Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-vfjqv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T08:31:16.122Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 3 - Psychological Factors in Autism Spectrum Disorders

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 January 2019

Fred R. Volkmar
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adams, N. C., and Jarrold, C. (2012). Inhibition in autism: Children with autism have difficulty inhibiting irrelevant distractors but not prepotent responses. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(6), 10521063. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1345-3Google Scholar
Adamson, L. B. (1995). Communication Development during Infancy. Madison, WI: Brown & Benchmark Publishers.Google Scholar
Ainsworth, M. (1976). Discussion of papers by Suomi and Bowlby. In Animal Models in Human Psychobiology (pp. 3747). Springer.Google Scholar
Allen, G., and Courchesne, E. (2001). Attention function and dysfunction in autism. Frontiers in Bioscience, 6, D10519. https://doi.org/11171544Google Scholar
Ambery, F. Z., Russell, A. J., Perry, K., Morris, R., and Murphy, D. G. M. (2006). Neuropsychological functioning in adults with Asperger syndrome. Autism, 10(6), 551564. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361306068507Google Scholar
Andersen, P. N., Skogli, E. W., Hovik, K. T., et al. (2014). Working memory arrest in children with high-functioning autism compared to children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Results from a 2-year longitudinal study. Autism. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361314524844Google Scholar
Antezana, L., Mosner, M. G., Troiani, V., and Yerys, B. E. (2015). Social-emotional inhibition of return in children with autism spectrum disorder versus typical development. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(4), 12361246. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2661-9Google Scholar
APA (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th edn), Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.Google Scholar
Barnes, K. A., Howard, J. H., Howard, D. V., et al. (2008). Intact implicit learning of spatial context and temporal sequences in childhood autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychology, 22(5), 563570. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.22.5.563CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT press.Google Scholar
Bedford, R., Pickles, A., Gliga, T., et al. (2014). Additive effects of social and non‐social attention during infancy relate to later autism spectrum disorder. Developmental Science, 17(4), 612620.Google Scholar
Bennetto, L., Pennington, B. F., and Rogers, S. J. (1996). Intact and impaired memory functions in autism. Child Development, 67(4), 18161835.Google Scholar
Ben-Itzchak, E., Watson, L. R., and Zachor, D. A. (2014). Cognitive ability is associated with different outcome trajectories in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 9, 22212229.Google Scholar
Bennett, E. A. and Heaton, P. (2017). Defining the clinical and cognitive phenotype of child savants with autism spectrum disorder. Current Pediatric Research, 21(1), 140147.Google Scholar
Bigham, S., Boucher, J., Mayes, A., and Anns, S. (2010). Assessing recollection and familiarity in autistic spectrum disorders: Methods and findings. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(7), 878889. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-0937-7Google Scholar
Bodner, K. E., Williams, D. L., Engelhardt, C. R., and Minshew, N. J. (2014). A comparison of measures for assessing the level and nature of intelligence in verbal children and adults with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 11, 14341442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bölte, S., Duketis, E., Poustka, F., and Holtmann, M. (2011). Sex differences in cognitive domains and their clinical correlates in higher-functioning autism spectrum disorders. Autism, 4, 497511.Google Scholar
Bölte, S., Dziobek, I., and Poustka, F. (2009). Brief report: The level and nature of autistic intelligence revisited. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 4, 678682.Google Scholar
Bölte, S. and Poustka, F. (2004). Comparing the intelligence profiles of savant and nonsavant individuals with autistic disorder, Intelligence, 2, 121131.Google Scholar
Bolton, P. F., Golding, J., Emond, A., and Steer, C. D. (2012). Autism spectrum disorder and autistic traits in the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children: Precursors and early signs. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(3), 249260.Google Scholar
Boucher, J., Cowell, P., Howard, M., et al. (2005). A combined clinical, neuropsychological, and neuroanatomical study of adults with high functioning autism. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 10(3), 165213. https://doi.org/P01U93L300864771Google Scholar
Boucher, J., Mayes, A., and Bigham, S. (2012). Memory in autistic spectrum disorder. Psychological Bulletin, 138(3), 458496. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026869Google Scholar
Boucher, J. and Warrington, E. K. (1976). Memory deficits in early infantile autism: Some similarities to the amnesic syndrome. British Journal of Psychology, 67(1), 73.Google Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and Loss: Attachment. New York: V. Basic BooksGoogle Scholar
Bowler, D. M., Gaigg, S. B., and Gardiner, J. M. (2008). Effects of related and unrelated context on recall and recognition by adults with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia, 46(4), 993999. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.12.004Google Scholar
Brady, D. I., Schwean, V. L., Saklofske, D. H., et al. (2013). Conceptual and Perceptual Set-shifting executive abilities in young adults with Asperger's syndrome. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7(12), 16311637.Google Scholar
Brewer, R., Biotti, F., Catmur, C., et al. (2016). Can neurotypical individuals read autistic facial expressions? Atypical production of emotional facial expressions in autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research, 9(2), 262271.Google Scholar
Brown, J., Aczel, B., Jimenez, L., Kaufman, S. B., and Grant, K. P. (2010). Intact implicit learning in autism spectrum conditions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006), 124. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210903536910Google Scholar
Casey, B. J., Gordon, C. T., Mannheim, G. B., and Rumsey, J. M. (1993). Dysfunctional attention in autistic savants. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 15(6), 933946. https://doi.org/10.1080/01688639308402609Google Scholar
Christ, S. E., Holt, D. D., White, D. A., and Green, L. (2007). Inhibitory control in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(6), 11551165. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0259-yGoogle Scholar
Christ, S. E., Kester, L. E., Bodner, K. E., and Miles, J. H. (2011). Evidence for selective inhibitory impairment in individuals with autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychology. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024256Google Scholar
