Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-vvkck Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T01:54:39.743Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Is cognition integral to psychopathology? A population-based cohort study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 April 2023

Anat Rotstein*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York, USA Department of Gerontology, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel
Suzanne Fund
Affiliation:
Department of Behavioral Sciences, Israel Defense Forces, Israel
Stephen Z. Levine
Affiliation:
School of Public Health, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel
Abraham Reichenberg
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York, USA Department of Environmental Medicine & Public Health, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York, USA
Judy Goldenberg
Affiliation:
Department of Behavioral Sciences, Israel Defense Forces, Israel
*
Corresponding author: Anat Rotstein, E-mail: anat.rotstein@mssm.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Lower cognitive functioning has been documented across psychiatric disorders and hypothesized to be a core deficit of mental disorders. Situating psychopathology and cognition as part of a unitary construct is therefore important to understanding the etiology of psychiatric disorders. The current study aims to test competing structural models of psychopathology and cognition in a large national cohort of adolescents.

Methods

The analytic sample consisted of 1189 participants aged 16–17 years, screened by the Israeli Draft Board. Psychopathology was assessed using a modified version of the Brief Symptom Inventory, and cognition was assessed based on four standardized test scores ((1) mathematical reasoning, concentration, and concept manipulation; (2) visual-spatial problem-solving skills and nonverbal abstract reasoning; (3) verbal understanding; (4) categorization and verbal abstraction). Confirmatory factor analysis was implemented to compare competing structural models of psychopathology with and without cognition. Sensitivity analyses examined the models in different subpopulations.

Results

Confirmatory factor analysis indicated a better model fit of psychopathological symptoms without cognition (RMSEA = 0.037; TLI = 0.991; CFI = 0.992) than with cognition (RMSEA = 0.04–0.042; TLI = 0.987–0.988; CFI = 0.988–0.989). Sensitivity analyses supported the robustness of these results with a single exception. Among participants with low cognitive abilities (N = 139), models that integrated psychopathological symptoms with cognition had a better fit compared to models of psychopathology without cognition.

Conclusions

The current study suggests that cognition and psychopathology are, generally, independent constructs. However, within low cognitive abilities, cognition was integral to the structure of psychopathology. Our results point toward an increased vulnerability to psychopathology in individuals with low cognitive abilities and may provide valuable information for clinicians.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Converging evidence indicates that psychopathology and cognitive dysfunction often coexist. Indeed, the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5-TR, American Psychiatric Association, 2022) states that psychopathology is characterized by clinically significant disturbances of emotion regulation, behavior, or cognition. Recent psychiatric conceptualizations have suggested that impaired cognitive ability may play a core role in psychopathology (Harvey et al., Reference Harvey, Bosia, Cavallaro, Howes, Kahn, Leucht and Vita2022; Kahn, Reference Kahn2020; Moura et al., Reference Moura, van Rooijen, Schirmbeck, Wigman, Madeira, Harten and Marcelis2021; Tripoli et al., Reference Tripoli, Quattrone, Ferraro, Gayer-Anderson, Rodriguez, La Cascia and Tarricone2021). Large population-based studies have supported this notion showing cognitive impairments across psychiatric disorders, although with different manifestations. For instance, the extent and timing of cognitive deficits in schizophrenia differ between cognitive domains [e.g. processing speed deficits appear early in life, while deficits in working and verbal memory may only appear later (Fett, Reichenberg, & Velthorst, Reference Fett, Reichenberg and Velthorst2022)]. Depressed individuals exhibit impairments in executive function and attention throughout the course of the disorder, including in remission (Rock, Roiser, Riedel, & Blackwell, Reference Rock, Roiser, Riedel and Blackwell2014). Other psychopathological disorders present primarily impairment in domains of executive functioning, such as the executive component of the working memory system in anxiety (Eysenck, Derakshan, Santos, & Calvo, Reference Eysenck, Derakshan, Santos and Calvo2007), executive functioning and verbal memory in post-traumatic stress disorder (Polak, Witteveen, Reitsma, & Olff, Reference Polak, Witteveen, Reitsma and Olff2012), and planning in borderline personality disorder (McClure, Hawes, & Dadds, Reference McClure, Hawes and Dadds2016). The high rates of comorbidity between psychiatric disorders and cognitive impairments suggest that cognition may be integral to psychopathology. It is, however, unclear whether cognitive impairments are themselves part of a pathway to psychopathology, or merely indicate vulnerability (Reichenberg, Reference Reichenberg2005).

