Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-wq484 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T05:03:52.114Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

21 - Working memory dysfunctions in stroke patients

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 October 2009

Martial Van der Linden
Affiliation:
University of Geneva; University of Liège
Martine Poncelet
Affiliation:
University of Liège
Steve Majerus
Affiliation:
University of Liège; Belgian National Fund of Scientific Research (FNRS)
Olivier Godefroy
Affiliation:
Université de Picardie Jules Verne, Amiens
Julien Bogousslavsky
Affiliation:
Université de Lausanne, Switzerland
Get access

Summary

Working memory and the Baddeley and Hitch multi-component model

Working memory refers to a system involved in the short-term maintenance and manipulation of information necessary for performing complex cognitive tasks. One of the most influential views of working memory has been put forth by Baddeley and Hitch (1974; see also Baddeley, 1986). The Baddeley and Hitch model comprises a modality-free controlling central executive that is aided by a number of subsidiary slave systems which ensure temporary maintenance of information. Two such systems have been more deeply explored: the phonological loop and the visuospatial sketchpad. The phonological loop system is specialized for processing acoustic and verbal material and is composed of two subsystems: a phonological store which can hold memory traces for a few seconds, and an articulatory rehearsal mechanism (analogous to subvocal speech) which permits memory traces to be refreshed. Neuropsychological and neuroimaging data (Baddeley, 2003) suggest that the phonological store and the articulatory rehearsal mechanism are associated with the left inferior parietal region (BA 40) and the left inferior frontal cortex (Broca's area)/premotor cortex (BA 6/44), respectively. The visuospatial sketchpad system is assumed to be involved in temporarily maintaining and manipulating visuospatial information. It may actually be composed of two distinct systems, one for maintaining visual representations, and another for spatial ones. A fractionation of the visuospatial sketchpad, analogous to that of the phonological loop, has also been proposed: a visual storage component and a more dynamic retrieval and rehearsal mechanism.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Amberla, K., Wäljas, M., Tuominen, S., et al. (2004). Insidious cognitive decline in CADASIL. Stroke, 35, 1598–602.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. (1966). Short-term memory for word sequences as a function of acoustic, semantic and formal similarity. Q. J. Exp. Psychol., 18, 362–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working Memory. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory?Trends Cogn. Sci., 4, 417–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 4, 829–39.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. and Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In Bower, G., ed., The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Vol. 8, New York: Academic Press, pp. 47–90.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D., Papagno, C. and Vallar, G. (1988). When long-term learning depends on short-term storage. J. Mem. Lang., 27, 586–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartha, B. and Benke, T. (2003). Acute conduction aphasia: An analysis of 20 cases. Brain Lang., 85, 93–108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Basso, A., Spinnler, H., Vallar, G. and Zanobio, M. E. (1982). Left hemisphere damage and selective impairment of auditory verbal short-term memory. A case study. Neuropsychologia, 20, 263–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belleville, S., Peretz, I. and Arguin, M. (1992). Contribution of articulatory rehearsal to short-term memory: Evidence from a case of selective disruption. Brain Lang., 43, 713–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collette, F. and Linden, M. (2002). Brain imaging of the central executive component of working memory. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev., 26, 105–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collette, F., Linden, M., Laureys, S., et al. (2005). Exploring the unity and diversity of the neural substrates of executive functioning. Human Brain Mapping, 25, 409–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dagenbach, D., Kubat-Silman, A. K. and Absher, J. R. (2001). Human verbal working memory impairments associated with thalamic damage. Int. J. Neurosci., 111, 67–87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Della Sala, S., Logie, R. H., Beschin, N. and Denis, M. (2004). Preserved visuo-spatial transformations in representational neglect. Neuropsychologia, 42, 1358–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Francis, D. R., Clark, N. and Humphreys, G. W. (2003). The treatment of an auditory working memory deficit and the