Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-5wvtr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-17T03:32:01.395Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

1 - Localization in the Brain and Other Illusions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 February 2010

Valerie Gray Hardcastle
Affiliation:
Professor of Philosophy, Virginia Polytechnic
C. Matthew Stewart
Affiliation:
Resident, Johns Hopkins University in Otolaryngology
Andrew Brook
Affiliation:
Carleton University, Ottawa
Kathleen Akins
Affiliation:
Simon Fraser University, British Columbia
Get access

Summary

We are all probably too aware of the latest push in neuroscience to localize brain and cognitive function. Headlines scream with claims of scientists locating the morality center of the brain or the home for violence and aggression. It seems that we have taken phrenology inside the head and are now feeling our way around brain bumps in search of the exact location of any and all thought.

We trace this trend to three independent moves in academia. First, the recent and very popular emphasis on evolutionary psychology and its concomitant emphasis on modularity fuel the need to find the modules of thought that Mother Nature supposedly crafted during the Pleistocene. Second, there has been a renewed interest in philosophy of science and elsewhere in mechanistic explanations. We are told at every turn that neuroscience explanations are explanations of mechanism. How else to isolate mechanisms except through localization studies? Third and finally, the fairly recent emergence of cognitive neuroscience and fMRI studies as the methodological exemplars for cognitive science push us to see the brain as a static machine that only instantiates what psychologists tell us it does. Further, almost no one does just straight cognitive psychology any more – you have to connect your psychological experiments to some sort of imaging study these days. This approach, with its infamous subtraction method, can only lead to increased putative localization of putative cognitive functions.

Type
Chapter
Information
Cognition and the Brain
The Philosophy and Neuroscience Movement
, pp. 27 - 39
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2005

