Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-pfhbr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-13T15:56:46.335Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

47 - Long-term impact of neonatal events on speech, language development, and academic achievement

from Section 6 - Assessing outcome of the brain-injured infant

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 January 2010

David K. Stevenson
Affiliation:
Stanford University School of Medicine, California
William E. Benitz
Affiliation:
Stanford University School of Medicine, California
Philip Sunshine
Affiliation:
Stanford University School of Medicine, California
Susan R. Hintz
Affiliation:
Stanford University School of Medicine, California
Maurice L. Druzin
Affiliation:
Stanford University School of Medicine, California
Get access

Summary

Introduction

Language is the medium by which people exchange greetings, requests, thoughts, information, and emotions. As such, language and speech are the foundation of human communication, social interaction, and learning. Children with delays in early language and speech development are at high risk for later disorders in reading, spelling, and writing, academic skills which are highly dependent on language abilities. They are also at risk for general academic underachievement, behavioral disorders, and poor social skills. It is highly important to understand the impact of prematurity, low birthweight (LBW), and associated adverse neonatal events on the early development of language and speech, and also on reading, spelling, writing, and academic achievement.

This chapter begins with a brief summary of developmental milestones in language, speech, and reading as a background to the specific topic. Methods of assessment are described to facilitate understanding of studies to be discussed. The chapter then describes the equivocal results of studies of early language and speech development in children born prematurely. We will demonstrate that delays in language development are usually components of a broader disorder of cognition, sensory abilities, and/or motor skills. The chapter then considers the development of reading, spelling, and writing as language-based skills, as well as overall academic achievement, in children born prematurely. We will demonstrate that problems in reading are also often a component of broader disorders, including cognitive impairments and deficits in executive function. We consider outcomes as a function of potential moderators, including age, gender, and socioeconomic status (SES).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Feldman, HM. Evaluation and management of language and speech disorders in preschool children. Pediatr Rev 2005; 26: 131–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rayner, K, Foorman, BR, Perfetti, CA, et al. How psychological science informs the teaching of reading. Psychol Sci 2001; 2: 31–74.Google ScholarPubMed
Wagner, RK, Torgesen, JK. The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychol Bull 1987; 101: 192–212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feldman, HM, Dollaghan, CA, Campbell, TF, et al. Measurement properties of the MacArthur communicative development inventories at ages one and two years. Child Dev 2000; 71: 310–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Law, J, Boyle, J, Harris, F, et al. Prevalence and natural history of primary speech and language delay: findings from a systematic review of the literature. Int J Lang Commun Disord 2000; 35: 165–88.Google ScholarPubMed
Scarborough, HS, ed. Developmental Relationships Between Language and Reading: Reconciling a Beautiful Hypothesis with Some Ugly Facts. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 2005.Google Scholar
Briscoe, J, Bishop, DV, Norbury, CF, et al. Phonological processing, language, and literacy: a comparison of children with mild-to-moderate sensorineural hearing loss and those with specific language impairment. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2001; 42: 329–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reilly, S, Eadie, P, Bavin, EL, et al. Growth of infant communication between 8 and 12 months: a population study. J Paediatr Child Health 2006; 42: 764–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eilers, RE, Oller, D, Levine, S, et al. The role of prematurity and socioeconomic status in the onset of canonical babbling in infants. Infant Behav Dev 1993; 16: 297–315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stolt, S, Klippi, A, Launonen, K, et al. Size and composition of the lexicon in prematurely born very-low-birth-weight and full-term Finnish children at two years of age. J Child Lang 2007; 34: 283–310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menyuk, P, Liebergott, J, Schultz, M. Early Language Development in Full-term and Premature Infants. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1995.Google Scholar
Foster-Cohen, S, Edgin, JO, Champion, PR, et al. Early delayed language development in very preterm infants: evidence from the MacArthur-Bates CDI. J Child Lang 2007; 34: 655–75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vohr, BR, Garcia-Coll, C, Oh, W. Language and neurodevelopmental outcome of low-birthweight infants at three years. Dev Med Child Neurol 1989; 31: 582–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kilbride, HW, Thorstad, K, Daily, DK. Preschool outcome of less than 801-gram preterm infants compared with full-term siblings. Pediatrics 2004; 113: 742–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keegstra, AL, Knijff, WA, Post, WJ, et al. Children with language problems in a speech and hearing clinic: background variables and extent of language problems. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol 2007; 71: 815–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vohr, B, Allan, WC, Scott, DT, et al. Early-onset intraventricular hemorrhage in preterm neonates: incidence of neurodevelopmental handicap. Semin Perinatol 1999; 23: 212–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hemphill, L, Feldman, HM, Camp, L, et al. Developmental changes in narrative and non-narrative discourse in children with and without brain injury. J Commun Disord 1994; 27: 107–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feldman, HM, Evans, JL, Brown, RE, et al. Early language and communicative abilities of children with periventricular leukomalacia. Am J Ment Retard 1992; 97: 222–34.Google ScholarPubMed
