Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-8bljj Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-06T11:04:18.036Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

10 - The That-Trace Effect

from Part II - Experimental Studies of Specific Phenomena

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 December 2021

Grant Goodall
Affiliation:
University of California, San Diego
Get access

Summary

The that-trace effect is one of several long-distance extraction phenomena that have been the focus of much work in generative syntax. The core pattern, though easily stated, remains a conundrum. It is evident in numbers of unrelated languages, but also appears to vary even in closely related languages. Experimental work that has tested the core contrasts both corroborates and refines them across languages and dialects. We find consistent evidence for the core pattern, as well as a reduction in the acceptability of object extraction over that. Experiments on German have uncovered previously unrecognized subject-object asymmetries, though the relevance to that-trace remains unclear. We also discuss the use of experimental approaches to assess theoretical accounts of the that-trace effect. We conclude with a summary of experimental findings bearing on that-trace and their implications for the general role of judgments in generative theory.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2021

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bayer, J. (2005). Was beschränkt die Extraktion? Subjekt–Objekt vs. Topik–Fokus. In D’Avis, F.-J., ed., Deutsche Syntax: Empirie und Theorie. Gothenburg: Acta Universitatis Gothoburgensis, pp. 233257.Google Scholar
Bennis, H. (1986). Gaps and Dummies. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Bentzen, K. (2014). Subject and object extraction from embedded clauses. Nordic Atlas of Language Structures, 1, 435446.Google Scholar
Bresnan, J. (1977). Variables in the theory of transformations. In Culicover, P. W., Wasow, T., & Akmajian, A., eds., Formal Syntax. New York: Academic Press, pp. 157196.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1995). The Minimalist Program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cowart, W. (1997). Experimental Syntax: Applying Objective Methods to Sentence Judgments. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Cowart, W. (2003). Detecting syntactic dialects: the that-trace phenomenon. Presentation at the 39th Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society, April, University of Chicago.Google Scholar
Featherston, S. (2004). Bridge verbs and V2 verbs – the same thing in spades? Zeitschrift für Sprachwissenschaft, 23(2), 181209.Google Scholar
Featherston, S. (2005). That-trace in German. Lingua, 115(9), 12771302.Google Scholar
Featherston, S. (2008). Thermometer judgements as linguistic evidence. In Riehl, C. M. & Rothe, A., eds., Was ist linguistische Evidenz? Aachen: Shaker Verlag, pp. 6990.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1974). Special sciences (or: The disunity of science as a working hypothesis). Synthese, 28(2), 97115.Google Scholar
Kandybowicz, J. (2006). Comp-trace effects explained away. In Baumer, D., Montero, D., & Scanlon, M., eds., Proceedings of the 25th West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project, pp. 220228.Google Scholar
Kiziak, T. (2010). Extraction Asymmetries: Experimental Evidence from German. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Lindstad, A., Nøklestad, M., Johannessen, A., J. B., & Vangsnes, Ø. A. (2009). The Nordic Dialect Database: Mapping microsyntactic variation in the Scandinavian languages. In Jokinen, K. & Bick, E., eds., Proceedings of the 17th Nordic Conference of Computational Linguistics (NoDaLiDa) (NEALT Proceedings Series, 4). Helsinki: Northern European Association for Language Technology, pp. 283286.Google Scholar
McDaniel, D. (2018). Long-distance extraction attraction: A production-based account of an unexpected cross-linguistic structure. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics, 3(1), 95.Google Scholar
McDaniel, D., McKee, C., Cowart, W., & Garrett, M. F. (2015). The role of the language production system in shaping grammars. Language, 91(2), 415–41.Google Scholar
Perlmutter, D. M. (1968). Deep and surface structure constraints in syntax. Doctoral dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
Perlmutter, D. M. (1971) Deep and Surface Structure Constraints in Generative Grammar. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.Google Scholar
Pesetsky, D. (2017) Complementizer-trace effects. In Everaert, M. & van Riemsdijk, H. C., eds., The Wiley Blackwell Companion to Syntax, 2nd ed. New York: John Wiley & Sons, pp. 134.Google Scholar
Phillips, C. (2013). On the nature of island constraints. II: Language learning and innateness. Sprouse, In J. & Hornstein, N., eds., Experimental Syntax and Island Effects. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 132157.Google Scholar
Ritchart, A., Goodall, G., & Garellek, M. (2016). Prosody and the that-trace effect: An experimental study. In Kim, K.-m., Umbal, P., Block, T., Chan, Q., Cheng, T., Finney, K., Katz, M., Nickel-Thompson, S., & Shorten, L., eds., Proceedings of the 33rd West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project, pp. 320328.Google Scholar
Salzmann, M., Häussler, J., Bader, M., & Bayer, J. (2013). That-trace effects without traces: An experimental investigation. Presented at the NELS 42.Google Scholar
Schippers, A. (2019). COMP-trace revisited: An indirect dependency analysis. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Sobin, N. (1987). The variable status of comp-trace phenomena. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 5(1), 3360.Google Scholar
Stevens, S. S. (1975). Psychophysics: Introduction to its Perceptual, Neural, and Social Prospects. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Strik, N. (2008). Syntaxe et acquisition des phrases interrogatives en français et en néerlandais: une étude contrastive. Doctoral dissertation, University of Paris VIII.Google Scholar
Westergaard, M., Vangsnes, Ø. A., & Lohndal, T. (2017). Variation and change in Norwegian wh-questions: The role of the complementizer som. Linguistic Variation, 17(1), 843.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, L. (1986). Island effects in second language acquisition. Montreal Working Papers in Linguistics, 3, 127.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×