Still, contradictory evidence exists and the association between cognition and psychopathology remains a topic of debate. Low levels of intellectual performance are not always observed in psychiatric disorders (MacCabe et al., Reference MacCabe, Lambe, Cnattingius, Sham, David, Reichenberg and Hultman2010, Reference MacCabe, Brébion, Reichenberg, Ganguly, McKenna, Murray and David2012; Oomen et al., Reference Oomen, Begemann, Brand, de Haan, Veling, Koops and Sommer2021; Wraw, Deary, Der, & Gale, Reference Wraw, Deary, Der and Gale2016), and medications that have efficacy in the treatment of psychopathological symptoms, do not affect the associated cognitive impairment (Kahn & Keefe, Reference Kahn and Keefe2013; Keefe et al., Reference Keefe, Buchanan, Marder, Schooler, Dugar, Zivkov and Stewart2013), suggesting independence of psychopathology and cognitive ability. Despite the interest in the structure of psychopathology, the hypothesis that cognition is integral to the structure of psychopathology has not been empirically tested except for the specific case of schizophrenia (Toulopoulou et al., Reference Toulopoulou, Picchioni, Rijsdijk, Hua-Hall, Ettinger, Sham and Murray2007).

The current study examined whether cognition is integral to the structure of psychopathology using a large national population-based cohort of adolescents. Given that adolescence marks the onset of approximately 50% of all lifetime psychiatric disorders (Fusar-Poli et al., Reference Fusar-Poli, Correll, Arango, Berk, Patel and Ioannidis2021; Paus, Keshavan, & Giedd, Reference Paus, Keshavan and Giedd2008), focusing on this population segment is particularly relevant to psychopathology research.

Methods

Study population and procedure

The source population consisted of Israeli adolescents aged 16–17, undergoing mandatory Israeli Draft Board screening (described in detail elsewhere; Weiser et al. Reference Weiser, Reichenberg, Rabinowitz, Knobler, Lubin, Yazvitzky and Davidson2004, Reference Weiser, Reichenberg, Rabinowitz, Nahon, Kravitz, Lubin and Noy2007). The Draft Board screening is applied regardless of whether an individual is eligible for national service or not based on medical, psychiatric, or social grounds. All participants (N = 1374; average age of 16.34; s.d. = 0.48) were assessed on randomly selected days in 2017 (see online Supplementary Fig. 1 for study flow diagram). This study received ethical approval with a waiver of informed consent from the Institutional Review Board at the University of Haifa (Application no. 090/21).

Psychopathological symptoms

Psychopathological symptoms were assessed using a modified version of the Brief Symptom Inventory (Derogatis & Melisaratos, Reference Derogatis and Melisaratos1983; Rotstein, Goldenberg, Fund, Levine, & Reichenberg, Reference Rotstein, Goldenberg, Fund, Levine and Reichenberg2021). The inventory covers nine dimensions of psychopathology: psychoticism, paranoid ideation, depression, anxiety, phobic anxiety, somatization, obsession-compulsion, interpersonal sensitivity, hostility (i.e. conduct disorder), and substance abuse (i.e. alcohol and drug use). The latter was added by the Israeli Draft Board. Symptom ratings characterize the intensity of distress during the past month on a 5-point Likert scale from 0 (Not at all) to 4 (Extremely). The entire inventory took approximately 10–12 min to complete. Previous studies have shown this measure to have sufficient internal reliability (α = 0.95 for the general symptom severity index; average α = 0.71 for all subscales) and convergence validity (r = −0.62 for the general symptom severity index; average r = −0.49 for all subscales; Canetti, Shalev, & De-Nour, Reference Canetti, Shalev and De-Nour1994).

