Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-25wd4 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T05:54:01.665Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

11 - How Sophisticated Is Infants’ Theory of Mind?

from Subpart II.1 - Infancy: The Roots of Human Thinking

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 February 2022

Olivier Houdé
Affiliation:
Université de Paris V
Grégoire Borst
Affiliation:
Université de Paris V
Get access

Summary

Imagine the following scenario (inspired by true events): We are grocery shopping with our friend Anita, who notices a woman down the aisle and waves and smiles at her. The woman looks confused and walks away. Perplexed, Anita tells us that she spent the previous day training the woman, a new employee at their firm, and is surprised by her behavior because she thought they had established a friendly rapport. We suggest a possible explanation: Perhaps the woman did not recognize Anita outside of work. Later at the checkout line, we notice the woman standing with someone who is obviously her identical twin. In light of this new information, we revise our initial explanation: Anita must have waved at her trainee’s twin, falsely believing her to be her trainee, and the twin responded with confusion because she had never met Anita before. Anita then joins us at the checkout, one of the twins waves happily to her, and the mystery is resolved with smiles all around.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2022

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adrián, J. E., Clemente, R. A., & Villanueva, L. (2007). Mothers’ use of cognitive state verbs in picture-book reading and the development of children’s understanding of mind: A longitudinal study. Child Development, 78, 10521067.Google Scholar
Antilici, F., & Baillargeon, R. (2020). 2.5-year-olds pass an explicit unexpected-transfer false-belief task when processing demands are reduced. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the International Congress of Infant Studies, July 2020, Glasgow, Scotland.Google Scholar
Apperly, I. A., & Butterfill, S. A. (2009). Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief-like states? Psychological Review, 116, 953970.Google Scholar
Baillargeon, R., Buttelmann, D., & Southgate, V. (2018). Invited Commentary: Interpreting failed replications of early false-belief findings: Methodological and theoretical considerations. Cognitive Development, 46, 112124.Google Scholar
Baillargeon, R., He, Z., Setoh, P., Scott, R. M., Sloane, S., & Yang, D. Y. J. (2013). False-belief understanding and why it matters. In Mahzarin, R., & Gelman, S. A. (eds.), Navigating the Social World: What Infants, Children, and Other Species Can Teach Us (pp. 8895). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baillargeon, R., Scott, R. M., & Bian, L. (2016). Psychological reasoning in infancy. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 159186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baillargeon, R., Scott, R. M., He, Z., Sloane, S., Setoh, P., Jin, K., … Bian, L. (2015). Psychological and sociomoral reasoning in infancy. In Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P. R. (eds.), Borgida, E., & Bargh, J. A. (assoc. eds.), APA Handbook of Personality and Social Psychology: Vol.1. Attitudes and Social Cognition (pp. 79150). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Bardi, L., Desmet, C., Nijhof, A., Wiersema, J. R., & Brass, M. (2017). Brain activation for spontaneous and explicit false belief tasks overlaps: New fMRI evidence on belief processing and violation of expectation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12, 391400.Google ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, 3746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett, H. C., Broesch, T., Scott, R. M., He, Z., Baillargeon, R., Wu, D., … Laurence, S. (2013). Early false-belief understanding in traditional non-Western societies. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 280, 20122654.Google Scholar
Bartsch, K. (1996). Between desires and beliefs: Young children’s action predictions. Child Development, 67, 16711685.Google Scholar
Begus, K., & Southgate, V. (2012). Infant pointing serves an interrogative function. Developmental Science, 15, 611617.Google Scholar
Bergelson, E., & Swingley, D. (2012). At 6–9 months, human infants know the meanings of many common nouns. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 109, 32533258.Google Scholar
