Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-p2v8j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-12T15:05:40.924Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

4 - Stress and Working Memory: Between-Person and Within-Person Relationships

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 May 2010

Martin Sliwinski
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Huntington Hall 430, Syracuse University, Syracuse, NY 13244
Joshua Smyth
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Huntington Hall 430, Syracuse University, Syracuse, NY 13244
Robert S. Stawski
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Huntington Hall 430, Syracuse University, Syracuse, NY 13244
Christina Wasylyshyn
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Huntington Hall 430, Syracuse University, Syracuse, NY 13244
Randall W. Engle
Affiliation:
Georgia Institute of Technology
Grzegorz Sedek
Affiliation:
Warsaw School of Social Psychology and Polish Academy of Sciences
Ulrich von Hecker
Affiliation:
Cardiff University
Daniel N. McIntosh
Affiliation:
University of Denver
Get access

Summary

The individual differences construct of working memory capacity (WMC) is a topic of increasing interest in cognitive psychology. The primary reason for this interest stems from research that demonstrates consistently strong relationships between measures of WMC and higher order cognitive function, such as fluid intelligence (Engle, Tuholski, Laughlin, & Conway, 1999). In addition to being an important predictor of psychometric measures of intellectual ability, WMC predicts performance on standardized achievement tests and grade point average (Cantor, Engle, & Hamilton, 1991; Engle et al., 1999). According to Engle et al. (1999) and Engle (2002), WMC reflects the ability to maintain relevant information in the focus of attention in the face of irrelevant and distracting stimuli. Engle and colleagues (Engle et al., 1999; Unsworth, Heitz, & Engle, this volume) argued that WMC is a central component of fluid intelligence and, hence, is a cognitive primitive.

Although WMC is often viewed as a stable trait, some theorists have argued that changes in arousal, motivation, and the experience of stress can produce fluctuations in attentional reserve (Eysenck & Calvo, 1992; Kahneman, 1973; Hasher & Zacks, 1988). Consistent with this view, recent research suggests that the experience of even mild life-event stress might impair working memory performance (Klein & Boals, 2001a) and that interventions that ameliorate the effects of stress can improve working memory (Klein & Boals, 2001b). One theoretical account of this relationship proposes that the experience of stress leads to the subsequent experience of intrusive or worrisome thoughts.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2005