Cognition

Cognitive functioning was assessed using four progressive time-limited tests that measure: (1) Mathematical reasoning, concentration and concept manipulation; (2) Visual-spatial problem-solving skills and nonverbal abstract reasoning; (3) Verbal understanding (based on the ability to comprehend and perform verbal instructions); (4) Categorization and verbal abstraction. Test scores reflect the number of correct answers. The total score of this assessment was found valid when compared to the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale score (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1955; r > 0.90) and associated with external outcomes (i.e. rank upon discharge; r > 0.41; Gal, Reference Gal1986). The assessment is described in detail elsewhere (Reichenberg et al., Reference Reichenberg, Weiser, Rapp, Rabinowitz, Caspi, Schmeidler and Harvey2005).

Analytic sample and data analyses

We excluded 185 individuals (13.5%) due to missing data on psychopathological symptoms and/or cognitive assessment scores (online Supplementary Fig. 1). Descriptive statistics of the association between psychopathological symptoms and cognitive ability were computed and visualized using continuous (total psychopathological symptoms scores; overall cognitive assessment scores) and categorical data (high standardized cognitive assessments scores: >115; low standardized cognitive assessments scores:< 85; average standardized cognitive assessments scores: >85, <115; high psychopathology: top 20% of the sample, based on total psychopathological symptom scores; low psychopathology: bottom 20% of the sample; average psychopathology: participants between the top 20% and bottom 20% of the sample).

The primary analysis consisted of fitting confirmatory factor analytic models to the entire cohort. Namely, three types of standard models of the structure of psychopathology were analyzed based on previous research (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Houts, Belsky, Goldman-Mellor, Harrington, Israel and Poulton2014): A correlated-factor model, a hierarchical model, and a single factor model (see Fig. 1). These models display variations of one general psychopathology dimension (i.e. P) and three higher-order psychopathology factors (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Houts, Belsky, Goldman-Mellor, Harrington, Israel and Poulton2014): an internalizing liability to depression and anxiety; an externalizing liability to antisocial and substance-use disorders; and a thought disorder liability to symptoms of psychosis. The externalizing higher-order psychopathology factor included the following dimensions of psychopathology: substance abuse, conduct disorder (i.e. hostility), and interpersonal sensitivity. The internalizing higher-order psychopathology factor included the following dimensions of psychopathology: depression, anxiety, and phobic anxiety. The thought disorder higher-order psychopathology factor included the following dimensions of psychopathology: obsessive-compulsive, psychoticism, and paranoid ideation. The somatization dimension of psychopathology was not used in the current study because previous evidence indicates it is a separate factor (Marek et al., Reference Marek, Anderson, Tarescavage, Martin-Fernandez, Haugh, Block and Ben-Porath2020).

Figure 1. models of the structure of psychopathology with and without cognition.

Note. Abbreviations. P, A general factor of psychopathology; Ex, Externalizing; In, Internalizing; TD, Thought Disorder; SA, Substance Abuse; CD, Conduct Disorder (i.e. Hostility); IS, Interpersonal Sensitivity; De, Depression; An, Anxiety; PA, Phobic Anxiety; OC, Obsessive-Compulsive, Ps, Psychoticism; PI, Paranoid Ideation; IQ, Intelligence Quotient; VU, Verbal Understanding; VS, Visual-Spatial problem-solving abilities; MR, Mathematical reasoning; Ca, Categorization abilities.

The numbers represent the standardized loadings beta estimates. Green lines represent positive estimates, whereas red lines represent negative estimates. All factor loadings are statistically significant. Due to multicollinearity, the standardized loadings estimates can exceed the bounds of (−1,1) (Deegan, Reference Deegan1978).