Caparulo, B. K. and Cohen, D. J. (1977). Cognitive structures, language, and emerging social competence in autistic and aphasic children. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 16(4), 620645.Google Scholar
Chakrabarti, S. and Fombonne, E. (2005). Pervasive developmental disorders in preschool children: Confirmation of high prevalence. American Journal of Psychiatry, 6, 11331141.Google Scholar
Charman, T., Jones, C. R. G., Pickles, A., et al. (2011a). Defining the cognitive phenotype of autism. Brain Research, Supplement C, 1021.Google Scholar
Charman, T., Pickles, A., Simonoff, E., et al., (2011b). IQ in children with autism spectrum disorders: Data from the Special Needs and Autism Project (SNAP), Psychological Medicine, 3, 619627.Google Scholar
Chatham, C. H., Taylor, K. I., Charman, T., et al. (2017). Adaptive behavior in autism: Minimal clinically important differences on the Vineland-II, Autism Research, n/a-n/a.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chawarska, K. and Bearss, K. (2008). Assessment of cognitive and adaptive skills. In Chawarsha, K., Klin, A., and Volkmar, F. R. (Eds), Autism spectrum disorders in infants and toddlers (pp. 5075). New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Chawarska, K., Macari, S., and Shic, F. (2013). Decreased spontaneous attention to social scenes in 6-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 74(3), 195203.Google Scholar
Chevallier, C., Kohls, G., Troiani, V., et al. (2012). The social motivation theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(4), 231239.Google Scholar
Chevallier, C. (2013). Theory of Mind. In Encyclopedia of Autism Spectrum Disorders (pp. 31113115). Springer: New York.Google Scholar
Chetcuti, L., Hudry, K., Grant, M., and Vivanti, G. (2017) Imitation impairments in autism spectrum disorder: A social motivation or motor-execution problem? Autism, doi: 10.1177/13623613177340Google Scholar
Christensen, D. L., Bilder, D. A., Zahorodny, W., et al. (2016). Prevalence and characteristics of autism spectrum disorder among 4-year-old children in the autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 1, 18.Google Scholar
D'Cruz, A.-M., Ragozzino, M. E., Mosconi, M. W., Shrestha, S., Cook, E. H., and Sweeney, J. A. (2013). Reduced behavioral flexibility in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychology, 27(2), 152160. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031721Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R., and Maurer, R. G. (1978). A neurological model for childhood autism. Archives of Neurology, 35(12), 777786.Google Scholar
Dalsant, A., Truzzi, A., Setoh, P., and Esposito, G. (2015). Maternal bonding in childhood moderates autonomic responses to distress stimuli in adult males. Behavioural Brain Research, 292, 428431.Google Scholar
Dapretto, M., Davies, M. S., Pfeifer, J., et al. (2006). Understanding emotions in others: Mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 9(1), 2830.Google Scholar
Dawson, G., Meltzoff, A. N., Osterling, J., et al. (1998). Children with autism fail to orient to naturally occurring social stimuli. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28(6), 479485.Google Scholar
Dawson, G. and Bernier, R. (2007). Social brain circuitry in autism. In Coch, D., Dawson, G., and Fischer, K. (Eds), Human Behavior and the Developing Brain. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Dawson, G., Sterling, L., Faja, S. (2009) Autism: risk factors, risk processes, and outcome. In: de Haan, M., Gunnar, M. (Eds). Handbook of Developmental Social Neuroscience. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 435458.Google Scholar
Dawson, M., Soulières, I., Ann Gernsbacher, M., and Mottron, L. (2007). The level and nature of autistic intelligence, Psychological Science, 8, 657662.Google Scholar
Dawson, G., Munson, J., Estes, A., et al. (2002). Neurocognitive function and joint attention ability in young children with autism spectrum disorder versus developmental delay. Child Development, 73(2), 345358.Google Scholar
DeLong, G. R. (1992). Autism, amnesia, hippocampus, and learning. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 16(1), 6370.Google Scholar
Dichter, G. S. and Belger, A. (2007). Social stimuli interfere with cognitive control in autism. NeuroImage, 35(3), 12191230. https://doi.org/S1053-8119(06)01242-0Google Scholar
Elison, J. T., Paterson, S. J., Wolff, J. J., IBIS Network, et al. (2013). White matter microstructure and atypical visual orienting in 7-month-olds at risk for autism. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 170(8), 899908. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12091150Google Scholar
Di Giorgio, E., Frasnelli, E., Salva, O., et al. (2016). Difference in visual social predispositions between newborns at low-and high-risk for autism. Scientific Reports, 6.Google Scholar
DeMyer, M. K., Barton, S., Alpern, G. D., et al. (1974). The measured intelligence of autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 1, 4260.Google Scholar
Dissanayake, C. and Crossley, S. A. (1996). Proximity and sociable behaviours in autism: Evidence for attachment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37(2), 149156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dufour, N., Redcay, E., Young, L. et al. (2013). Similar brain activation during false belief tasks in a large sample of adults with and without autism. PloS one, 8(9), e75468.Google Scholar
Edmunds, S., Ibañez, L., Warren, Z., et al. (2017). Longitudinal prediction of language emergence in infants at high and low risk for autism spectrum disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 29(1), 319329.Google Scholar
Edwards, L. A. (2014). A meta‐analysis of imitation abilities in individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research, 7(3), 363380.Google Scholar
Elsabbagh, M., Fernandes, J., Jane Webb, S., Dawson, G., Charman, T., and Johnson, M. H. (2013). Disengagement of visual attention in infancy is associated with emerging autism in toddlerhood. Biological Psychiatry. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.11.030Google Scholar
Elsabbagh, M., Volein, A., Holmboe, K., et al. (2009). Visual orienting in the early broader autism phenotype: disengagement and facilitation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 50(5), 637642. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.02051.xGoogle Scholar
Evers, K., Steyaert, J., Noens, I., and Wagemans, J. (2015). Reduced recognition of dynamic facial emotional expressions and emotion-specific response bias in children with an autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(6), 1774.Google Scholar
Fan, J., Bernardi, S., Dam, N. T., et al. (2012). Functional deficits of the attentional networks in autism. Brain and Behavior, 2(5), 647660. https://doi.org/10.1002/brb3.90Google Scholar
Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., and Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(3), 340347. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892902317361886Google Scholar
Feldman, R. and Greenbaum, C. W. (1997). Affect regulation and synchrony in mother–infant play as precursors to the development of symbolic competence. Infant Mental Health Journal, 18(1), 423.Google Scholar