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allport, G. W. (1940). The psychologist's frame of reference. Psychological Bulletin, 37, 1–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Almeida, D., Wethington, E., & Kessler, R. (2002). The Daily Inventory of Stressful Experiences (DISE): An interview-based approach for measuring daily stressors. Assessment, 9, 41–55.Google Scholar
Ashcraft, M. H., & Kirk, E. P. (2001). The relationships among working memory, math anxiety, and performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 224–237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Awh, E., Jonides, J., Smith, E., Schumacher, E., Koeppe, R., & Katz, S., (1996). Dissociation of storage and rehearsal in verbal working memory: Evidence from PET, Psychological Science, 7, 25–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borsboom, D., Mellenbergh, G., & Heerden, J. (2003). The theoretical status of latent variables?Psychological Review, 130, 208–218.Google Scholar
Bremner, J. D., Southwick, S. M., & Charney, D. S. (1999). The neurobiology of posttraumatic stress disorder: An integration of animal and human research. In Saigh, P. A. & Bremner, J. D. (Eds.), Posttraumatic stress disorder: A comprehensive text (pp. 103–143).Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Cantor, J., Engle, R. W., & Hamilton, G. (1991). Short-term memory, working memory, and verbal abilities: How do they relate?Intelligence, 15, 229–246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J. D., Perlstein, W. M., Braver, T. S., Nystrom, L. E., Noll, D. C., Jonides, J., & Smith, E. E. (1997). Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a working memory task. Nature, 386, 604–608.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24, 385–396.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Devrets, W., Price, J., Simpson, T., Todd, R., Reich, T., Vannier, M., & Raichle, F. (1997). Subgenual prefrontal cortext abnormalities in mood disorders. Nature, 386, 824–827.Google Scholar
Dimsdale, J. E. (1984). Generalizing from laboratory studies to field studies of human stress physiology. Psychosomatic Medicine, 46, 463–469.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, J. L., & Wollenberg, A. L. (1993). Local homogeneity in latent trait models: A characterization of the homogeneous monotone IRT model. Psychometrika, 58, 417–429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engle, R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 19–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E., & Conway, A. R. A. (1999). Working memory, short-term memory and general fluid intelligence: A latent variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 309–331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eysenck, M. W. (1989). Anxiety and cognition: Theory and research. In Archer, T. & Nillson, L-G. (Eds.), Aversion, avoidance, and anxiety: Perspectives on aversively motivated behavior (pp. 323–334). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Eysenck, M. W., & Calvo, M. G. (1992). Anxiety and performance: The processing efficiency theory. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 409–434.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garavan, H. (1998). Serial attention within working memory. Memory & Cognition, 26, 263–276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. (1988). Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and a new view. In Bower, G. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 193–225). San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Horowitz, M. J., Wilner, N., & Alvarez, W. (1979). Impact of Event Scale: A measure of subjective stress. Psychosomatic Medicine, 41(3), 209–218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hultsch, D. F., MacDonald, S. W. S., & Dixon, R. A. (2002). Variability of reaction time performance of younger and older adults. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 57B, P101–P115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D. (1973) Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2002). The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: An individual differences perspective. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 637–671.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kemeny, M. E. (2003). The psychobiology of stress. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 124–129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirschbaum, C., Wolf, O., May, M., Wippich, W., & Hellhammer, D. (1996). Stress- and treatment-induced elevations of cortisol levels associated with impaired declarative memory in healthy adults. Life Sciences, 58, 1475–1483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, K., & Boals, A. (2001a). Expressive writing can increase working memory capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 520–533.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klein, K., & Boals, A. (2001b). The relationship of life stress and working memory. Journal of Applied Cognitive Psychology, 15, 565–579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lamiell, J. T. (1981). Toward an idiothetic psychology of personality. American Psychologist, 36, 276–289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lupien, S., Gillin, C., & Hauger, R. (1999). Working memory is more sensitive than declarative memory to acute effects of corticosteroids: A dose-response study in humans. Behavioral Neuroscience 113, 420–430.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lupien, S. J., & Lapage, M. (2001). Stress, memory, and the hippocampus: Can't live with it, can't live without it. Behavioural Brain Research, 127, 137–158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lupien, S., & McEwen, B. (1997). The acute effects of corticosteroids on cognition: Integration of animal and human model studies. Brain Research Reviews, 24, 1–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McEwen, B. (2000). Allostasis and allostatic load: Implications for neuropsychopharmacology. Neuropsychopharmacology, 22, 108–124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Rettinger, D. A., Shah, P., & Hegarty, M. (2001). How are visuospatial working memory, executive functioning, and spatial abilities related? A latent variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 621–640.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Molenaar, P. (1999). Longitudinal analysis. In Ader, H. J. & Mellenbergh, G. J. (Eds.), Research methodology in the life, behavioral and social sciences. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nesselroade, J. R. (1991). The warp and woof of the developmental fabric. In Downs, R., Liben, L., & Palermo, D. S. (Eds.), Visions of aesthetics, the environment, and development: The legacy of Joachim F. Wohwill (pp. 213–240). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Nesselroade, J. R. & Molenaar, P. C. (1999). Pooling lagged covariance structures based on short, multivariate time-series for dynamic factor analysis. In Hoyle, R. H. (Ed.), Statistical strategies for small sample research. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Newcomer, J., Selke, G., Melson, A., Kershey, T., Craft, S., Richards, K., & Alderson, A. (1999). Decreased memory performance in healthy humans induced by stress-level cortisol treatment. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 527–533.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robinson, W. S. (1950). Ecological correlations and the behavior of individuals. American Sociological Review, 15, 569–583.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103, 403–428.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sarason, I. G., Johnson, J. H., & Siegel, J. M. (1978). Assessing the impact of life changes: Development of the life experiences survey. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46, 932–946.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seeman, T. E., McEwen, B. S., Rowe, J. W., & Singer, B. H. (2001). Allostatic load as a marker of cumulative biological risk: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Proceedings of National Academy of Sciences, 98, 4770–4775.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sliwinski, M., & Hofer, S. (1999). How strong is the evidence for mediators of age-related memory loss?Gerontology, 45, 351–354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sliwinski, M., Smyth, J., Hofer, S., & Stawski, R. S. (in review). Intraindividual coupling of daily stress and cognition.
Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. J. (1997). Working memory: A view from neuroimaging. Cognitive Psychology, 33, 5–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smyth, J., Gaudy, J., & Sliwinski, M. (in review). The relation of reported past trauma to working memory. Cognition & Emotion.Google Scholar
Snidjers, T., & Bosker, R. (1999). Multilevel Analysis: An introduction to basic and advanced multilevel modeling. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., & Lushene, R. E. (1969). The State Trait Anxiety Inventory manual.Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Stawski, R. S., Wasylyshyn, C., & Sliwinski, M. J. (2004, April). The relationship between stress and cognitive performance in older adults. Poster session presented at the biannual Cognitive Aging Conference, Atlanta, GA.Google Scholar
Stawski, R. S., Wasylyshyn, C., Sliwinski, M. J., Smyth, J. M., & Hofer, S. M. Negative psychological stress and intentional thought control: Two separate but related constructs. Manuscript under review.
Stone, A. A., & Shiffman, S. (1994). Ecological momentary assessment (EMA) in behavorial medicine. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 16, 199–202.Google Scholar
Turner, M. L., & Engle, R. W. (1989). Working memory capacity: An individual differences approach. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 127–154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eck, M. M. M., Nicolson, N. A., Berkhof, H., & Sulon, J. (1996). Individual differences in cortisol responses to a laboratory speech task and their relationship to responses to stressful daily events. Biological Psychology, 43, 69–84.Google ScholarPubMed
Vasterling, J. J., Brailey, K., Constans, J. I., & Sutker, P. B. (1998). Attention and memory dysfunction in posttraumatic stress disorder. Neuropsychology, 12, 125–133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vasterling, J. J., Duke, L. M., Brailey, K., Constans, J. I., Allain, A. N. Jr., & Sutker, P. B. (2002). Attention, learning, and memory performance and intellectual resources in Vietnam veterans: PTSD and no disorder comparisons. Neuropsychology, 16, 5–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wasylyshyn, C., Stawski, R. S., & Sliwinski, M. J. A latent variable analysis of the relationship between stress, intentional thought control and cognitive performance. Manuscript in preparation.
Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101, 34–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wegner, D. M., & Zanakos, S. (1994). Chronic thought suppression. Journal of Personality, 62, 615–640.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wells, A., & Davies, M. I. (1994). The thought control questionnaire: A measure of individual differences in the control of unwanted thoughts. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32, 871–878.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×