All three models were run twice. First, for psychopathological symptoms without cognition, then for psychopathological symptoms with cognition. Psychopathological symptoms were treated as ordered variables, whereas subscales of cognitive ability were treated as continuous. The models were implemented with the Weighted Least Squares Means variance adjusted estimator and the Yuan–Bentler test. These do not assume normally distributed variables and provide the best option for modeling ordered data (Brown, Reference Brown2015). Fit indices of models with and without cognition were compared for each type of model. Specifically, the goodness of fit was determined with the Tucker–Lewis Index (TLI), where values over 0.95 represent good fit, the Comparative Fit Index (CFI), where values over 0.95 represent good fit, and the Root-Mean-Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) where values under 0.05 represent good fit, similar to previous research of psychopathology (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Houts, Belsky, Goldman-Mellor, Harrington, Israel and Poulton2014).

Sensitivity analyses

Sensitivity analyses were implemented to examine the models above in subpopulations with previously shown inconsistent associations between psychopathology and cognition. Specifically, we examined highly symptomatic participants (top 20% of the sample, based on total psychopathological symptom scores) because symptom severity was found related to cognitive performance (for a review, see Russo, Murray, & Reichenberg, Reference Russo, Murray, Reichenberg and Harvey2013). We next focused on participants with low standardized cognitive assessment scores (< 85), because of previously found associations between low cognitive abilities and most psychiatric disorders (Weiser et al., Reference Weiser, Frenkel, Fenchel, Tzur, Sandin, Janecka and Reichenberg2021). We also separately investigated male and female participants due to evidence of disorder-specific, sex-specific cognitive differences (Zanelli et al., Reference Zanelli, Morgan, Dazzan, Morgan, Russo, Pilecka and Jones2013). An additional sensitivity analysis was run on the entire cohort to examine psychopathology models based only on psychotic symptoms, on the grounds that schizophrenia and other psychoses are strongly associated with cognitive impairment (Zanelli et al., Reference Zanelli, Mollon, Sandin, Morgan, Dazzan, Pilecka and Fearon2019). All analyses were conducted in R version 4.1.0.

Results

Sample characteristics

The analytic sample (N = 1189) included 603 males and 586 females and had an average age of 16.32 (s.d. = 0.43) years. Sample characteristics are displayed in online Supplementary sTable 1. In general, higher cognitive ability was associated with lower psychopathology in both continuous (r = −0.23; p < 0.001) and categorical data (χ2 = 39.22; df = 4; p < 0.001). Bivariate Pearson correlations between the study variables are displayed in Fig. 2. Percentages of high, average, and low cognitive ability for distinct psychopathological groups as well as high, average, and low psychopathology for distinct cognitive ability groups are presented in Fig. 3.

Figure 2. Bivariate Pearson correlations between the study variables.

Figure 3. (a)The percentage of high, average, and low cognition for distinct levels of psychopathology. (b) The percentage of high, average, and low psychopathology for distinct levels of cognition.

Is cognition integral to psychopathology?

Comparing the fit of three confirmatory factor analytic models (a correlated-factors model, a hierarchical model, and a single factor model) showed a better fit for psychopathological symptoms without cognition (RMSEA = 0.037; TLI = 0.991; CFI = 0.992), than for psychopathological symptoms with cognition (RMSEA = 0.04–0.042; TLI = 0.987–0.988; CFI = 0.988–0.989). Specifically, the RMSEA was lower, and the TLI and CFI were higher for a correlated-factors model, a hierarchical model, and a single factor model of psychopathology without cognition. Note that these three confirmatory factor analytic models of psychopathology fit our data similarly well, with the single factor model offering a slightly more parsimonious solution. Fit indices are presented in Table 1. Factor loadings are presented in online Supplementary sTables 2–7.

Table 1. Fit indices for models of psychopathology with and without cognition

Note. The goodness of fit was determined by the Tucker–Lewis Index (TLI) where values over 0.95 represent good fit, the Comparative Fit Index (CFI) where values over 0.95 represent good fit, and the Root-Mean-Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), where values under 0.05 represent good fit, similar to previous research of psychopathology (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Houts, Belsky, Goldman-Mellor, Harrington, Israel and Poulton2014). For all models fitted to the entire cohort, RMSEA P-value = 1; For all models fitted to participants with low cognitive abilities, RMSEA P-value = 0.