Bian, L., He, Z., & Baillargeon, R. (2017). False-belief understanding in young infants: Evidence from anticipatory-looking and violation-of-expectation measures. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, April 2017, Austin, TX.Google Scholar
Bíro, S., Verschoor, S., & Coenen, L. (2011). Evidence for a unitary goal concept in 12-month-old infants. Developmental Science, 14, 12551260.Google Scholar
Buresh, J. S., & Woodward, A. L. (2007). Infants track action goals within and across agents. Cognition, 104, 287314.Google Scholar
Butler, A. G. (2013). Exploring the role of social reasoning and self-efficacy in the mathematics problem-solving performance of lower-and higher-income children. Journal of Educational Research and Practice, 3, 93119.Google Scholar
Buttelmann, D., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Eighteen-month-old infants show false belief understanding in an active helping paradigm. Cognition, 112, 337342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buttelmann, F., Suhrke, J., & Buttelmann, D. (2015). What you get is what you believe: Eighteen-month-olds demonstrate belief understanding in an unexpected-identity task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 131, 94103.Google Scholar
Butterfill, S., & Apperly, I. A. (2013). How to construct a minimal theory of mind. Mind and Language, 28, 606637.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butterworth, G., & Jarrett, N. (1991). What minds have in common is space: Spatial mechanisms serving joint visual attention in infancy. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 5572.Google Scholar
Carlson, S. M., & Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Development, 72, 10321053.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carruthers, P. (2016). Two systems for mindreading? Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 7, 141162.Google Scholar
Carruthers, P. (2018). Young children flexibly attribute mental states to others. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 115, 1135111353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cesana-Arlotti, N., Kovács, Á. M., & Téglás, E. (2020). Infants recruit logic to learn about the social world. Nature Communications, 11, 5999.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Choi, Y. J., & Luo, Y. (2015). 13-month-olds’ understanding of social interactions. Psychological Science, 26, 274283.Google Scholar
Choi, Y., Luo, Y., & Baillargeon, R. (in press). Can 5-month-old infants consider the perspective of a novel eyeless agent? New evidence for early mentalistic reasoning. Child Development.Google Scholar
Choi, Y. J., Mou, Y., & Luo, Y. (2018). How do 3-month-old infants attribute preferences to a human agent? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 172, 96106.Google Scholar
Cowell, J. M., Lee, K., Malcolm-Smith, S., Selcuk, B., Zhou, X., & Decety, J. (2017). The development of generosity and moral cognition across five cultures. Developmental Science, 20, e12403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crivello, C., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2018). Infants’ false belief understanding: A non-replication of the helping task. Cognitive Development, 46, 5157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Csibra, G. (2008). Goal attribution to inanimate agents by 6.5-month-old infants. Cognition, 107, 705717.Google Scholar
Csibra, G., Bíró, S., Koós, O., & Gergely, G. (2003). One-year-old infants use teleological representations of actions productively. Cognitive Science, 27, 111133.Google Scholar
Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2009). Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 148153.Google Scholar
Csibra, G., Gergely, G., Bíró, S., Koos, O., & Brockbank, M. (1999). Goal attribution without agency cues: The perception of ‘pure reason’ in infancy. Cognition, 72, 237267.Google Scholar
Cutting, A. L., & Dunn, J. (1999). Theory of mind, emotion understanding, language, and family background: Individual differences and interrelations. Child Development, 70, 853865.Google Scholar
de Villiers, J. & de Villiers, P. (2003). Language for thought: Coming to understand false beliefs. In Gentner, D., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (eds.), Language in Mind: Advances in the Study of Language and Thought (pp. 335384). Harvard, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1987). The Intentional Stance. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Devine, R. T., & Hughes, C. (2014). Relations between false belief understanding and executive function in early childhood: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 85, 17771794.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Devine, R. T., & Hughes, C. (2018). Family correlates of false belief understanding in early childhood: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 89, 971987.Google Scholar