Fischer, J., Koldewyn, K., Jiang, Y. V., and Kanwisher, N. (2014). Unimpaired attentional disengagement and social orienting in children with autism. Clinical Psychological Science, 2(2), 214223. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702613496242Google Scholar
Fridenson-Hayo, S., Berggren, S., Lassalle, A., et al. (2016). Basic and complex emotion recognition in children with autism: Cross-cultural findings. Molecular Autism, 7(1), 52.Google Scholar
Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the Enigma, (2nd edn) Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Fombonne, E. (1999). The epidemiology of autism: a review, Psychological Medicine, 4, 769.Google Scholar
Fombonne, E. (2009). Epidemiology of pervasive developmental disorders, Pediatric Research, 6, 591598.Google Scholar
Gallagher, S. and Varga, S. (2015). Conceptual issues in autism spectrum disorders. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 28(2), 127132.Google Scholar
Gallese, V., Rochat, M. J., and Berchio, C. (2013). The mirror mechanism and its potential role in autism spectrum disorder. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 55(1), 1522.Google Scholar
Gillespie-Lynch, K., Sepeta, L., Wang, Y., et al. (2012). Early childhood predictors of the social competence of adults with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(2), 161174.Google Scholar
Gliga, T. and Csibra, G. (2007). Seeing the face through the eyes: a developmental perspective on face expertise. Progress in Brain Research, 164, 323339.Google Scholar
Grelotti, D. J., Gauthier, I., and Schultz, R. T. (2002). Social interest and the development of cortical face specialization: What autism teaches us about face processing. Developmental Psychobiology, 40(3), 213225.Google Scholar
Gaigg, S. B. and Bowler, D. M. (2007). Differential fear conditioning in Asperger's syndrome: Implications for an amygdala theory of autism. Neuropsychologia, 45(9), 21252134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.01.012Google Scholar
Gardiner, J. M., Bowler, D. M., and Grice, S. J. (2003). Further evidence of preserved priming and impaired recall in adults with Asperger's syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33(3), 259269. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1024450416355Google Scholar
Geurts, H. M., Corbett, B., and Solomon, M. (2009). The paradox of cognitive flexibility in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(2), 7482. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.006Google Scholar
Geurts, H. M., Luman, M., and van Meel, C. S. (2008). What's in a game: The effect of social motivation on interference control in boys with ADHD and autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(8), 848857. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01916.xGoogle Scholar
Geurts, H. M., van den Bergh, S. F. W. M., and Ruzzano, L. (2014). Prepotent response inhibition and interference control in autism spectrum disorders: two meta-analyses. Autism Research, 7(4), 407420. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1369Google Scholar
Gonsiorowski, A., Williamson, R. A., & Robins, D. L. (2016). Brief report: Imitation of object-directed acts in young children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 46(2), 691697.Google Scholar
Greene, D. J., Colich, N., Iacoboni, M., Zaidel, E., Bookheimer, S. Y., and Dapretto, M. (2011). Atypical neural networks for social orienting in autism spectrum disorders. NeuroImage, 56(1), 354362. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.031Google Scholar
Hadjikhani, N., Joseph, R. M., Manoach, D. S., et al. (2009). Body expressions of emotion do not trigger fear contagion in autism spectrum disorder. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4(1), 7078.Google Scholar
Hamilton, A. (2013). Reflecting on the mirror neuron system in autism: a systematic review of current theories. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 91105.Google Scholar
Happé, F. G. (1995). The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism, Child Development, 3, 843855.Google Scholar
Happé, F. G. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters' thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(2), 129154.Google Scholar
Happé, F., Ronald, A., and Plomin, R. (2006). Time to give up on a single explanation for autism. Nature Neuroscience, 9(10), 12181220.Google Scholar
Happé, F., Cook, J. L., and Bird, G. (2017). The structure of social cognition: In(ter) dependence of sociocognitive processes. Annual Review of Psychology, 68, 243267.Google Scholar
Harms, M., Martin, A., and Wallace, G. L. (2010). Facial emotion recognition in autism spectrum disorders: a review of behavioral and neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychology Review, 20(3), 290322.Google Scholar
Heaton, P., Williams, K., Cummins, O., and Happé, F. G. E. (2007). Beyond perception: musical representation and on-line processing in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(7), 13551360. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0283-yGoogle Scholar
Hobson, R. P. (2014). Autism and emotion. In Volkmar, F., Paul, R., Rogers, S. J., Pelphrey, K. A. (Eds) Handbook of Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders, Fourth edition. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons.Google Scholar
Howlin, P. (1986). An overview of social behavior in autism. In Schopler, E. and Mesibov, G. B. (Eds), Social Behavior in Autism, (103131). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Howlin, P., Goode, S., Hutton, J., and Rutter, M. (2009). Savant skills in autism: psychometric approaches and parental reports, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 1522, 13591367.Google Scholar
Hutman, T., Rozga, A., DeLaurentis, A., et al. (2010). Response to distress in infants at risk for autism: a prospective longitudinal study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(9), 10101020.Google Scholar
Iacoboni, M. and Dapretto, M. (2006). The mirror neuron system and the consequences of its dysfunction. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7(12), 942951.Google Scholar
Ingersoll, B., Schreibman, L., and Tran, Q. H. (2003). Effect of sensory feedback on immediate object imitation in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33(6), 673683.Google Scholar
Izawa, J., Pekny, S. E., Marko, M. et al. (2012). Motor learning relies on integrated sensory inputs in ADHD, but over-selectively on proprioception in autism spectrum conditions. Autism Research, 5(2), 124136.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. H. (2014). Autism: demise of the innate social orienting hypothesis. Current Biology, 24(1), R30R31.Google Scholar
Jones, A. P., Happé, F. G., Gilbert, F., et al. (2010). Feeling, caring, knowing: different types of empathy deficit in boys with psychopathic tendencies and autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(11), 11881197.Google Scholar
Jones, C. R., Pickles, A., Falcaro, M., et al. (2011). A multimodal approach to emotion recognition ability in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(3), 275285.Google Scholar
