Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-45l2p Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T06:21:47.769Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

1 - The Human Psyche Lives in Semiospheres

from Part I - Theoretical and Methodological Issues

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 June 2018

Alberto Rosa
Affiliation:
Universidad Autónoma de Madrid
Jaan Valsiner
Affiliation:
Aalborg University, Denmark
Get access
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2018

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

Ash, M. G. (1995). Gestalt Psychology in German Culture, 1890–1967: Holism and the Quest for Objectivity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577660.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barsalou, L. W. (2003). Abstraction in perceptual symbol systems. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 358, 11771187. DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2003.1319.Google Scholar
Bartlett, F. C. (1925). Feeling, imaging and thinking. British Journal of Psychology, 16, 1628.Google Scholar
Bertalanffy, L. von (1968/1976). General System Theory: Foundations, Development, Applications (rev. edn). New York: George Braziller.Google Scholar
Billard, A. & Dautenhahn, K. (1998). Grounding communication in autonomous robots: An experimental study. Robotics and Autonomous Systems, 24(1–2), 7179.Google Scholar
Billard, A. & Hayes, G. (1997). Robot's first steps, robot's first words. In Sorace, P. & Heycock, S. (Eds.), Proceedings of the GALA ‘97 Conference on Language Acquisition, Edinburgh. Edinburgh: Human Communication Research Centre.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. W. & Whiten, A. (1988). Machiavellian Intelligence. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Carruthers, P. & Smith, P. K. (Eds.). (1996). Theories of Theories of Mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Damasio, A. R. (1989). Time-locked multiregional retroactivation: A systems-level proposal for the neural substrates of recall and recognition. Cognition, 33, 2562.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. R. (1872). The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals (1st edn). London: John Murray. Retrieved from http://darwin-online.org.uk/content/frameset?pageseq=1&itemID=F1142&viewtype=text.Google Scholar
Diriwächter, R. (2004). Völkerpsychologie: The synthesis that never was. Culture & Psychology, 10(1), 179203.Google Scholar
Diriwächter, R. & Valsiner, J. (Eds.). (2008). Striving for the Whole: Creating Theoretical Syntheses. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers.Google Scholar
Innis, R. (2016). Affective semiosis: Philosophical links to a cultural psychology. In Valsiner, J., Marsico, G., Chaudhary, G., Sato, T. & Dazzani, V. (Eds.), Psychology as a Science of Human Being: The Yokohama Manifesto. Cham, Switzerland: Springer.Google Scholar
Jahoda, G. (1992). Crossroads between Culture and Mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Kanizsa, G. (1979). Organization in Vision: Essays on Gestalt Psychology. New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
Kull, K. (2011). Yuri Lotman in English: Bibliography. Sign Systems Studies, 39(2/4), 343356.Google Scholar
Lotman, I. M. (1996). La semiosfera I: Semiótica de la cultura y del texto (trans. from Russian by Desiderio Navarro). Madrid: Cátedra.Google Scholar
Lotman, I. M. (1998). La semiosfera II: Semiótica de la cultura y del texto de la conducta y del espacio (trans. from Russian by Desiderio Navarro). Madrid: Cátedra.Google Scholar
Lotman, I. M. (2000). La semiosfera III. Semiótica de las artes y de la cultura (trans. from Russian by Desiderio Navarro). Madrid: Cátedra.Google Scholar
Lotman, J. (2005). On the semiosphere. Sign Systems Studies, 33(1), 206229 (trans. by Wilma Clark).Google Scholar
Mammen, J. (2016). Using a topological model in psychology: Developing sense and choice categories. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 50(2), 196233.Google Scholar
Martin, J. & Gillespie, A. (2010). A neo-Meadian approach to human agency: Relating the social and the psychological in the ontogenesis of perspective-coordinating persons. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 44, 252272. DOI 10.1007/s12124–010–9126–7.Google Scholar
Maturana, H. & Varela, F. (1992). The Tree of Knowledge: The Biological Roots of Human Understanding. Boston: Shambhala Publications.Google Scholar
Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, Self and Society. Chicago: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Metzger, W. (2006). Laws of Seeing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Metzger, W. (2008). Gesetze der Sehens. Eschborn, Germany: Dietmar Klotz.Google Scholar
Ong, W. (1982). Orality and Literacy: The Technologizing of the World. London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. & Inhelder, B. (1966). L'image mental chez l'enfant. Paris: Press Universitaires de France.Google Scholar
Premack, D. G. & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 515526.Google Scholar
Rivière, A. & Sotillo, M. (1999). Comunicazione, suspensione e semiosi umana: Le origine edella prattica e de la comprensione interpersonale. Metis, 1, 4572.Google Scholar
Rosa, A. (2007a). Acts of psyche: Actuations as synthesis of semiosis and action. In Valsiner, J. & Rosa, A. (Eds.), Cambridge Handbook of Sociocultural Psychology (pp. 205237). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rosa, A. (2007b). Dramaturgical actuations and symbolic communication: Or how beliefs make up reality. In Valsiner, J. & Rosa, A. (Eds.), Cambridge Handbook of Sociocultural Psychology (pp. 293317). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rosa, A. (2015). The reflective mind and reflexivity in psychology: Description and explanation within a psychology of experience. In Marsico, G., Ruggieri, R., & Salvatore, S. (Eds.), Reflexivity and Psychology (Yearbook of Idiographic Science, vol. 6, pp. 1744). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