Ment, LR, Vohr, B, Allan, W, et al. Outcome of children in the indomethacin intraventricular hemorrhage prevention trial. Pediatrics 2000; 105: 485–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ment, LR, Vohr, BR, Makuch, RW, et al. Prevention of intraventricular hemorrhage by indomethacin in male preterm infants. J Pediatr 2004; 145: 832–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Downie, AL, Frisk, V, Jakobson, LS. The impact of periventricular brain injury on reading and spelling abilities in the late elementary and adolescent years. Child Neuropsychol 2005; 11: 479–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aylward, GP. Neurodevelopmental outcomes of infants born prematurely. J Dev Behav Pediatr 2005; 26: 427–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aylward, GP. Cognitive and neuropsychological outcomes: more than IQ scores. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev 2002; 8: 234–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolke, D, Meyer, R. Cognitive status, language attainment, and prereading skills of 6-year-old very preterm children and their peers: the Bavarian Longitudinal Study. Dev Med Child Neurol 1999; 41: 94–109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneider, W, Wolke, D, Schlagmuller, M, et al. Pathways to school achievement in very preterm and full term children. Eur J Psychol Educ 2004; 19: 385–406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vohr, BR, Allan, WC, Westerveld, M, et al. School-age outcomes of very low birth weight infants in the indomethacin intraventricular hemorrhage prevention trial. Pediatrics 2003; 111: e340–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stjernqvist, K, Svenningsen, NW, Stjernqvist, K, et al. Ten-year follow-up of children born before 29 gestational weeks: health, cognitive development, behaviour and school achievement. Acta Paediatr 1999; 88: 557–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saigal, S, Ouden, L, Wolke, D, et al. School-age outcomes in children who were extremely low birth weight from four international population-based cohorts. Pediatrics 2003; 112: 943–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hack, M, Flannery, DJ, Schluchter, M, et al. Outcomes in young adulthood for very-low-birth-weight infants. N Eng J Med 2002; 346: 149–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saigal, S, Stoskopf, B, Streiner, D, et al. Transition of extremely low-birth-weight infants from adolescence to young adulthood: comparison with normal birth-weight controls. JAMA 2006; 295: 667–75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ment, LR, Vohr, B, Allan, W, et al. Change in cognitive function over time in very low-birth-weight infants. JAMA 2003; 289: 705–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hintz, SR, Kendrick, , Vohr, BR, et al. Gender differences in neurodevelopmental outcomes among extremely preterm, extremely-low-birthweight infants. Acta Paediatr 2006; 95: 1239–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reiss, AL, Kesler, SR, Vohr, B, et al. Sex differences in cerebral volumes of 8-year-olds born preterm. J Pediatr 2004; 145: 242–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ment, LR, Vohr, B, Allan, W, et al. The etiology and outcome of cerebral ventriculomegaly at term in very low birth weight preterm infants. Pediatrics 1999; 104: 243–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landry, SH, Smith, KE, Swank, PR. Environmental effects on language development in normal and high-risk child populations. Semin Pediatr Neurol 2002; 9: 192–200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landry, SH, Smith, KE, Swank, PR. Responsive parenting: establishing early foundations for social, communication, and independent problem-solving skills. Dev Psychol 2006; 42: 627–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caravale, B, Tozzi, C, Albino, G, et al. Cognitive development in low risk preterm infants at 3–4 years of life. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 2005; 90: F474–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mikkola, K, Ritari, N, Tommiska, V, et al. Neurodevelopmental outcome at 5 years of age of a national cohort of extremely low birth weight infants who were born in 1996–1997. Pediatrics 2005; 116: 1391–400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aram, DM, Hack, M, Hawkins, S, et al. Very-low-birthweight children and speech and language development. J Speech Hear Res 1991; 34: 1169–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, PJ, Doyle, LW, Victorian Infant Collaborative Study Group. Executive functioning in school-aged children who were born very preterm or with extremely low birth weight in the 1990s. Pediatrics 2004; 114: 50–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, HG, Klein, N, Drotar, D, et al. Consequences and risks of < 1000-g birth weight for neuropsychological skills, achievement, and adaptive functioning. J Dev Behav Pediatr 2006; 27: 459–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luciana, M, Lindeke, L, Georgieff, M, et al. Neurobehavioral evidence for working-memory deficits in school-aged children with histories of prematurity. Dev Med Child Neurol 1999; 41: 521–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, HG, Minich, N, Bangert, B, et al. Long-term neuropsychological outcomes of very low birth weight: associations with early risks for periventricular brain insults. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2004; 10: 987–1004.Google ScholarPubMed