Dörrenberg, S., Rakoczy, H., & Liszkowski, U. (2018). How (not) to measure infant Theory of Mind: Testing the replicability and validity of four non-verbal measures. Cognitive Development, 46, 1230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duh, S., Paik, J. H., Miller, P. H., Gluck, S. C., Li, H., & Himelfarb, I. (2016). Theory of mind and executive function in Chinese preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 52, 582591.Google Scholar
Dunfield, K. A., & Kuhlmeier, V. A. (2010). Intention-mediated selective helping in infancy. Psychological Science, 21, 523527.Google Scholar
Egyed, K., Király, I., & Gergely, G. (2013). Communicating shared knowledge in infancy. Psychological Science, 24, 13481353.Google Scholar
Ensor, R., & Hughes, C. (2008). Content or connectedness? Mother–child talk and early social understanding. Child Development, 79, 201216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forgács, B., Gervain, J., Parise, E., Csibra, G., Gergely, G., Baross, J., & Király, I. (2020). Electrophysiological investigation of infants’ understanding of understanding. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 43, 100783.Google Scholar
Forgács, B., Parise, E., Csibra, G., Gergely, G., Jacquey, L., & Gervain, J. (2019). Fourteen-month-old infants track the language comprehension of communicative partners. Developmental Science, 22, e12751.Google Scholar
Garnham, W. A., & Ruffman, T. (2001). Doesn’t see, doesn’t know: Is anticipatory looking really related to understanding or belief? Developmental Science, 4, 94100.Google Scholar
Gergely, G., & Csibra, G. (2003). Teleological reasoning in infancy: The naive theory of rational action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 287292.Google Scholar
Gergely, G., Nádasdy, Z., Csibra, G., & Bíró, S. (1995). Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition, 56, 165193.Google Scholar
Glenwright, M., Scott, R. M., Bilevicius, E., Pronovost, M., & Hanlon-Dearman, A. (2021). Children with autism spectrum disorder can attribute false beliefs in a spontaneous-response preferential-looking task. Frontiers in Communication, 6, 146.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A., & Astington, J. W. (1988). Children’s understanding of representational change and its relation to the understanding of false belief and the appearance-reality distinction. Child Development, 59, 2637.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A., & Wellman, H. M. (1994). The theory theory. In Hirschfeld, L. A., & Gelman, S. A. (eds.), Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity in Cognition and Culture (pp. 257293). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Grosse Wiesmann, C. G., Friederici, A. D., Disla, D., Steinbeis, N., & Singer, T. (2018). Longitudinal evidence for 4-year-olds’ but not 2-and 3-year-olds’ false belief-related action anticipation. Cognitive Development, 46, 5868.Google Scholar
Grosso, S. S., Schuwerk, T., Kaltefleiter, L. J., & Sodian, B. (2019). 33-month-old children succeed in a false-belief task with reduced processing demands: A replication of Setoh et al. (2016). Infant Behavior and Development, 54, 151155.Google Scholar
Hamlin, J. K. (2013). Failed attempts to help and harm: Intention versus outcome in preverbal infants’ social evaluations. Cognition, 18, 451474.Google Scholar
Hamlin, J. K., & Wynn, K. (2011). Young infants prefer prosocial to antisocial others. Cognitive Development, 26, 3039.Google Scholar
Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2007). Social evaluation by preverbal infants. Nature, 450, 557559.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hansen, M. B. (2010). If you know something, say something: Young children’s problem with false beliefs. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 23.Google Scholar
Hayne, H. (2004). Infant memory development: Implications for childhood amnesia. Developmental Review, 24, 3373.Google Scholar
He, Z., Bolz, M., & Baillargeon, R. (2011). False-belief understanding in 2.5-year-olds: Evidence from violation-of-expectation change-of-location, and unexpected-contents tasks. Developmental Science, 14, 292305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