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Berthoz, A. 1988. The role of gaze in compensation of vestibular dysfunction: The gaze substitution hypothesis. Progress in Brain Research 7: 411–420CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Churchland, P. S. 1986. Neurophilosophy. Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar
Clark, S. A., Allard, T., Jenkins, W. M., and Merzenich, M. M. 1988. Receptive fields in the body-surface map in adult cortex defined by temporally correlated inputs. Nature 332: 444–445CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Courjon, J. H., Jeannerod, M., Ossuzio, I., and Schmid, R. 1977. The role of vision in compensation of vestibulo-ocular reflex after hemilabyrinthectomy in the cat. Experimental Brain Research 8: 235–248Google Scholar
Fetter, M., Zee, D. S., and Proctor, L. R. 1988. Effect of lack of vision and of occipital lobectomy upon recovery from unilateral labyrinthectomy in rhesus monkey. Journal of Neurophysiology 59: 394–407CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flourens, P. 1824. Recherches experimentales sur les propriétés et functions du système nerveux dans les animaux vertebras. Paris: BalliereGoogle Scholar
Galiana, H. L., Flohr, H., and Melvill Jones, G. 1984. A re-evaluation of intervestibular nuclear coupling: Its role in vestibular compensation. Journal of Neurophysiology 51: 258–275CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberger, M. E. 1980. Motor recovery after lesions. Trends in Neuroscience 3: 288–291CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goode, C. T., Maney, D. L., Rubel, E. W., and Fuchs, A. F. 2001. Visual influences on the development and recovery of the vestibulo-ocular reflex in the chicken. Journal of Neurophysiology 85: 1119–1128CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hain, T. C., Fetter, M., and Zee, D. S. 1987. Head-shaking nystagmus in patients with unilateral peripheral vestibular lesions. American Journal of Otolaryngology 8: 36–47CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hardcastle, V. G., and Stewart, C. M. 2002. What do brain data really show? Philosophy of Science 69: 72–82CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hardcastle, V. G., and Stewart, C. M. 2003. The art of single cell recordings. Biology and Philosophy 18: 195–208CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, M., Guitton, D., and Berthoz, A. 1988. Changing patterns of eye-head coordination during 6 hours of optically reversed vision. Experimental Brain Research 69: 531–544Google Scholar
Kjerulf, T. D., O'Neal, J. T., Calvin, W. H., Loeser, J. D., and Westrum, L. E. 1973. Deafferentation effects in lateral cuneate nucleus of the cat: Correlation of structural changes with firing patterns changes. Experimental Neurology 39: 86–102CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lacour, M., and Xerri, C. 1980. Compensation of postural reactions to free-fall in the vestibular neurectomized monkey: Role of the visual motion cues. Experimental Brain Research 40: 103–110CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lacour, M., Vidal, P. P., and Xerri, C. 1981. Visual influences in vestibulospinal reflexes during vertical linear motion in normal and hemilabyrinthectomized monkeys. Experimental Brain Research 43: 383–394Google ScholarPubMed
Loeser, J. D., Ward, A. A. Jr., and White, L. E. Jr. 1968. Chronic deafferentation of human spinal cord neurons. Journal of Neurosurgery 29: 48–50CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merzenich, M. M., Kaas, J. H., Sur, M., Nelson, R. J., and Felleman, D. J. 1983. Progression of change following median nerve section in the cortical representation of the hand in areas 3b and 1 in adult owl and squirrel monkeys. Neuroscience 10 (3): 639–665CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miles, F. A., and Lisberger, S. G. 1981. Plasticity in the vestibulo-ocular reflex: A new hypothesis. Annual Review of Neuroscience 4: 273–299CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mustari, M. J., Fuchs, A. F., Kaneko, C. R. S., and Robinson, F. R. 1994. Anatomical connections of the primate pretectal nucleus of the optic tract. Journal of Comparative Neuroanatomy 349: 111–128CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Putkonen, P. T. S., Courjon, J. H., and Jeannerod, M. 1977. Compensation of postural effects of hemilabyrinthectomy in the cat. A sensory substitution process? Experimental Brain Research 28: 249–257Google Scholar
Robinson, D. A. 1981. The use of control systems analysis in the neurophysiology of eye movements. Annual Review of Neuroscience 4: 463–503CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schaefer, K.-P., and Meyer, D. L. 1973. Compensatory mechanisms following labyrinthine lesions in the guinea pig: A simple model of learning. In Zippel, H. P. (ed.), Memory and Transfer of Information. New York: Plenum PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, P. F., and Curthoys, I. S. 1989. Mechanisms of recovery following a unilateral labyrinthectomy: A review. Brain Research Reviews 14: 155–180CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stewart, C. M. 2002. Visual-Vestibular Interactions During Compensation. Doctoral dissertation. University of Texas Medical Branch at Galveston
Stewart, C. M., Perachio, A. A., Mustari, M. J., and Allen, T. C. 1999. Effects of the pretectal nucleus of the optic tract and hemilabyrinthectomy on vestibulo-ocular reflex compensation in rhesus monkey. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts. Maimi: Society for NeuroscienceGoogle Scholar
Takehashi, S., Fetter, M., Koenig, E., and Dichgans, J. 1990. The clinical significance of head-shaking nystagmus in the dizzy patient. Acta Otolaryngology 109: 8–14CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tusa, R. J., Mustari, M. J., Burrows, A. F., and Fuchs, A. F. 2001. Gaze-stabilizing deficits and latent nystagmus in monkeys with brief, early-onset visual deprivation: Eye movement recordings. Journal of Neurophysiology 86: 651–661CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xerri, C., and Zennou, Y. 1988. Sensory, functional, and behavioral substitution processes in vestibular compensation. In Lacour, M., Toupet, M., and Denise, P. (eds.), Vestibular Compensation, Facts, Theories, and Clinical Perspectives. Paris: ElsevierGoogle Scholar
Xerri, C., Lacour, M., and Borel, L. 1988. Multimodel sensory substitution process in vestibular compensation. In Flohr, H. (ed.), Post-lesion Neuronal Plasticity. Berlin: Springer-VerlagCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zennou-Azogui, Y., Xerri, C., and Harlay, F. 1994. Visual sensory substitution in vestibular compensation: Neuronal substrates in the alert cat. Experimental Brain Research 98: 457–473CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×