Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-5g6vh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-28T05:02:55.976Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Perception and action

from Part VII - Motor and related development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 October 2017

Brian Hopkins
Affiliation:
Lancaster University
Elena Geangu
Affiliation:
Lancaster University
Sally Linkenauger
Affiliation:
Lancaster University
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2017

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Further reading

Adolph, K.E., & Berger, S.E. (2006). Reclaiming motor development. In Daimon, W. & Lerner, R. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 2: Cognition, perception, and language (6th ed., pp. 161213). New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
Rosenbaum, D.A. (2005). The Cinderella of psychology: The neglect of motor control in the science of mental life and behavior. American Psychologist, 60, 308317.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

References

Bakker, M., Sommerville, J.A., & Gredebäck, G. (2016). Enhanced neural processing of goal-directed actions after active training in 4-month-old infants. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 28, 472482.Google Scholar
Bizzi, E., & Ajemian, R. (2015). A hard scientific quest: Understanding voluntary movements. Daedalus, 144, 8395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekberg, T.L., Falck-Ytter, T., lte, S., Gredebäck, G., & the EASE team ( 2016). Reduced prospective motor control in 10-month-olds at risk for autism spectrum disorder. Clinical Psychological Science, 4, 129135.Google Scholar
Elsner, C., D’Ausilio, A., Gredebäck, G., Falck-Ytter, T., & Fadiga, L. (2013). The motor cortex is causally related to predictive eye movements during action observation. Neuropsychologia, 51, 488492.Google Scholar
Falck-Ytter, T., Gredebäck, G., & von Hofsten, C. (2006). Infants predict other people’s action goals. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 878879.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gottwald, J.M., & Gredebäck, G. (2015). Infants’ prospective control during object manipulation in an uncertain environment. Experimental Brain Research, 233, 23832390.Google Scholar
Gredebäck, G., & Daum, M. (2015). The microstructure of action perception in infancy: Decomposing the temporal structure of social information processing. Child Development Perspectives, 9, 7983.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gredebäck, G., & Falck-Ytter, T. (2015). Eye movements during action observation. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10, 591598.Google Scholar
Gredebäck, G., & von Hofsten, C. (2007). Taking an action perspective on infant’s object representations. Progress in Brain Research, 164, 265282.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grossman, T., & Johnson, M.H. (2007). The development of the social brain in human infancy. European Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 909919.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, D.N. (1976). A theory of visual control of braking based on information about time-to-collision. Perception 5, 437459.Google Scholar
Molenberghs, P., Cunnington, R., & Mattingley, J.B. (2012). Brain regions with mirror properties: A meta-analysis of 125 human fMRI studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36, 341349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Needham, A., Barrett, T., & Peterham, K. (2002). A pick-me-up for infants’ exploratory skills: Early simulated experiences reaching for objects using “sticky mitten” enhances young infants’ object exploration skills. Infant Behavior and Development, 25, 279295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nyström, P., Ljunghammar, T., Rosander, K., & von Hofsten, C. (2011). Using mu rhythm desynchronization to measure mirror neuron activity in infants. Developmental Science, 14, 327335.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosander, K., & von Hofsten, C. (2011). 10-month-olds have a developed action plan to be applied when observing others. Neuropsychologia, 49, 29112917.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rutherford, M.D., & Kuhlmeier, V.A. (2013). Social perception, detection and interpretation of animacy, agency, and intention. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skerry, A.E., Carey, S.E., & Spelke, E.S. (2013). First-person action experience reveals sensitivity to action efficiency in prereaching infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110, 1872818733.Google Scholar
Southgate, V., Johnson, M.H., Osborne, T., & Csibra, G. (2009). Predictive motor activation during action observation in human infants. Biology Letters, 5, 769772.Google Scholar
Spelke, E., Breinlinger, K., Macomber, J., & Jacobson, K. (1992). Origins of knowledge. Psychological Review, 99, 605632.Google Scholar
Van Elk, M., van Schie, H.T., Hunnius, S., Vesper, C., & Bekkering, H. (2008). You’ll never crawl alone: Neurophysiological evidence for experience-dependent motor resonance in infancy. NeuroImage, 43, 808814.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Von Hofsten, C. (1983). Catching skills in infancy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, 7585.Google Scholar
Von Hofsten, C. (2007). Action in development. Developmental Science, 10, 5460.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Von Hofsten, C., & Rosander, K. (2015). On the development of the mirror neuron system. In Rizzolatti, G. & Ferrari, P. (Eds.), New frontiers in mirror neurons research (pp. 274295). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Witherington, D.C., von Hofsten, C., Rosander, K., Robinette, A., Woollacott, M.H., & Bertenthal, B.I. (2002). The development of anticipatory postural adjustments in infancy. Infancy, 3, 495517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zoia, S., Bleason, L., D’Ottavio, G., Biancotto, M., Bulgheroni, M., & Castello, U. (2013). The development of upper limb movements: From fetal to post-natal life. PLoS ONE, 8, e80876.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×