Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-7drxs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-16T09:31:00.775Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

5 - Earwitness evidence: Memory for a perpetrator's voice

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 August 2010

David Frank Ross
Affiliation:
Boise State University, Idaho
J. Don Read
Affiliation:
University of Lethbridge, Alberta
Michael P. Toglia
Affiliation:
State University of New York
Get access

Summary

Earwitness testimony, in contrast to eyewitness testimony, has not received a great amount of attention from either psycholegal researchers or the courts, possibly because of the greater reliance on information processed visually rather than orally. Some crimes may include both visual and auditory information, or may only be seen but not heard. For some crimes, however, such as those committed in darkness, or with perpetrators wearing masks, or those committed over the telephone, the sole source of identification evidence may be auditory.

Experimental research on earwitness testimony has been pursued by researchers in North America, the United Kingdom, and Europe (see reviews by Bull & Clifford, 1984; Clifford, 1983; Deffenbacher, Cross, Handkins, Chance, Goldstein, Hammersley, & Read, 1989; Hammersley & Read, in press; Kunzel, 1990). This research has had only limited impact on police practices and on the courts (see Yarmey, 1990), however, and has not been widely reported in the mass media. For example, Kleiman (1988) reported in the New York Times that prosecutors in a Riverhead, Long Island, trial believed their use of a voice lineup as the primary method to identify an accused serial rapist was a ground breaking procedure in criminal proceedings. English courts have accepted voice identification evidence from lay witnesses at least since 1660 (Hollien, Bennett, & Gelfer, 1983). If the Times article is representative of the general knowledge of officers of the court, the construction of voice lineups and judgments about the influence of selected estimator and structural factors on earwitness identifications are based on intuition or common knowledge rather than on scientific findings and theory.

Type
Chapter
Information
Adult Eyewitness Testimony
Current Trends and Developments
, pp. 101 - 124
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 1994

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×