Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-vsgnj Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-19T12:19:25.454Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

13 - Tool use, hand cooperation and the development of object manipulation in human and non-human primates

from SECTION IV - ACQUISITION OF SKILLS

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 May 2010

Alex Fedde Kalverboer
Affiliation:
Rijksuniversiteit Groningen, The Netherlands
Brian Hopkins
Affiliation:
Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam
Reint Geuze
Affiliation:
Rijksuniversiteit Groningen, The Netherlands
Get access

Summary

INTRODUCTION

One of the distinguishing characteristics of human intelligence is the use of external objects to facilitate goal-directed activities. Without the use of external objects as tools, most of the artifices that make up human culture would not be possible. Due to its crucial role in human evolution, tool use was thought for a long time to be an exclusive ability of humans. However, since the experiments of Koehler (1925) and the observations of Goodall (1968) on chimpanzees, we know that this species (among other non-human primates) is able to use objects to attain different goals, as exemplified in the use of sticks to ‘fish’ for termites and ants.

In reality, the evidence for tool use, defined in its broadest sense, is not limited to primates. Thus, a variety of birds, lower mammals and even some fish incorporate external objects into their activities (Beck, 1980). For example, seagulls drop clams onto rocks in order to break them. If we want to confer special status to primate tool use, it will be necessary to distinguish it from other cases of tool use in the animal kingdom. For that purpose, a taxonomy for distinguishing various forms of object manipulation and tool use has been proposed by Parker & Gibson (1977). Their classification rests on the definition of complex object manipulation considered as ‘the manipulation of one detached object relative to another involving subsequent change of state of one or both of the objects’ (Parker & Gibson, 1977, p. 624).

Type
Chapter
Information
Motor Development in Early and Later Childhood
Longitudinal Approaches
, pp. 205 - 216
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 1993

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×