Jones, E. J., Gliga, T., Bedford, R., et al. (2014). Developmental pathways to autism: a review of prospective studies of infants at risk. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 39, 133.Google Scholar
Jones, W. and Klin, A. (2013). Attention to eyes is present but in decline in 2–6-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism. Nature, 504(7480), 427431.Google Scholar
Kamio, Y. and Toichi, M. (2007). Memory illusion in high-functioning autism and Asperger's disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(5), 867876. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0214-yGoogle Scholar
Kana, R., Maximo, J. O., Williams, D., et al. (2015). Aberrant functioning of the theory-of-mind network in children and adolescents with autism. Molecular Autism, 6(1), 59.Google Scholar
Kanne, S. M., Gerber, A. J., Quirmbach, L. M., et al. (2011). The role of adaptive behavior in autism spectrum disorders: Implications for functional outcome, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 8, 10071018.Google Scholar
Kasari, C., Sigman, M., and Yirmiya, N. (1993). Focused and social attention of autistic children in interactions with familiar and unfamiliar adults: A comparison of autistic, mentally retarded, and normal children. Development and Psychopathology, 5(3), 403414.Google Scholar
Kawakubo, Y., Kasai, K., Okazaki, S., et al. (2007). Electrophysiological abnormalities of spatial attention in adults with autism during the gap overlap task. Clinical Neurophysiology, 118(7), 14641471. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.015Google Scholar
Kirkovski, M., Enticott, P. G., and Fitzgerald, P. B. (2013). A review of the role of female gender in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 11, 25842603.Google Scholar
Klin, A. (2000). Attributing social meaning to ambiguous visual stimuli in higher-functioning autism and Asperger syndrome: The social attribution task. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 41(7), 831846.Google Scholar
Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., and Volkmar, F. (2003). The enactive mind, or from actions to cognition: lessons from autism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 358(1430), 345360.Google Scholar
Klin, A., Saulnier, C. A., Sparrow, S. S., et al. (2007). Social and communication abilities and disabilities in higher functioning individuals with autism spectrum disorders: The Vineland and the ADOS. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 4, 748759.Google Scholar
Keehn, B., Lincoln, A. J., Müller, R.-A., and Townsend, J. (2010). Attentional networks in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 51(11), 12511259. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02257.xGoogle Scholar
Keehn, B., Müller, R.-A., and Townsend, J. (2013). Atypical attentional networks and the emergence of autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(2), 164183. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.11.014Google Scholar
Kikuchi, Y., Senju, A., Akechi, H., Tojo, Y., Osanai, H., and Hasegawa, T. (2011). Atypical disengagement from faces and its modulation by the control of eye fixation in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(5), 629645. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-1082-zGoogle Scholar
Kleberg, J. L., Thorup, E., and Falck-Ytter, T. (2017). Reduced visual disengagement but intact phasic alerting in young children with autism. Autism Research, 10(3), 539545. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1675Google Scholar
Kourkoulou, A., Leekam, S. R., and Findlay, J. M. (2011). Implicit learning of local context in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1237-6Google Scholar
Kokkinaki, T. (2003). A longitudinal, naturalistic and cross‐cultural study on emotions in early infant‐parent imitative interactions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21(2), 243258.Google Scholar
Koyama, T., Kamio, Y., Inada, N., and Kurita, H. (2009). Sex differences in WISC-III profiles of children with high-functioning pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 1, 135.Google Scholar
Kuschner, E. S., Bennetto, L., and Yost, K. (2007). Patterns of nonverbal cognitive functioning in young children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 5, 795807.Google Scholar
Lai, C. L. E., Lau, Z., Lui, S. S. Y., et al. (2016). Meta-analysis of neuropsychological measures of executive functioning in children and adolescents with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1723Google Scholar
Lai, M.-C., Lombardo, M. V., Ruigrok, A. N., et al. (2012). Cognition in males and females with autism: Similarities and differences, PLoS One, 10, e47198.Google Scholar
Lartseva, A., Dijkstra, T., and Buitelaar, J. K. (2014). Emotional language processing in autism spectrum disorders: a systematic review. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 991.Google Scholar
Lerner, M. D., McPartland, J. C., and Morris, J. P. (2013). Multimodal emotion processing in autism spectrum disorders: An event-related potential study. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 1121.Google Scholar
Leekam, S. (2016). Social cognitive impairment and autism: What are we trying to explain? Philosophical Transitions of the Royal Society B, 371(1686), 20150082.Google Scholar
Leslie, A. M. and Thaiss, L. (1992). Domain specificity in conceptual development: Neuropsychological evidence from autism. Cognition, 43(3), 225251.Google Scholar
Leung, R. C. and Zakzanis, K. K. (2014). Brief Report: Cognitive Flexibility in Autism Spectrum Disorders: A Quantitative Review. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-014-2136-4Google Scholar
Lind, S. E. (2010). Memory and the self in autism. Autism, 14(5), 430456.Google Scholar
Livingston, L. A. and Happé, F. (2017). Conceptualising compensation in neurodevelopmental disorders: Reflections from autism spectrum disorder, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, Supplement C, 729742.Google Scholar
Lockyer, L. and Rutter, M. (1970). A five‐to fifteen‐year follow‐up study of infantile psychosis: IV. Patterns of Cognitive Ability, British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 2, 152163.Google Scholar
Loomes, R., Hull, L., and Mandy, W. P. L. (2017). What is the male-to-female ratio in autism spectrum disorder? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 6, 466474.Google Scholar
Lord, C. and Schopler, E. (1985). Brief report: Differences in sex ratios in autism as a function of measured intelligence. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2, 185193.Google Scholar
Libero, L. E., Maximo, J. O., Deshpande, H. D., et al. (2014). The role of mirroring and mentalizing networks in mediating action intentions in autism. Molecular Autism, 5(1), 50.Google Scholar
Lombardo, M. V., Chakrabarti, B., Bullmore, E. T., Baron-Cohen, S. (2011). Specialization of right temporo-parietal junction for mentalizing and its relation to social impairments in autism. Neuroimage, 56, 18321838.Google Scholar
Lovaas, O. I., Koegel, R. L., and Schreibman, L. (1979). Stimulus overselectivity in autism: A review of research. Psychological Bulletin, 86(6), 1236.Google Scholar
