Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-fv566 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-21T11:20:44.844Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Epilogue: the past and future of scientific psychology

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 September 2015

John D. Greenwood
Affiliation:
City University of New York
Get access

Summary

The last chapter demonstrates an important feature of the historical development of clinical psychology and other subdisciplines such as social and developmental psychology in relation to the general development of psychology in America in the twentieth century. The development of many of these subdisciplines did not simply parallel the general development of psychology in terms of the historical progression from structural to functional psychology, and from behaviorism to cognitive psychology. While theories and therapies based upon behaviorist learning theory and cognitive processing were developed in clinical psychology, they were developed together in the postwar period, which marked the end of the behaviorist hegemony in general psychology. While some early social psychologists called themselves behaviorists, they remained committed to the study of social attitudes and public opinion from the beginning to the end of the twentieth century. Moreover, developmental psychologists never embraced the general commitment by psychologists (including social psychologists) to experimentation as the essence of scientific psychology.

This illustrates the contingency of the development of American psychology. Although one may trace the conceptual continuities and discontinuities between the development of structural and functional psychology, and behaviorism and cognitive psychology, it is well to remember that the development of American psychology depended upon the particularities and peculiarities of the social, cultural, political, and institutional context in which it developed, and the vagaries of the careers of individual psychologists.

This contingency becomes especially apparent when one compares the twentieth-century development of American psychology with the development of psychology in other countries (Baker, 2012; Sexton and Hogan, 1992; Sexton and Misiak, 1976). To take a few illustrative examples, Italian psychology embraced cognitive psychology in the early 1920s and never looked back (Mecacci, 1992). When George Miller critiqued the behaviorist position at a talk he gave in London in the 5411960s, his host pointed out that there were only two behaviorists in Britain, and apologized for the fact that neither was in attendance (Miller, 1989). Henri Pieron (1881–1964) articulated the behaviorist position in France in 1908 (Pieron, 1908), but considered cognitive psychology to be an essential component of experimental psychology (Pieron, 1929).

Type
Chapter
Information
A Conceptual History of Psychology
Exploring the Tangled Web
, pp. 540 - 544
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2015

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baker, D. B. (Ed.) (2012). The Oxford Handbook of the History of Psychology: Global Perspectives. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cole, M. (1996). Cultural Psychology: The Once and Future Discipline. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Danziger, K. (2006). Universalism and indigenization in the history of modern psychology. In Brock, A., Louw, J., and Hoorn, W. van (Eds.), Rediscovering the History of Psychology: Essays Inspired by the Work of Kurt Danziger. New York, NY: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Greenwood, J. D. (2004). The Disappearance of the Social in American Social Psychology. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Harré, R. and Moghaddam, F. M. (2012). Psychology for the Third Millennium: Integrating Cultural and Neuroscience Perspectives. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Markus, H. R. and Hamedani, M. G. (2007). Sociocultural psychology: the dynamic interdependence among self systems and social systems. In Kitayama, S. and Cohen, D. (Eds.), Handbook of Cultural Psychology. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Mecacci, L. (1992). La psicologia italiana. Fonti e document. (2 vols.) Bologna: Pitagora.Google Scholar
Miller, G. A. (1989). George A. Miller. In Lindzey, G. (Ed.), A History of Psychology in Autobiography. (Vol. 8.) Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Pickrin, W. E. and Rutherford, A. (2010). A History of Modern Psychology in Context. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Pieron, H. (1908). L'evolution du psychisme. Revue Du Mois.
Pieron, H. (1929). Principles of Experimental Psychology. (Miner, J. B., Trans.) London: Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner and Co., Ltd.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sexton, V. S. and Misiak, H. (Eds.) (1976). Psychology Around the World. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Sexton, V. S. and Hogan, J. D. (Eds.) (1992). International Psychology: Views from Around the World. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Sinha, D. (1997). Indigenizing psychology. In Berry, J. W., Poortinga, Y. H., and Pandey, J. (Eds.), Handbook of Cross-Cultural Psychology: Vol. 1. Theory and Method. (edn.) Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.Google Scholar
Staeuble, I. (2004). De-centering Western perspectives: psychology and the disciplinary order in the First and Third World. In Brock, A., Louw, J., and Hoorn, W. van (Eds.), Rediscovering the History of Psychology: Essays Inspired by the Work of Kurt Danziger. New York, NY: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Stubbe, H. and Ramon, L. (1997). Psychology in Latin America. In Bringmann, W. G., Lück, H. E., Miller, R., and Early, C. E. (Eds.), A Pictorial History of Psychology. Chicago, IL: Quintessence.Google Scholar
Zinchenko, V. P. (1997). Russian psychology. In Bringmann, W. G., Lück, H. E., Miller, R., and Early, C. E. (Eds.), A Pictorial History of Psychology. Chicago, IL: Quintessence.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×