Taylor, HG, Minich, NM, Klein, N, et al. Longitudinal outcomes of very low birth weight: neuropsychological findings. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2004; 10: 149–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, RT, Freeman, WS, Perrin, JM, et al. Prevalence and assessment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in primary care settings. Pediatrics 2001; 107: E43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mick, E, Biederman, J, Prince, J, et al. Impact of low birth weight on attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Dev Behav Pediatr 2002; 23: 16–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breslau, N, Brown, GG, DelDotto, JE, et al. Psychiatric sequelae of low birth weight at 6 years of age. J Abnorm Child Psychol 1996; 24: 385–400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breslau, N, Chilcoat, HD. Psychiatric sequelae of low birth weight at 11 years of age. Biol Psychiatry 2000; 47: 1005–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Botting, N, Powls, A, Cooke, RW, et al. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorders and other psychiatric outcomes in very low birthweight children at 12 years. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1997; 38: 931–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Indredavik, MS, Vik, T, Heyerdahl, S, et al. Psychiatric symptoms and disorders in adolescents with low birth weight. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 2004; 89: F445–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, H, Hack, M, Klein, NK. Attention deficits in children with < 750 gm birth weight. Child Neuropsychol 1998; 4: 21–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elgen, I, Lundervold, AJ, Sommerfelt, K, et al. Aspects of inattention in low birth weight children. Pediatr Neurol 2004; 30: 92–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, DW, Burns, B, Snyder, E, et al. Attention problems in very low birth weight preschoolers: are new screening measures needed for this special population?J Child Adolesc Psychiatr Nurs 2007; 20: 74–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Szatmari, P, Saigal, S, Rosenbaum, P, et al. Psychiatric disorders at five years among children with birthweights less than 1000 g: a regional perspective. Dev Med Child Neurol 1990; 32: 954–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hack, M, Youngstrom, EA, Cartar, L, et al. Behavioral outcomes and evidence of psychopathology among very low birth weight infants at age 20 years. Pediatrics 2004; 114: 932–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weindrich, D, Jennen-Steinmetz, C, Laucht, M, et al. Late sequelae of low birthweight: mediators of poor school performance at 11 years. Dev Med Child Neurol 2003; 45: 463–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, P, Doyle, LW, Callanan, C, et al. Neurobehavioral outcomes of school-age children born extremely low birth weight or very preterm in the 1990s. JAMA 2003; 289: 3264–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Back, SA. Perinatal white matter injury: the changing spectrum of pathology and emerging insights into pathogenetic mechanisms. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev 2006; 12: 129–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peterson, J, Taylor, HG, Minich, N, et al. Subnormal head circumference in very low birth weight children: neonatal correlates and school-age consequences. Early Hum Dev 2006; 82: 325–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hack, M, Breslau, N, Weissman, B, et al. Effect of very low birth weight and subnormal head size on cognitive abilities at school age. N Eng J Med 1991; 325: 231–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kesler, SR, Ment, LR, Vohr, B, et al. Volumetric analysis of regional cerebral development in preterm children. Pediatr Neurol 2004; 31: 318–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kesler, SR, Vohr, B, Schneider, KC, et al. Increased temporal lobe gyrification in preterm children. Neuropsychologia 2006; 44: 445–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feldman, HM, Janosky, JE, Scher, MS, et al. Language abilities following prematurity, periventricular brain injury, and cerebral palsy. J Commun Disord 1994; 27: 71–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, E, Reilly, J, Wulfeck, B, et al. Differential effects of unilateral lesions on language production in children and adults. Brain Lang 2001; 79: 223–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Booth, JR, MacWhinney, B, Thulborn, KR, et al. Developmental and lesion effects in brain activation during sentence comprehension and mental rotation. Dev Neuropsychol 2000; 18: 139–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Booth, JR, Macwhinney, B, Thulborn, KR, et al. Functional organization of activation patterns in children: whole brain fMRI imaging during three different cognitive tasks. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 1999; 23: 669–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Volpe, JJ. Neurology of the Newborn, 4th edn. Philadelphia, PA: Saunders, 2001.Google Scholar
Fanaroff, AA, Stoll, BJ, Wright, LL, et al. Trends in neonatal morbidity and mortality for very low birthweight infants. Am J Obstet Gynecol 2007; 196: 147.e1–8.
Nosarti, C, Rushe, TM, Woodruff, PWR, et al. Corpus callosum size and very preterm birth: relationship to neuropsychological outcome. Brain 2004; 127: 2080–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deutsch, GK, Dougherty, RF, Bammer, R, et al. Children's reading performance is correlated with white matter structure measured by tensor imaging. Cortex 2005; 41: 354–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ben-Shachar, M, Dougherty, RF, Wandell, BA. White matter pathways in reading. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2007; 17: 258–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Limperopoulos, C, Bassan, H, Gauvreau, K, et al. Does cerebellar injury in premature infants contribute to the high prevalence of long-term cognitive, learning, and behavioral disability in survivors?Pediatrics 2007; 120: 584–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leonard, C, Eckert, M, Given, B, et al. Individual differences in anatomy predict reading and oral language impairments in children. Brain 2006; 129: 3329–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ment, LR, Peterson, BS, Vohr, B, et al. Cortical recruitment patterns in children born prematurely compared with control subjects during a passive listening functional magnetic resonance imaging task. J Pediatr 2006; 149: 490–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Law, J, Garrett, Z, Nye, C. Speech and language therapy interventions for children with primary speech and language delay or disorder. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2007; (3): CD004110.Google Scholar
,World Health Organization. The International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health. Geneva: WHO, 2001.

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×