Lozier, L. M., Vanmeter, J. W., and Marsh, A. A. (2014). Impairments in facial affect recognition associated with autism spectrum disorders: A meta-analysis. Development and Psychopathology, 26(4pt1), 933945.Google Scholar
Luna, B., Doll, S. K., Hegedus, S. J., Minshew, N. J., and Sweeney, J. A. (2007). Maturation of executive function in autism. Biological Psychiatry, 61(4), 474481. https://doi.org/S0006-3223(06)00381-7Google Scholar
Luyster, R. J., Bick, J., Westerlund, A., and Nelson, C. A. (2017). Testing the effects of expression, intensity and age on emotional face processing in ASD. Neuropsychologia. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.06.023.Google Scholar
Maddox, B. B., Cleary, P., Kuschner, E. S., et al. (2017). Lagging skills contribute to challenging behaviors in children with autism spectrum disorder without intellectual disability. Autism, 1362361317712651. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361317712651Google Scholar
Magiati, I., Tay, X. W., and Howlin, P. (2014). Cognitive, language, social and behavioural outcomes in adults with autism spectrum disorders: a systematic review of longitudinal follow-up studies in adulthood, Clinical Psychology Review, 1, 7386.Google Scholar
Mahler, M. S. (1952). On child psychosis and schizophrenia 1: autistic and symbiotic infantile psychoses. The Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 7(1), 286305.Google Scholar
Mandy, W., Chilvers, R., Chowdhury, U., et al. (2012). Sex differences in autism spectrum disorder: evidence from a large sample of children and adolescents, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 7, 13041313.Google Scholar
Mandy, W., Murin, M., and Skuse, D. (2015). The cognitive profile in autism spectrum disorders. In Leboyer, M., & Chaste, P. (Eds), Autism spectrum disorders: Phenotypes, mechanisms and treatments (pp. 3445). Basel: Karger.Google Scholar
Marotta, A., Pasini, A., Ruggiero, S., Maccari, L., Rosa, C., Lupiáñez, J., and Casagrande, M. (2013). Inhibition of return in response to eye gaze and peripheral cues in young people with Asperger's Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(4), 917923. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1636-3Google Scholar
Marsh, L., Pearson, A., Ropar, D., and Hamilton, A. (2013). Children with autism do not overimitate. Current Biology, 23(7), R266R268.Google Scholar
Mayes, S. D. and Calhoun, S. L. (2003). Analysis of WISC-III, Stanford-Binet: IV, and academic achievement test scores in children with autism, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 3, 329341.Google Scholar
Maunder, R. G., Lancee, W. J., Nolan, R. P., Hunter, J. J., and Tannenbaum, D. W. (2006). The relationship of attachment insecurity to subjective stress and autonomic function during standardized acute stress in healthy adults. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 60(3), 283290.Google Scholar
Mazza, M., Pino, M. C., Mariano, M., et al. (2014). Affective and cognitive empathy in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 791.Google Scholar
Miller, E. K. and Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 167202. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167Google Scholar
Miller, M., Iosif, A. M., Hill, M., et al. (2017). Response to name in infants developing autism spectrum disorder: a prospective study. The Journal of Pediatrics, 183, 141146.Google Scholar
Miyake, A. and Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 814.Google Scholar
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., and Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49100. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734Google Scholar
Mosconi, M. W., Kay, M., D'Cruz, A.-M., Seidenfeld, A., Guter, S., Stanford, L. D., and Sweeney, J. A. (2009). Impaired inhibitory control is associated with higher-order repetitive behaviors in autism spectrum disorders. Psychological Medicine, 39(9), 15591566. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291708004984Google Scholar
Mostofsky, S. H. and Ewen, J. B. (2011). Altered connectivity and action model formation in autism is autism. The Neuroscientist, 17(4), 437448.Google Scholar
Mottron, L., Morasse, K., and Belleville, S. (2001). A study of memory functioning in individuals with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 42(2), 253260.Google Scholar
Mundy, P. (2016). Autism and joint attention: development, neuroscience, and clinical fundamentals. New York: Guilford Publications.Google Scholar
Mundy, P. (2011). The social behavior of autism: A parallel and distributed information processing perspective. In: Amaral, D. G., Dawson, G., Geschwind, D. H. (Eds). Autism Spectrum Disorders. Oxford University Press: New York, pp. 149171.Google Scholar
Mundy, P. (1995). Joint attention and social-emotional approach behavior in children with autism. Development and Psychopathology, 7(1), 6382.Google Scholar
Mutreja, R., Craig, C., and O'Boyle, M. W. (2015). Attentional network deficits in children with autism spectrum disorder. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 19. https://doi.org/10.3109/17518423.2015.1017663Google Scholar
Nader, A.-M., Courchesne, V., Dawson, M., and Soulières, I. (2016). Does WISC-IV underestimate the intelligence of autistic children? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 5, 15821589.Google Scholar
Naber, F. B., Swinkels, S. H., Buitelaar, J. K., Dietz, C., et al. (2007). Joint attention and attachment in toddlers with autism. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35(6), 899911.Google Scholar
Nebel, M. B., Eloyan, A., Nettles, C. A., et al. (2016). Intrinsic visual-motor synchrony correlates with social deficits in autism. Biological Psychiatry, 79(8), 633641.Google Scholar
Newell, A. (1990). Unified theories of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Nowell, K. P., Schanding, G. T., Kanne, S. M., and Goin-Kochel, R. P. (2015). Cognitive profiles in youth with autism spectrum disorder: an investigation of base rate discrepancies using the differential ability scales—second edition, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(7), 19781988.Google Scholar
Nuske, H. J., Vivanti, G., and Dissanayake, C. (2013). Are emotion impairments unique to, universal, or specific in autism spectrum disorder? A comprehensive review. Cognition and Emotion, 27(6), 10421061.Google Scholar
Nuske, H. J., Vivanti, G., Hudry, K., and Dissanayake, C. (2014). Pupillometry reveals reduced unconscious emotional reactivity in autism. Biological Psychology, 101, 2435.Google Scholar
Oliver, A., Johnson, M. H., Karmiloff-Smith, A., and Pennington, B. F. (2000). Deviations in the emergence of representations: A neuroconstructivist framework for analyzing developmental disorders. Developmental Science, 3, 140.Google Scholar
Ozonoff, S., Strayer, D. L., McMahon, W. M., and Filloux, F. (1994). Executive function abilities in autism: An information processing approach. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 10151031.Google Scholar
Paul, R., Augustyn, A., Klin, A., and Volkmar, F. R. (2005). Perception and production of prosody by speakers with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(2), 205220.Google Scholar