He, Z., Bolz, M., & Baillargeon, R. (2012). 2.5-year-olds succeed at a verbal anticipatory-looking false-belief task. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30, 1429.Google Scholar
Helming, K. A., Strickland, B., & Jacob, P. (2016). Solving the puzzle about early belief-ascription. Mind & Language, 31, 438469.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henderson, A. M., & Woodward, A. L. (2012). Nine-month-old infants generalize object labels, but not object preferences across individuals. Developmental Science, 15, 641652.Google Scholar
Hespos, S. J., & Baillargeon, R. (2001). Infants’ knowledge about occlusion and containment events: A surprising discrepancy. Psychological Science, 12, 141147.Google Scholar
Heyes, C. (2014). False belief in infancy: A fresh look. Developmental Science, 17, 647659.Google Scholar
Hofmann, S. G., Doan, S. N., Sprungc, M., Wilson, A., Ebesutanie, C., Andrews, L. A.Harris, P. L. (2016). Training children’s theory-of-mind: A meta-analysis of controlled studies. Cognition, 150, 200212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holmes, H. A., Black, C., & Miller, S. A. (1996). A cross-task comparison of false belief understanding in a Head Start population. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63, 263285.Google Scholar
Hughes, C., Adlam, A., Happé, F., Jackson, J., Taylor, A., & Caspi, A. (2000). Good test–retest reliability for standard and advanced false-belief tasks across a wide range of abilities. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 483490.Google Scholar
Hyde, D. C., Aparicio Betancourt, M., & Simon, C. E. (2015). Human temporal‐parietal junction spontaneously tracks others’ beliefs: A functional near-infrared spectroscopy study. Human Brain Mapping, 36, 48314846.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hyde, D. C., Simon, C. E., Ting, F., & Nikolaeva, J. I. (2018). Functional organization of the temporal–parietal junction for theory of mind in preverbal infants: A near-infrared spectroscopy study. Journal of Neuroscience, 38, 42644274.Google Scholar
Imuta, K., Henry, J. D., Slaughter, V., Selcuk, B. & Ruffman, T. (2016). Theory of mind and prosocial behavior in childhood: A meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology, 52, 11921205.Google Scholar
Jin, K. S., Houston, J. L., Baillargeon, R., Groh, A. M., & Roisman, G. I. (2018). Young infants expect an unfamiliar adult to comfort a crying baby: Evidence from a standard violation-of-expectation task and a novel infant-triggered-video task. Cognitive Psychology, 102, 120.Google Scholar
Jin, K. S., Kim, Y., Song, M., Kim, Y. J., Lee, H., Lee, Y., … Song, H. J. (2019). Fourteen-to eighteen-month-old infants use explicit linguistic information to update an agent’s false belief. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2508.Google Scholar
Jin, K. S., & Song, H. J. (2017). You changed your mind! Infants interpret a change in word as signaling a change in an agent’s goals. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 162, 149162.Google Scholar
Johnson, S. C., Shimizu, Y. A., & Ok, S. J. (2007). Actors and actions: The role of agent behavior in infants’ attribution of goals. Cognitive Development, 22, 310322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kamewari, K., Kato, M., Kanda, T., Ishiguro, H., & Hiraki, K. (2005). Six-and-a-half-month-old children positively attribute goals to human action and to humanoid-robot motion. Cognitive Development, 20, 303320.Google Scholar
Kampis, D., & Hamlin, K. (2019). ManyBabies 2: Theory of mind in infancy. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, March 2019, Baltimore, MD.Google Scholar
Kampis, D., Parise, E., Csibra, G., & Kovács, Á. M. (2015). Neural signatures for sustaining object representations attributed to others in preverbal human infants. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 282, 20151683.Google Scholar
Kim, E. Y., & Song, H. J. (2015). Six-month-olds actively predict others’ goal-directed actions. Cognitive Development, 33, 113.Google Scholar
Király, I., Oláh, K., Csibra, G., & Kovács, Á. M. (2018). Retrospective attribution of false beliefs in 3-year-old children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 115, 1147711482.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kloo, D., Kristen-Antonow, S., & Sodian, B. (2020). Progressing from an implicit to an explicit false belief understanding: A matter of executive control? International Journal of Behavioral Development, 44, 107–115.Google Scholar