implications for sentence comprehension abilities in mild “receptive” aphasia. Aphasiology, 17, 723–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freedman, M. and Martin, R. C. (2001). Dissociable components of short-term memory and their relation to long-term learning. Cogn. Neuropsychol., 18, 193–226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessels, R. P. C., Zandvoort, M. J. E., Postma, A., Kappelle, L. J. and Haan, E. H. F. (2000). The Corsi block-tapping task: Standardization and normative data. Appl. Neuropsychol., 7, 252–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leskelä, M., Hietanen, M., Kalska, H.,et al. (1999). Executive functions and speed of mental processing in elderly patients with frontal or nonfrontal ischemic stroke. Eur. J. Neurol., 6, 653–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Majerus, S., Laureys, S., Collette, F., et al. (2003). Phonological STM networks after recovery of Landau-Kleffner syndrome. Human Brain Mapping, 19, 133–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Majerus, S., Kaa, M. A., Renard, C., Linden, M. and Poncelet, M. (2005). Treating verbal short-term memory deficits by increasing the duration of temporary phonological representations: A case study. Brain Lang., 95(1), 174–75.
Majerus, S., Linden, M., Poncelet, M. and Metz-Lutz, M.-N. (2004). Can phonological and semantic STM be dissociated? Further evidence from Landau-Kleffner syndrome. Cogn. Neuropsychol., 21, 491–512.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malhotra, P., Jäger, H. R., Parton, A., et al. (2005). Spatial working memory capacity in unilateral neglect. Brain, 128, 424–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malm, J., Kristensen, B., Karlsson, T., et al. (1998). Cognitive impairment in young adults with infratentorial infarcts. Neurology, 51, 433–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malouin, F., Belleville, S., Richards, C. L., Desrosiers, J. and Doyon, J. (2004). Working memory and mental practice outcomes after stroke. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil., 85, 177–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshuetz, C., Smith, E. E., Jonides, J., DeGutis, J. and Chenevert, T. L. (2000). Order information in working memory: FMRI evidence for parietal and prefrontal mechanisms. J. Cogn. Neurosci., 12 (Suppl. 2), S130–S144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, R. C., Shelton, J. R. and Yaffee, L. S. (1994). Language processing and working memory: Neuropsychological evidence for separate phonological and semantic capacities. J. Mem. Lang., 33, 83–111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayer, E., Reicherts, M., Deloche, G., et al. (2003). Number processing after stroke: Anatomoclinical correlations in oral and written codes. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc., 9, 899–912.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H. and Howerter, A. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol., 41, 49–100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norman, D. A. and Shallice, T. (1980). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behavior. Center for Human Information Processing (Technical report No. 99). Reprinted in revised form in Consciousness and Self-regulation. Advances in Research, ed. Davidson, R. J., Schartz, G. E. and Shapiro, , (1986), Vol 4, New York: Plenum Press, pp. 1–18.Google Scholar
Prabhakaran, V., Narayanan, K., Zhao, Z. and Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2000). Integration of diverse information in working memory within the frontal lobe. Nature Neurosci., 3, 85–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ravizza, S. M. and Ciranni, M. A. (2002). Contributions of the prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia to set shifting. J. Cogn. Neurosci., 14, 472–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silveri, M. A. and Cappa, A. (2003). Segregation of the neural correlates of language and phonological short-term memory. Cortex, 39, 913–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Takayama, Y., Kinomoto, K. and Nakamura, K. (2004). Selective impairment of the auditory-verbal short-term memory due to a lesion of the superior temporal gyrus. Eur. Neurol., 51, 115–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vallar G. and Papagno, C. (2002). Neuropsychological impairments of verbal short-term memory. In Handbook of Memory Disorders, 2nd Edition, ed. Baddeley, A. D., Kopelman, M. D. and Wilson, B. A.. John Wiley & Sons Ltd, pp. 249–70.Google Scholar
Van der Linden, M. and Poncelet, M. (1998). The role of working memory in language and communication disorders. In Handbook of Neurolinguistics, ed. Stemmer, B. and Whitaker, H. A.. Academic Press, pp. 289–300.Google Scholar
Vataja, R., Pohjasvaara, T., Mäntylä, R., et al. (2003). MRI correlates of executive dysfunction in patients with ischaemic stroke. Eur. J. Neurol., 10, 625–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×