Pellicano, E., Kenny, L., Brede, J., et al. (2017). Executive function predicts school readiness in autistic and typical preschool children. Cognitive Development, 43, 113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.02.003Google Scholar
Pellicano, E., Kenny, L., Brede, J., et al. (2017). Executive function predicts school readiness in autistic and typical preschool children. Cognitive Development, 43, 113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.02.003Google Scholar
Pellicano, E. (2010). Individual differences in executive function and central coherence predict developmental changes in theory of mind in autism. Developmental Psychology, 46(2), 530544. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018287Google Scholar
Pellicano, E. (2007). Links between theory of mind and executive function in young children with autism: Clues to developmental primacy. Developmental Psychology, 43(4), 974990. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.4.974Google Scholar
Pennington, B. F. (2014). Explaining Abnormal Behavior: A Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Perner, J. and Lang, B. (1999). Development of theory of mind and executive control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(9), 337344.Google Scholar
Pickles, A., Le Couteur, A., Leadbitter, K., et al. (2016). Parent-mediated social communication therapy for young children with autism (PACT): Long-term follow-up of a randomised controlled trial. The Lancet, 388(10059), 25012509.Google Scholar
Pieron, M., Seassau, M., Leboyer, M., and Zalla, T. (2015). Accelerated time course of saccadic inhibition of return in individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Experimental Brain Research, 233(3), 767775.Google Scholar
Puce, A. and Bertenthal, B. I. (2015). The Many Faces of Social Attention: Behavioral and Neural Measures. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Petersen, S. E. and Posner, M. I. (2012). The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35(1), 7389. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150525Google Scholar
Posner, M. I. and Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 2542.Google Scholar
Pugliese, C. E., Anthony, L. G., Strang, J. F., et al. (2016). Longitudinal examination of adaptive behavior in autism spectrum disorders: influence of executive function. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(2), 467477. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2584-5Google Scholar
Pugliese, C. E., Anthony, L., Strang, J. F., et al. (2015). Increasing adaptive behavior skill deficits from childhood to adolescence in autism spectrum disorder: Role of executive function. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(6), 15791587. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-014-2309-1Google Scholar
Rapin, I. (2002). Diagnostic dilemmas in developmental disabilities: fuzzy margins at the edges of normality. An essay prompted by Thomas Sowell's new book: The Einstein Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(1), 4957.Google Scholar
Renner, P., Klinger, L. G., and Klinger, M. R. (2000). Implicit and explicit memory in autism: Is autism an amnesic disorder? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(1), 314. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005487009889Google Scholar
Rimland, B. (1978). Savant capabilities of autistic children and their cognitive implications. In Serban, G. (Ed.), Cognitive Defects in the Development of Mental Illness (pp. 4365). New York: Brunner-Mazel.Google Scholar
Rinehart, N. J., Bradshaw, J. L., Moss, S. A., Brereton, A. V., and Tonge, B. J. (2008). Brief report: Inhibition of return in young people with autism and Asperger's disorder. Autism, 12(3), 249260. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361307088754Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., Fabbri-Destro, M., and Cattaneo, L. (2009). Mirror neurons and their clinical relevance. Nature Reviews. Neurology, 5(1), 24.Google Scholar
Rogers, S. J., Dawson, G., and Vismara, L. A. (2012). An early start for your child with autism: Using everyday activities to help kids connect, communicate, and learn. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Rogers, S. J., Vismara, L., Wagner, A., et al. (2014). Autism treatment in the first year of life: a pilot study of infant start, a parent-implemented intervention for symptomatic infants. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(12), 29812995.Google Scholar
Rogers, S. J. and Williams, J. H. (2006). Imitation and the Social Mind: Autism and Typical Development. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Rogers, S. J., Young, G. S., Cook, I., et al. (2010). Imitating actions on objects in early-onset and regressive autism: Effects and implications of task characteristics on performance. Development and Psychopathology, 22(1), 7185.Google Scholar
Rogers, S. J., Ozonoff, S., and Maslin-Cole, C. (1991). A comparative study of attachment behavior in young children with autism or other psychiatric disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 30(3), 483488.Google Scholar
Rosenblau, G., Kliemann, D., Dziobek, I., and Heekeren, H. R. (2017). Emotional prosody processing in autism spectrum disorder. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(2), 224239.Google Scholar
Rosenthal, M., Wallace, G. L., Lawson, R., et al. (2013). Impairments in real-world executive function increase from childhood to adolescence in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychology, 27(1), 1318.Google Scholar
Rowberry, J., Macari, S., Chen, G., et al. (2015). Screening for autism spectrum disorders in 12-month-old high-risk siblings by parental report. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(1), 221229.Google Scholar
Rozga, A., Hutman, T., Young, G. S., et al. (2011). Behavioral profiles of affected and unaffected siblings of children with autism: Contribution of measures of mother–infant interaction and nonverbal communication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(3), 287301.Google Scholar
Rump, K. M., Giovannelli, J. L., Minshew, N. J., and Strauss, M. S. (2009). The development of emotion recognition in individuals with autism. Child Development, 80(5), 14341447.Google Scholar
Rutgers, A. H., Bakermans‐Kranenburg, M. J., Ijzendoorn, M. H., and Berckelaer‐Onnes, I. A. (2004). Autism and attachment: a meta‐analytic review. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(6), 11231134.Google Scholar
Rutter, M. (1983). Cognitive deficits in the pathogenesis of autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 24(4), 513531.Google Scholar
Ruysschaert, L., Warreyn, P., Wiersema, J. R., et al. (2014). Exploring the role of neural mirroring in children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 7(2), 197206.Google Scholar
Salomone, E., Shephard, E., Milosavljevic, B. et al. (2018). Adaptive Behaviour and Cognitive Skills: Stability and Change from 7 Months to 7 Years in Siblings at High Familial Risk of Autism Spectrum Disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48(9), 29012911.Google Scholar