Knudsen, B., & Liszkowski, U. (2012). 18-month-olds predict specific action mistakes through attribution of false belief, not ignorance, and intervene accordingly. Infancy, 17, 672691.Google Scholar
Koenig, M. A., & Woodward, A. L. (2010). Sensitivity of 24-month-olds to the prior inaccuracy of the source: Possible mechanisms. Developmental Psychology, 46, 815826.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kulke, L., Reiß, M., Krist, H., & Rakoczy, H. (2018a). How robust are anticipatory looking measures of Theory of Mind? Replication attempts across the life span. Cognitive Development, 46, 97111.Google Scholar
Kulke, L., von Duhn, B., Schneider, D., & Rakoczy, H. (2018b). Is implicit theory of mind a real and robust phenomenon? Results from a systematic replication study. Psychological Science, 29, 888900.Google Scholar
Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Thirty years and counting: Finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 621647.Google Scholar
Lecce, S., & Hughes, C. (2010). The Italian job?: Comparing theory of mind performance in British and Italian children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28, 747766.Google Scholar
Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation: The origins of “theory of mind.” Psychological Review, 94, 412426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leslie, A. M. (1994). ToMM, ToBy, and agency: Core architecture and domain specificity. In Hirschfeld, L. A., & Gelman, S. A. (eds.), Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity in Cognition and Culture (pp. 119148). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Leslie, A. M., Friedman, O., & German, T. P. (2004). Core mechanisms in ‘theory of mind’. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 528533.Google Scholar
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Twelve-month-olds communicate helpfully and appropriately for knowledgeable and ignorant partners. Cognition, 108, 732739.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, D., Wellman, H. M., Tardif, T., & Sabbagh, M. A. (2008). Theory of mind development in Chinese children: A meta-analysis of false-belief understanding across cultures and languages. Developmental Psychology, 44, 523531.Google Scholar
Liu, S., & Sun, R. (2018). Do great minds prefer alike? Thirteen-month-old infants generalize personal preferences across objects of like kind but not across people. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 2636.Google Scholar
Low, J., Apperly, I. A., Butterfill, S. A., & Rakoczy, H. (2016). Cognitive architecture of belief reasoning in children and adults: A primer on the two-systems account. Child Development Perspectives, 10, 184189.Google Scholar
Low, J., Drummond, W., Walmsley, A., & Wang, B. (2014). Representing how rabbits quack and competitors act: Limits on preschoolers’ efficient ability to track perspective. Child Development, 85, 15191534.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Low, J., & Watts, J. (2013). Attributing false beliefs about object identity reveals a signature blind spot in humans’ efficient mind-reading system. Psychological Science, 24, 305311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luo, Y. (2011). Three-month-old infants attribute goals to a non-human agent. Developmental Science, 14, 453460.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luo, Y., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Can a self-propelled box have a goal? Psychological reasoning in 5-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 16, 601608.Google Scholar
Luo, Y., & Baillargeon, R. (2007). Do 12.5-month-old infants consider what objects others can see when interpreting their actions? Cognition, 105, 489512.Google Scholar
Luo, Y., & Johnson, S. C. (2009). Recognizing the role of perception in action at 6 months. Developmental Science, 12, 142149.Google Scholar
Ma, F., Xu, F., Heyman, G. D., & Lee, K. (2011). Chinese children’s evaluations of white lies: Weighing the consequences for recipients. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 308321.Google Scholar
Margoni, F., Baillargeon, R., & Surian, L. (2018). Infants distinguish between leaders and bullies. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 115, E8835E8843.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Margoni, F., & Surian, L. (2020). Conceptual continuity in the development of moral judgment. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 194, 104812.Google Scholar
