Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-r6qrq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-28T17:16:41.084Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Social development

from Part VI - Social and emotional development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 October 2017

Brian Hopkins
Affiliation:
Lancaster University
Elena Geangu
Affiliation:
Lancaster University
Sally Linkenauger
Affiliation:
Lancaster University
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2017

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Further reading

Brownell, C.A., & Kopp, C.B. (Eds.) (2010). Socioemotional development in the toddler years: Transitions and transformations. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63, 1143.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R.A., Murphy, B., Karbon, M., Maszk, P., et al. (1994). The relations of emotionality and regulation to dispositional and situational empathy-related responding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 776797.Google Scholar
Smetana, J.G. (2006). Social-cognitive domain theory: Consistencies and variations in children’s moral and social judgments. In Killen, M. & Smetana, J.G. (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 119154). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2009). Why we cooperate. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

References

Brownell, C., Nichols, S., & Svetlova, M. (2013). Converging developments in prosocial behavior and self-other understanding in the second year of life: The second social-cognitive revolution. In Banaji, M. & Gelman, S. (Eds.), Navigating the social world: What infants, children, and other species can teach us (pp. 385390). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Callaghan, T., Moll, H., Rakoczy, H., Warneken, F., Liszkowski, U., Behne, T., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Early social cognition in three cultural contexts. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 76, 1142.Google ScholarPubMed
Gifford-Smith, M.E., & Brownell, C.A. (2003). Childhood peer relationships: Social acceptance, friendships, and peer networks. Journal of School Psychology, 41, 235284.Google Scholar
De Haan, M., & Carver, L.J. (2013). Development of brain networks for visual social‑emotional information processing in infancy. In Legerstee, M., Haley, D., & Bornstein, M. (Eds.), The developing infant mind: Integrating biology and experience (pp. 123145). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Herrmann, E., Call, J., Hernández-Lloreda, M.V., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition: The cultural intelligence hypothesis. Science, 317, 13601366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, S.S. (1996). Imitation or exploration? Young infants’ matching of adults’ oral gestures. Child Development, 67, 19521969.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Killen, M., & Rutland, A. (2011). Children and social exclusion: Morality, prejudice, and group identity. New York, NY: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Kinzler, K.D., Dupoux, E., & Spelke, E.S. (2007). The native language of social cognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 1257712580.Google Scholar
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Twelve-month-olds communicate helpfully and appropriately for knowledgeable and ignorant partners. Cognition, 108, 732739.Google Scholar
Lyons, D.E., Young, A.G., & Keil, F.C. (2007). The hidden structure of overimitation. Proceedings of the National Academy, 104, 1975119756.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A.N., & Moore, M.K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 7578.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paulus, M., & Moore, C. (2012). Producing and understanding prosocial actions in early childhood. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 42, 271305.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1997). The moral judgment of the child. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Sameroff, A.J. (Ed.) (2009). The transactional model of development: How children and contexts shape each other. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, M.F.H., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Young children enforce social norms. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 232236.Google Scholar
Schmidt, M.F., Svetlova, M., Johe, J., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Children’s developing understanding of legitimate reasons for allocating resources unequally. Cognitive Development, 37, 4252.Google Scholar
Svetlova, M., Nichols, S., & Brownell, C. (2010). Toddlers’ prosocial behavior: From instrumental to empathic to altruistic helping. Child Development, 81, 18141827.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In Moore, C. & Dunham, P. (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 103130). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in early infancy: A description of primary intersubjectivity. In Bullowa, M. (Ed.), Before speech. The beginning of interpersonal communication (pp. 321347). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2009). The roots of human altruism. British Journal of Psychology, 100, 455471.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×