Salmond, C. H., Ashburner, J., Connelly, A., et al. (2005). The role of the medial temporal lobe in autistic spectrum disorders. European Journal of Neuroscience, 22(3), 764772. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04217.xGoogle Scholar
Samson, A. C., Huber, O., and Gross, J. J. (2012). Emotion regulation in Asperger's syndrome and high-functioning autism. Emotion, 12(4), 659.Google Scholar
Samyn, V., Roeyers, H., Bijttebier, P., and Wiersema, J. R. (2017). Attentional networks in boys with ADHD or autism spectrum disorder and the relationship with effortful control. Journal of Attention Disorders, 21(3), 228239. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054712473183Google Scholar
Sandbank, M., Woynaroski, T., Watson, L. R., et al. (2017). Predicting intentional communication in preverbal preschoolers with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47(6), 15811594.Google Scholar
Scaife, M. and Bruner, J. S. (1975). The capacity for joint visual attention in the infant. Nature, 253(5489), 265266.Google Scholar
Schwenck, C., Mergenthaler, J., Keller, K., et al. (2012). Empathy in children with autism and conduct disorder: Group‐specific profiles and developmental aspects. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(6), 651659.Google Scholar
Senju, A. (2012). Spontaneous theory of mind and its absence in autism spectrum disorders. The Neuroscientist, 18(2), 108113.Google Scholar
Senju, A. and Johnson, M. H. (2009). Atypical eye contact in autism: models, mechanisms and development. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 33(8), 12041214.Google Scholar
Sigman, M. (1998). Change and continuity in the development of children with autism. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 39(6), 817827.Google Scholar
Sigman, M. and Ungerer, J. A. (1984). Cognitive and language skills in autistic, mentally retarded, and normal children. Developmental Psychology, 20(2), 293.Google Scholar
Sigman, M. and Mundy, P. (1989). Social attachments in autistic children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28(1), 7481.Google Scholar
Simpson, E. A., Murray, L., Paukner, A., and Ferrari, P. F. (2014). The mirror neuron system as revealed through neonatal imitation: presence from birth, predictive power and evidence of plasticity. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 369(1644), 20130289.Google Scholar
Smith, B. J., Gardiner, J. M., and Bowler, D. M. (2007). Deficits in free recall persist in Asperger's syndrome despite training in the use of list-appropriate learning strategies. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(3), 445454.Google Scholar
Smith, T., Klorman, R., and Mruzek, D. W. (2015). Predicting outcome of community-based early intensive behavioral intervention for children with autism. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43(7), 12711282.Google Scholar
Sperry, L. A. and Mesibov, G. B. (2005). Perceptions of social challenges of adults with autism spectrum disorder. Autism, 9(4), 362376.Google Scholar
Stahl, L. and Pry, R. (2002). Joint attention and set-shifting in young children with autism. Autism, 6(4), 383396.Google Scholar
Steele, S. D., Minshew, N. J., Luna, B., and Sweeney, J. A. (2007). Spatial working memory deficits in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(4), 605612. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0202-2Google Scholar
Szatmari, P., Georgiades, S., Duku, E., et al. (2015). Developmental trajectories of symptom severity and adaptive functioning in an inception cohort of preschool children with autism spectrum disorder, JAMA psychiatry, 3, 276283.Google Scholar
Tager‐Flusberg, H. (1992). Autistic children's talk about psychological states: Deficits in the early acquisition of a theory of mind. Child Development, 63(1), 161172.Google Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H. and Dawson, G. (1991). Semantic processing in the free recall of autistic children: Further evidence of a cognitive deficit. In Autism: Nature, diagnosis, and treatment (pp. 92109). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Tillmann, J., Ashwood, K., Absoud, M., S. et al. (2018). Evaluating Sex and Age Differences in ADI-R and ADOS Scores in a Large European Multi-site Sample of Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 48(7), 24902505.Google Scholar
Travers, B. G., Kana, R. K., Klinger, L. G., Klein, C. L., and Klinger, M. R. (2015). Motor learning in individuals with autism spectrum disorder: Activation in superior parietal lobule related to learning and repetitive behaviors. Autism Research, 8(1), 3851. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1403Google Scholar
Travers, B. G., Klinger, M. R., Mussey, J. L., and Klinger, L. G. (2010). Motor-linked implicit learning in persons with autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research, 3(2), 6877. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.123Google Scholar
Uzgiris, I. C. (1981). Two functions of imitation during infancy. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 4(1), 112.Google Scholar
Uljarevic, M. and Vivanti, G. (in press). Challenges to the social motivation theory of Autism: The dangers of counteracting an imprecise theory with even more imprecision. Behavioral and Brain Sciences.Google Scholar
Uljarevic, M. and Hamilton, A. (2013). Recognition of emotions in autism: a formal meta-analysis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(7), 15711526.Google Scholar
Uzefovsky, F., Allison, C., Smith, P., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2016). Brief Report: The Go/No-Go Task Online: Inhibitory Control Deficits in Autism in a Large Sample. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(8), 27742779.Google Scholar
Wan, M. W., Green, J., Elsabbagh, M., et al. (2013). Quality of interaction between at‐risk infants and caregiver at 12–15 months is associated with 3‐year autism outcome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(7), 763771.Google Scholar
Wang, Y. and Hamilton, A. F. D. C. (2012). Social top-down response modulation (STORM): A model of the control of mimicry in social interaction. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6.Google Scholar
Wainwright-Sharp, J. A. and Bryson, S. E. (1993). Visual orienting deficits in high-functioning people with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23(1), 113.Google Scholar
Wallace, G. L., Kenworthy, L., Pugliese, C. E., et al. (2016). Real-world executive functions in adults with autism spectrum disorder: Profiles of impairment and associations with adaptive functioning and co-morbid anxiety and depression. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(3), 10711083. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2655-7Google Scholar
Werling, D. M. and Geschwind, D. H. (2013). Sex differences in autism spectrum disorders, Current Opinion in Neurology, 2, 146153.Google Scholar
Williams, D. L., Goldstein, G., and Minshew, N. J. (2005). Impaired memory for faces and social scenes in autism: Clinical implications of memory dysfunction. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 20(1), 115. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2002.08.001Google Scholar