Sensitivity analyses

Generally, sensitivity analyses of the three models showed a better fit for psychopathological symptoms without cognition compared to the structure where cognition is integral to psychopathology (fit indices are presented in online Supplementary sTable 8). This was the case in highly symptomatic participants; in both males and females; and in psychopathology models based only on psychotic symptoms. In contrast, for participants with low standardized cognitive assessments scores (equivalent to < 85, N = 139), the models of psychopathological symptoms with cognition showed a better fit than the models without cognition (Table 2).

Discussion

Using a large national cohort of adolescents, the current study examined whether cognition is integral to the structure of psychopathology. The results showed that the structure of psychopathology is better outlined independently to cognitive functioning. This result was robust across different models of psychopathology (e.g. with and without a general factor of psychopathology), and in multiple subpopulations.

The current study results are inconsistent with recent conceptualizations of schizophrenia (Harvey et al., Reference Harvey, Bosia, Cavallaro, Howes, Kahn, Leucht and Vita2022; Kahn, Reference Kahn2020; Moura et al., Reference Moura, van Rooijen, Schirmbeck, Wigman, Madeira, Harten and Marcelis2021; Tripoli et al., Reference Tripoli, Quattrone, Ferraro, Gayer-Anderson, Rodriguez, La Cascia and Tarricone2021). Cognitive impairments represent one of the core features of schizophrenia and have been considered of great relevance since the earliest conceptualizations of the disorder (Kahn, Reference Kahn2020). Cognitive impairments are as prevalent as delusions, hallucinations, or thought disorders, and are present even before the development of full-blown psychosis (Davidson, Reference Davidson2019). Impaired cognition is persistent rather than intermittent (Davidson, Reference Davidson2019) and has a substantial negative impact on functional and recovery outcomes (Harvey et al., Reference Harvey, Bosia, Cavallaro, Howes, Kahn, Leucht and Vita2022).

Still, the results are consistent with prior findings of an overall pattern of weak correlations between psychopathology and cognition in the general population (<0.2; Caspi et al. Reference Caspi, Houts, Belsky, Goldman-Mellor, Harrington, Israel and Poulton2014; Southward, Cheavens, & Coccaro, Reference Southward, Cheavens and Coccaro2022). Our results are also consistent with recent research on high-risk samples that showed independence of cognition from psychopathology (Littlefield, Lane, Gette, Watts, & Sher, Reference Littlefield, Lane, Gette, Watts and Sher2021; Southward et al., Reference Southward, Cheavens and Coccaro2022). We extend these studies by using an unselected national sample and testing the structure of psychopathology within relevant sub-groups.

In our primary analysis, as well as in most sensitivity analyses (including models of highly symptomatic participants), the addition of cognition did not improve the fit of the models representing the structure of psychopathology. A single exception was of models fitted to individuals with low cognitive abilities, in which psychopathology and cognition were better represented as a single integrated construct. The integration of psychopathology and cognition suggests that cognition may not only function as a possible marker for early detection and prevention of psychiatric illness (Harvey et al., Reference Harvey, Bosia, Cavallaro, Howes, Kahn, Leucht and Vita2022) but may also contribute substantially to other deficits and poor functional outcomes (Harvey & Strassnig, Reference Harvey and Strassnig2019). Nevertheless, this may also suggest that the observed association between lower cognitive functioning and higher psychopathology across psychiatric disorders is mostly due to an epiphenomenon and/or other shared etiological factors (e.g. Harvey, Koren, Reichenberg, & Bowie, Reference Harvey, Koren, Reichenberg and Bowie2006; Reichenberg, Reference Reichenberg2005).