Martin, A., Onishi, K. H., & Vouloumanos, A. (2012). Understanding the abstract role of speech in communication at 12 months. Cognition, 123, 5060.Google Scholar
Mayer, A., & Träuble, B. E. (2013). Synchrony in the onset of mental state understanding across cultures? A study among children in Samoa. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 37, 2128.Google Scholar
McAlister, A., & Peterson, C. C. (2006). Mental playmates: Siblings, executive functioning, and theory of mind. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24, 733751.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology, 31, 838850.Google Scholar
Meristo, M., & Surian, L. (2013). Do infants detect indirect reciprocity? Cognition, 129, 102113.Google Scholar
Milligan, K., Astington, J. W., & Dack, L. A. (2007). Language and theory of mind: Meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding. Child Development, 78, 622646.Google Scholar
Moll, H., Khalulyan, A., & Moffett, L. (2017). 2.5-year-olds express suspense when others approach reality with false expectations. Child Development, 88, 114122.Google Scholar
Naito, M., & Koyama, K. (2006). The development of false-belief understanding in Japanese children: Delay and difference? International Journal of Behavioral Development, 30, 290304.Google Scholar
Onishi, K. H., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science, 308, 255258.Google Scholar
Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the Representational Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Perner, J. (2010). Who took the cog out of cognitive science? Mentalism in an era of anti-cognitivism. In Frensch, P. A., & Schwarzer, R. (eds.), Cognition and Neuropsychology: International Perspectives on Psychological Science (Vol. 1, pp. 241261). Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Perner, J., Leekam, S. R., & Wimmer, H. (1987). Three-year-olds’ difficulty with false belief: The case for a conceptual deficit. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 125137.Google Scholar
Peterson, C. C., Wellman, H. M., & Slaughter, V. (2012). The mind behind the message: Advancing theory-of-mind scales for typically developing children, and those with deafness, autism, or Asperger syndrome. Child Development, 83, 469485.Google Scholar
Poulin-Dubois, D., Brooker, I., & Polonia, A. (2011). Infants prefer to imitate a reliable person. Infant Behavior and Development, 34, 303309.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poulin-Dubois, D., Polonia, A., & Yott, J. (2013). Is false belief skin-deep? The agent’s eye status influences infants’ reasoning in belief-inducing situations. Journal of Cognition and Development, 14, 8799.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poulin-Dubois, D., Rakoczy, H., Burnside, K., Crivello, C., Dörrenberg, S., Edwards, K., … Perner, J. (2018). Do infants understand false beliefs? We don’t know yet – A commentary on Baillargeon, Buttelmann, and Southgate’s commentary. Cognitive Development, 48, 302315.Google Scholar
Powell, L. J., Hobbs, K., Bardis, A., Carey, S., & Saxe, R. (2018). Replications of implicit theory of mind tasks with varying representational demands. Cognitive Development, 46, 4050.Google Scholar
Priewasser, B., Rafetseder, E., Gargitter, C., & Perner, J. (2018). Helping as an early indicator of a theory of mind: Mentalism or teleology? Cognitive Development, 46, 6978.Google Scholar
Rhodes, M., & Brandone, A. C. (2014). Three-year-olds’ theories of mind in actions and words. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 263.Google Scholar
Richardson, H., Lisandrelli, G., Riobueno-Naylor, A., & Saxe, R. (2018). Development of the social brain from age three to twelve years. Nature Communications, 9, 1027.Google Scholar
Roby, E., & Scott, R. M. (2016). Rethinking the relationship between social experience and false-belief understanding: A mentalistic account. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1721.Google Scholar
Roby, E., & Scott, R. M. (2018). The relationship between parental mental-state language and 2.5-year-olds’ performance on a nontraditional false-belief task. Cognition, 180, 1023.Google Scholar
Rubio-Fernández, P., & Geurts, B. (2013). How to pass the false-belief task before your fourth birthday. Psychological Science, 24, 2733.Google Scholar
Rubio-Fernández, P., & Geurts, B. (2016). Don’t mention the marble! The role of attentional processes in false-belief tasks. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 7, 835850.Google Scholar