Williams, D. L., Goldstein, G., and Minshew, N. J. (2006). The profile of memory function in children with autism. Neuropsychology, 20(1), 2129. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.20.1.21Google Scholar
Williams, D. and Happé, F. (2010). Representing intentions in self and other: Studies of autism and typical development. Developmental Science, 13(2), 307319.Google Scholar
Van Etten, H. M. and Carver, L. J. (2015). Does impaired social motivation drive imitation deficits in children with autism spectrum disorder? Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2(3), 310319.Google Scholar
Van Eylen, L., Boets, B., Steyaert, J., et al. (2011). Cognitive flexibility in autism spectrum disorder: Explaining the inconsistencies? Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5(4), 13901401.Google Scholar
Van Hecke, A. V., Mundy, P., Block, J. J., Delgado, C. E., Parlade, M. V., Pomares, Y. B., and Hobson, J. A. (2012). Infant responding to joint attention, executive processes, and self-regulation in preschool children. Infant Behavior and Development, 35(2), 303311.Google Scholar
Vanvuchelen, M., Roeyers, H., and De Weerdt, W. (2007). Nature of motor imitation problems in school-aged boys with autism: A motor or a cognitive problem? Autism, 11(3), 225240.Google Scholar
Van Etten, H. M. and Carver, L. J. (2015). Does impaired social motivation drive imitation deficits in children with autism spectrum disorder? Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2(3), 310319.Google Scholar
Vivanti, G., Hocking, D. R., Fanning, P., and Dissanayake, C. (2017). The social nature of overimitation: Insights from Autism and Williams syndrome. Cognition, 161, 1018.Google Scholar
Vivanti, G., Kasari, C., Green, J., et al. (2018). Implementing and evaluating early intervention for children with autism: Where are the gaps and what should we do?. Autism Research, 11(1), 1623.Google Scholar
Vivanti, G. and Nuske, H. J. (2017). Autism, attachment, and social learning: Three challenges and a way forward. Behavioural Brain Research, 325, 251259.Google Scholar
Vivanti, G., Fanning, P., Hocking, D.R., et al. (2017a). Social attention, joint attention and sustained attention in Autism Spectrum Disorder and Williams syndrome: Convergences and divergences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, doi: 10.1007/s10803-017-3107-4Google Scholar
Vivanti, G., Dawson, G., and Rogers, S. J. (2017b). Early Learning in Autism. In Implementing the Group-Based Early Start Denver Model for Preschoolers with Autism (pp. 112). Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing.Google Scholar
Vivanti, G., Hocking, D. R., Fanning, P., and Dissanayake, C. (2016). Social affiliation motives modulate spontaneous learning in Williams syndrome but not in autism. Molecular Autism, 7(1), 40.Google Scholar
Vivanti, G. and Dissanayake, C. (2014). Propensity to imitate in autism is not modulated by the model's gaze direction: An eye‐tracking study. Autism Research, 7(3), 392399.Google Scholar
Vivanti, G. and Rogers, S. J. (2014). Autism and the mirror neuron system: Insights from learning and teaching. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 369(1644), 2013018420130184.Google Scholar
Vivanti, G. and Hamilton, A. F. (2014). Imitation in autism spectrum disorders. In: Volkmar, F., Paul, R., Rogers, S., Pelphrey, K. (Eds), The Handbook of Autism and Developmental Disorders. New York: Wiley, pp. 278301.Google Scholar
Vivanti, G., Dissanayake, C., Zierhut, C., Rogers, S. J., and Victorian ASELCC Team. (2013). Brief report: Predictors of outcomes in the early start Denver model delivered in a group setting. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(7), 17171724.Google Scholar
Vivanti, G. and Hamilton, A. (2014). Imitation in autism spectrum disorders. In Volkmar, F. R., Paul, R., Rogers, S. J., & Pelphrey, K. A. (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (Vol. 1, pp. 278301). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Vivanti, G., McCormick, C., Young, G. S., et al. (2011). Intact and impaired mechanisms of action understanding in autism. Developmental Psychology, 47(3), 841.Google Scholar
Volkmar, F. R., Lord, C., Bailey, A., Schultz, R. T., and Klin, A. (2004). Autism and pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 1, 135170.Google Scholar
Yang, J. and Hofmann, J. (2016). Action observation and imitation in autism spectrum disorders: An ALE meta-analysis of fMRI studies. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 10(4), 960969.Google Scholar
Yerys, B. E., Hepburn, S. L., Pennington, B. F., and Rogers, S. J. (2007). Executive function in preschoolers with autism: evidence consistent with a secondary deficit. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(6), 10681079. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0250-7Google Scholar
Yerys, B. E., Kenworthy, L., Jankowski, K. F., Strang, J., & Wallace, G. L. (2013). Separate components of emotional go/no-go performance relate to autism versus attention symptoms in children with autism. Neuropsychology, 27(5), 537.Google Scholar
Yerys, B. E., Wallace, G. L., Jankowski, K. F., Bollich, A., & Kenworthy, L. (2011). Impaired Consonant Trigrams Test (CTT) performance relates to everyday working memory difficulties in children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Child Neuropsychology, 17(4), 391399.Google Scholar
Yerys, B. E., Wallace, G. L., Harrison, B., et al. (2009). Set-shifting in children with autism spectrum disorders: Reversal shifting deficits on the Intradimensional/Extradimensional Shift Test correlate with repetitive behaviors. Autism, 13(5), 523538. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361309335716Google Scholar
Yerys, B. E., Wallace, G. L., Sokoloff, J. L., et al. (2009). Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms moderate cognition and behavior in children with autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research, 2(6), 322333. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.103Google Scholar
Yerys, B. E., Wolff, B. C., Moody, E., Pennington, B. F., & Hepburn, S. L. (2012). Brief Report: Impaired Flexible Item Selection Task (FIST) in School-Age Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(9), 20132020.Google Scholar
Yoder, K. J. & Belmonte, M. K. (2011). Information processing and integration. In: Amaral, D. G., Dawson, G. and Geschwind, D. H. (eds), Autism Spectrum Disorders. New York: Oxford University Press, 10101027.Google Scholar
Yoder, P. J., Watson, L., and Lambert, W. (2015). Value-added predictors of expressive and receptive language growth in initially nonverbal preschoolers with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(5), 12541270.Google Scholar
Zantinge, G., van Rijn, S., Stockmann, L., and Swaab, H. (2017). Psychophysiological responses to emotions of others in young children with autism spectrum disorders: Correlates of social functioning. Autism Research, 10(9), 14991509.Google Scholar
Zelazo, P. D., Carlson, S. M., and Kesek, A. (2008). Development of executive function in childhood. In Nelson, C. A. and Luciana, M. (Eds), Handbook of Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience (2nd Edn). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×