visual display behavior of gekkonid lizards. Integrative and Comparative Biology, 17, 251260.Google Scholar
Moore, M. C. (1988). Testosterone control of territorial behavior: tonic-release implants fully restore seasonal and short-term aggressive responses in free-living castrated lizards. General and Comparative Endocrinology, 70, 450459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mueller-Paul, J., Wilkinson, A., Hall, G., and Huber, L. (2012). Response-stereotypy in the jewelled lizard (Timon lepidus) in a radial-arm maze. Herpetology Notes, 5, 243246.Google Scholar
Nakagawa, S., and Foster, T. M. (2004). The case against retrospective power analyses with an introduction to power analysis. Acta Ethologica, 7, 103108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakagawa, S., and Hauber, M. E. (2011). Great challenges with few subjects: statistical strategies for neuroscientists. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 462473.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noble, D. W. A., Carazo, P., and Whiting, M. J. (2012). Learning outdoors: male lizards show flexible spatial learning under semi-natural conditions. Biology Letters, 8, 946948.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noble, D. W. A., Wechmann, K., Keogh, J. S., and Whiting, M. J. (2013). Behavioral and morphological traits interact to promote the evolution of alternative reproductive tactics in a lizard. American Naturalist, 182, 726742.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noble, D. W. A., Byrne, R. W., and Whiting, M. J. (2014). Age-dependent social learning in a lizard. Biology Letters, 10, 20140430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noble, D. W. A., Stenhouse, V., and Schwanz, L. E. (2018). Developmental temperatures and phenotypic plasticty in reptiles: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Biological Reviews, 93, 7297.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paulissen, M. A. (2008). Spatial learning in the little brown skink, Scincella lateralis: the importance of experience. Animal Behaviour, 76, 135141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paulisson, M. A. (2014). The role of visual cues in learning escape behaviour in the little brown skink (Scincella lateralis). Behaviour, 151, 20152028.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pérez-Cembranos, A., and Pérez-Mellado, V. (2015). Local enhancement and social foraging in a non-social insular lizard. Animal Cognition, 18, 629637.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pianka, E. R., and Vitt, L. J. (2003). Lizards: windows to the evolution of diversity. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Powell, R. W., and Peck, K. (1970). Instrumental aversive conditioning in the skink Eumeces inexpectatus studied with two test chambers. Psychonomic Science, 18, 263264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reilly, S. M., McBrayer, L. B., and Miles, D. B. (2007). Lizard ecology: the evolutionary consequences of foraging mode. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riley, J. L., Noble, D. W. A., Bryne, R. W., and Whiting, M. J. (2017). Does social environment influence learning ability in a family-living lizard? Animal Cognition, 20, 449458.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, A. C., Robbins, T. W., and Everitt, B. J. (1988). The effects of intradimensional and extradimensional shifts on visual discrimination learning in humans and non-human primates. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section B, 40, 321341.Google ScholarPubMed
Shine, R. (2014). Evolution of an evolutionary hypothesis: a history of changing ideas about the adaptive significance of viviparity in reptiles. Journal of Herpetology, 48, 147161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sinn, D. L., While, G. M., and Wapstra, E. (2008). Maternal care in a social lizard: links between female aggression and offspring fitness. Animal Behaviour, 76, 12491257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stamps, J. A. (1983). Sexual selection, sexual dimorphism and territoriality. In Lizard ecology: studies of a model organism (pp. 169204). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Striedter, G. F. (2015). Evolution of the hippocampus in reptiles and birds. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 524, 496517.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team. (2017). R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Perfusion protocol for lizards, including a method for brain removal. Vienna: Foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
Titulaer, M., van Oers, K., and Naguib, M. (2012). Personality affects learning performance in difficult tasks in a sex-dependent way. Animal Behaviour, 83, 723730.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wack, C. L., Fox, S. F., Hellgren, E. C., and Lovern, M. B. (2008). Effects of sex, age, and season on plasma steroids in free-ranging Texas horned lizards (Phrynosoma cornutum). General and Comparative Endocrinology, 155, 589596.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weed, M. R., Bryant, R., and Perry, S. (2008). Cognitive development in macaques: attentional set-shifting in juvenile and adult rhesus monkeys. Neuroscience, 157, 2228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
While, G. M., Halliwell, B., and Uller, T. (2014). The evolutionary ecology of parental care in lizards. In Reproductive biology and phylogeny of lizards and tuatara (pp. 590619). Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.Google Scholar
Whiting, M. J., and Greeff, J. M. (1997). Facultative frugivory in the cape flat lizard, Platysaurus capensis (Sauria: Cordylidae). Copeia, 1997, 811818.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiting, M. J., and While, G. M. (2017). Sociality in lizards. In Comparative social evolution (pp. 390426). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiting, M. J., Xu, F., Kar, F., Riley, J. L., Byrne, R. W., and Noble, D. W. A. (in review). Social learning in a family-living lizard.Google Scholar
Wilkinson, A., and Huber, L. (2012). Cold-blooded cognition: reptilian cognitive abilities. In The Oxford handbook of comparative evolutionary psychology (pp. 129143). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×