Ruffman, T. (2014). To belief or not belief: Children’s theory of mind. Developmental Review, 34, 265293.Google Scholar
Ruffman, T., Slade, L., & Crowe, E. (2002). The relation between children’s and mothers’ mental state language and theory-of-mind understanding. Child Development, 73, 734751.Google Scholar
Schulze, C., & Buttelmann, D. (2021). Small procedural differences matter: Conceptual and direct replication attempts of the communication-intervention effect on infants’ false-belief ascriptions. Cognitive Development, 59, 101054Google Scholar
Schuwerk, T., Priewasser, B., Sodian, B., & Perner, J. (2018). The robustness and generalizability of findings on spontaneous false belief sensitivity: A replication attempt. Royal Society Open Science, 5, 172273.Google Scholar
Scott, R. M. (2017a). Surprise! 20-month-old infants understand the emotional consequences of false beliefs. Cognition, 159, 33–47.Google Scholar
Scott, R. M. (2017b). The developmental origins of false-belief understanding. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26, 6874.Google Scholar
Scott, R. M., & Baillargeon, R. (2009). Which penguin is this? Attributing false beliefs about object identity at 18 months. Child Development, 80, 11721196.Google Scholar
Scott, R. M., & Baillargeon, R. (2017). Early false-belief understanding. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21, 237249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, R. M., & Baillargeon, R. (2013). Do infants really expect others to act efficiently? A critical test of the rationality principle. Psychological Science, 24, 466474.Google Scholar
Scott, R. M., Baillargeon, R., Song, H. J., & Leslie, A. M. (2010). Attributing false beliefs about non-obvious properties at 18 months. Cognitive Psychology, 61, 366395.Google Scholar
Scott, R. M., He, Z., Baillargeon, R., & Cummins, D. (2012). False-belief understanding in 2.5-year-olds: Evidence from two novel verbal spontaneous-response tasks. Developmental Science, 15, 181193.Google Scholar
Scott, R. M., Richman, J. C., & Baillargeon, R. (2015). Infants understand deceptive intentions to implant false beliefs about identity: New evidence for early mentalistic reasoning. Cognitive Psychology, 82, 3256.Google Scholar
Scott, R. M., & Roby, E. (2015). Processing demands impact 3-year-olds’ performance in a spontaneous-response task: New evidence for the processing-load account of early false-belief understanding. PLoS ONE, 10, e0142405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scott, R. M., Roby, E., & Setoh, P. (2020). 2.5-year-olds succeed in identity and location elicited-response false-belief tasks with adequate response practice. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 198, 104890.Google Scholar
Senju, A., Southgate, V., Miura, Y., Matsui, T., Hasegawa, T., Tojo, Y., … Csibra, G. (2010). Absence of spontaneous action anticipation by false belief attribution in children with autism spectrum disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 22, 353360.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Senju, A., Southgate, V., Snape, C., Leonard, M., & Csibra, G. (2011). Do 18-month-olds really attribute mental states to others? A critical test. Psychological Science, 22, 878880.Google Scholar
Senju, A., Southgate, V., White, S., & Frith, U. (2009). Mindblind eyes: An absence of spontaneous theory of mind in Asperger syndrome. Science, 325, 883885.Google Scholar
Setoh, P., Scott, R. M., & Baillargeon, R. (2016). Two-and-a-half-year-olds succeed at a traditional false-belief task with reduced processing demands. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 113, 1336013365.Google Scholar
Slaughter, V., Peterson, C. C., & Mackintosh, E. (2007). Mind what mother says: Narrative input and theory of mind in typical children and those on the autism spectrum. Child Development, 78, 839858.Google Scholar
Sloane, S., Baillargeon, R., & Premack, D. (2012). Do infants have a sense of fairness? Psychological Science, 23, 196204.Google Scholar
Song, H. J., & Baillargeon, R. (2008). Infants’ reasoning about others’ false perceptions. Developmental Psychology, 44, 17891795.Google Scholar
Song, H. J., Baillargeon, R., & Fisher, C. (2014). The development of infants’ use of novel verbal information when reasoning about others’ actions. PLoS ONE, 9, e92387.Google Scholar
Song, H. J., Onishi, K. H., Baillargeon, R., & Fisher, C. (2008). Can an agent’s false belief be corrected by an appropriate communication? Psychological reasoning in 18-month-old infants. Cognition, 109, 295315.Google Scholar