Rosa, A. (2016). The self rises up from lived experiences: A micro-semiotic analysis of the unfolding of trajectories of experience when performing ethics. In Valsiner, J., Marsico, G., Chaudhary, N., Sato, T., & Dazzani, V. (Eds.), Psychology as the Science of Human Being: The Yokohama Manifesto (pp. 105127). Cham, Switzerland: Springer.Google Scholar
Rosch, E. & Mervis, C. B. (1975). Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 573605.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychological Review, 110(1), 145172. DOI: 10.1037/0033–295X.110.1.145.Google Scholar
Salvatore, S. & Valsiner, J. (2010). Between the general and the unique: Overcoming the nomothetic versus idiographic opposition. Theory & Psychology, 20(6), 817833.Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R. (2004). Feelings integrate the central representation of appraisal-driven response organization in emotion. In Manstead, A. S. R., Frijda, N. & Fischer, A. (Eds.), Feelings and Emotions (pp. 136157). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sonesson, G. (1994). The concept of text in cultural semiotics. Communication to the 3rd Congress of the Nordic Association for Semiotic Studies, Trodheim. Retrieved from www.academia.edu/5424645/GÖRAN_SONESSON_The_Concept_of_Text_in_Cultural_Semiotics_1_The_Concept_of_Text_in_Cultural_Semiotics.Google Scholar
Sonesson, G. (2010). Here comes the semiotic species: Reflections on the semiotic turn in the cognitive sciences. In Wagoneer, B. (ed.), Symbolic Transformation: The Mind in Movement through Culture and Society (pp. 3858). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Steels, L. (1996). Emergent adaptive lexicons. In Maes, P. (Ed.), From Animals To Animats: Proceedings of the Fourth International Conference on Simulating Adaptive Behavior (vol. 4). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Steels, L. & McIntyre, A. (1999). Spatially distributed naming games. Advances in Complex Systems, 1(4), 301323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stjernfeldt, F. (2014). Natural Propositions: The Actuality of Peirce's Doctrine of Dicisigns. Boston: Docent Press.Google Scholar
Toomela, A. (2015). Vygotsky's theory on the Procrustes’ bed of linear thinking: Looking for structural–systemic Theseus to save the idea of “social formation of mind.” Culture & Psychology, 21: 318339. DOI: 10.1177/1354067X15570490.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toulmin, S. & Goodfield, J. (1965). The Discovery of Time. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Valsiner, J. (2002). Irreversibility of time and ontopotentiality of signs. Estudios de Psicologia, 23(1), 4959.Google Scholar
Valsiner, J. (2009). Cultural psychology today: Innovations and oversights. Culture & Psychology, 15(1), 539.Google Scholar
Valsiner, J. (2012). A Guided Science: History of Psychology in the Mirror of its Making. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers.Google Scholar
Valsiner, J. (2014). Invitation to Cultural Psychology. London: SAGE.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valsiner, J. (2015). The place for synthesis: Vygotsky's analysis of affective generalization. History of the Human Sciences, 28(2), 93102.Google Scholar
Valsiner, J. (2016). The nomothetic function of the idiographic approach: Looking from inside out. Journal of Person-Oriented Research, 2(1–2), 515. DOI: 10.17505/jpor:2016.02.Google Scholar
Valsiner, J. & Rosa, A. (2007). The myth, and beyond: Ontology of psyche and epistemology of psychology. In Valsiner, J. & Rosa, A. (Eds.), Cambridge Handbook of Sociocultural Psychology (pp. 2339). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Varela, F. J. & Shear, J. (1999). First-person methodologies: What, why, how? Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6(12), 114.Google Scholar
Vogt, P. (2002). The physical symbol grounding problem. Cognitive Systems Research, 3, 429457.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and Language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, (1925/1971). Psychology of Art. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wagoner, B. (2017). Frederic Bartlett – A Life. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. (1958). Philosophical Investigations. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Wundt, W. M. (1874). Grundzüge der physiologischen Psychologie. Leipzig, Germany: W. Engelman.Google Scholar
Ziemke, T. & Sharkey, N. E. (2001). A stroll through the worlds of robots and animals: Applying Jakob von Uexküll's theory of meaning to adaptive robots and artificial life. Semiotica, 134(1–4), 701746.Google Scholar

Further Reading

Braudel, F. (1969). Écrits sur l'histoire. Paris: Flammarion.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. (2004). Emotions and action. In Manstead, A. S. R., Frijda, N., & Fischer, A. (Eds.), Feelings and Emotions (pp. 158173). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Harnad, S. (1990). The symbol grounding problem. Physica D, 42, 335346.Google Scholar
Harris, M. (1979). Cultural Materialism: The Struggle for a Science of Culture. Walnut Creek, CA: Altamira Press.Google Scholar
Lotman, I. M. (1990). Cultura y explosión: lo previsible y lo imprevisible en los procesos de cambio social. Barcelona: Gedisa.Google Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1980). Minds, brains, and programs. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3:417424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sun, R. (2000). Symbol grounding: A new look at an old idea. Philosophical Psychology, 13(2), 149172.Google Scholar
Valsiner, J. (1998). The Guided Mind: A Sociogenetic Approach to Personality. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×