Southgate, V., Chevallier, C., & Csibra, G. (2010). Seventeen-month-olds appeal to false beliefs to interpret others’ referential communication. Developmental Science, 13, 907912.Google Scholar
Southgate, V., Senju, A., & Csibra, G. (2007). Action anticipation through attribution of false belief by 2-year-olds. Psychological Science, 18, 587592.Google Scholar
Southgate, V., & Vernetti, A. (2014). Belief-based action prediction in preverbal infants. Cognition, 130, 110.Google Scholar
Spaepen, E., & Spelke, E. (2007). Will any doll do? 12-month-olds’ reasoning about goal objects. Cognitive Psychology, 54, 133154.Google Scholar
Takagishi, H., Kameshima, S., Schug, J., Koizumi, M., & Yamagishi, T. (2010). Theory of mind enhances preference for fairness. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105, 130137.Google Scholar
Ting, F., He, Z., & Baillargeon, R. (2021). Five-month-old infants attribute inferences based on general knowledge to agents. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 208, 105126.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1999). Having intentions, understanding intentions, and understanding communicative intentions. In Zelazo, P. D., Astington, J. W., & Olson, D. R. (eds.), Developing Theories of Intention: Social Understanding and Self-Control (pp. 6375). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Haberl, K. (2003). Understanding attention: 12-and 18-month-olds know what is new for other persons. Developmental Psychology, 39, 906912.Google Scholar
Vouloumanos, A., Martin, A., & Onishi, K. H. (2014). Do 6-month-olds understand that speech can communicate? Developmental Science, 17, 872879.Google Scholar
Wang, S., Baillargeon, R., & Brueckner, L. (2004). Young infants’ reasoning about hidden objects: Evidence from violation-of-expectation tasks with test trials only. Cognition, 93, 167198.Google Scholar
Wang, Y., & Su, Y. (2009). False belief understanding: Children catch it from classmates of different ages. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 33, 331336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Welder, A. N., & Graham, S. A. (2001). The influence of shape similarity and shared labels on infants’ inductive inferences about nonobvious object properties. Child Development, 72, 16531673.Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory of mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72, 655684.Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M., Fang, F., & Peterson, C. C. (2011). Sequential progressions in a theory-of-mind scale: Longitudinal perspectives. Child Development, 82, 780792.Google Scholar
Westra, E., & Carruthers, P. (2017). Pragmatic development explains the Theory-of-Mind scale. Cognition, 158, 165176.Google Scholar
Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103128.Google Scholar
Woodward, A. L. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor’s reach. Cognition, 69, 134.Google Scholar
Woodward, A. L. (1999). Infants’ ability to distinguish between purposeful and non-purposeful behaviors. Infant Behavior and Development, 22, 145160.Google Scholar
Woodward, A. L. (2005). The infant origins of intentional understanding. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 33, 229262.Google Scholar
Woodward, A. L., Sommerville, J. A., & Guajardo, J. J. (2001). How infants make sense of intentional action. In Malle, B. F., Moses, L. J., & Baldwin, D. A. (eds.), Intentions and Intentionality: Foundations of Social Cognition (pp. 149169). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Yazdi, A. A., German, T. P., Defeyter, M. A., & Siegal, M. (2006). Competence and performance in belief-desire reasoning across two cultures: The truth, the whole truth, and nothing but the truth about false belief? Cognition, 100, 343368.Google Scholar
Yott, J., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2012). Breaking the rules: Do infants have a true understanding of false belief? British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30, 156171.Google Scholar
Yott, J., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2016). Are infants’ theory-of-mind abilities well integrated? Implicit understanding of intentions, desires, and beliefs. Journal of Cognition and Development, 17, 683698.Google Scholar
Zmyj, N., Buttelmann, D., Carpenter, M., & Daum, M. M. (2010). The reliability of a model influences 14-month-olds’ imitation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 106, 208220.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×