Limitations

The current study has some limitations. Psychopathology was not defined based on psychiatric diagnoses, unlike some previous studies (e.g. Rock et al., Reference Rock, Roiser, Riedel and Blackwell2014). Future studies may investigate the association between cognition and psychopathology based on medical diagnoses of mental disorders. In our study, psychopathology was measured with the Brief Symptom Inventory (Derogatis & Melisaratos, Reference Derogatis and Melisaratos1983), a screener based on symptoms reported in the last month. While this reporting timeframe may underestimate the prevalence of psychopathology, it may simultaneously limit recall biases. Underestimation or overestimation may also occur due to motivational factors that may affect both psychopathology and cognition, as these were assessed in the context of evaluation and selection. Also, our data is cross-sectional and causal inference is limited. Although most research supports the hypothesis that low cognitive abilities are a risk factor for increased psychopathology, intellectual performance may change post-onset (e.g. patients with psychoses experience cognitive decline after illness onset; Zanelli et al., Reference Zanelli, Mollon, Sandin, Morgan, Dazzan, Pilecka and Fearon2019). Furthermore, cross-sectional data does not account for variations occurring naturally over time in psychopathological measures. Therefore, whether the results would replicate longitudinally or with other measures is unclear. However, our study is based on an inventory that is well validated and has high reliability (Canetti et al., Reference Canetti, Shalev and De-Nour1994), thereby supporting the validity of our results. In terms of generalizability, this study is based on a national sample, yet it focuses on adolescents. While adolescence marks the onset of approximately 50% of all lifetime psychiatric disorders (Fusar-Poli et al., Reference Fusar-Poli, Correll, Arango, Berk, Patel and Ioannidis2021; Paus et al., Reference Paus, Keshavan and Giedd2008), our limited age range may not reflect the age at which different psychiatric disorders typically emerge and the results may vary in other age groups. Finally, our subsample of individuals with low cognitive abilities was small and likely underpowered. Future studies may examine larger samples.

Conclusions and implications

In summary, based on a large national sample, the current study results suggest that in late adolescence, cognition and psychopathology are, generally, independent constructs. However, within low cognitive abilities, integration of psychopathology and cognition was warranted. Therefore, our results point toward an increased vulnerability to psychopathology in individuals with low cognitive abilities. Although our study implies that impaired cognition may provide valuable diagnostic information for clinicians, it remains unclear whether cognitive impairment is part of a causal pathway to psychopathology.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291723000934

Acknowledgments

None.

Financial support

Dr Rotstein received funding from the Israeli National Insurance Institute (grant number 62869) and support from the Zuckerman-CHE Israeli Women Postdoctoral Scholarship.

Role of the funder/sponsor

None.

Competing interests

None.

References

American Psychiatric Association. (2022). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association Publishing.Google Scholar
Brown, T. A. (2015). Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research. New York, NY: Guilford Publications.Google Scholar
Canetti, L., Shalev, A. Y., & De-Nour, A. K. (1994). Israeli Adolescents' norms of the brief symptom inventory (BSI). Israel Journal of Psychiatry and Related Sciences, 31(1), 1318.Google ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A., Houts, R. M., Belsky, D. W., Goldman-Mellor, S. J., Harrington, H., Israel, S., … Poulton, R. (2014). The p factor: One general psychopathology factor in the structure of psychiatric disorders? Clinical Psychological Science, 2(2), 119137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, M. (2019). Cognitive impairment as a diagnostic criterion and treatment target in schizophrenia. World Psychiatry, 18(2), 171172. doi: 10.1002/wps.20651CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deegan, J. Jr (1978). On the occurrence of standardized regression coefficients greater than one. Educational and Psychological Measurement 38, 873888.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Derogatis, L. R., & Melisaratos, N. (1983). The brief symptom inventory: An introductory report. Psychological Medicine, 13(3), 595605.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 7(2), 336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fett, A.-K. J., Reichenberg, A., & Velthorst, E. (2022). Lifespan evolution of neurocognitive impairment in schizophrenia - A narrative review. Schizophrenia Research: Cognition, 28, 100237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scog.2022.100237.Google ScholarPubMed
Fusar-Poli, P., Correll, C. U., Arango, C., Berk, M., Patel, V., & Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2021). Preventive psychiatry: A blueprint for improving the mental health of young people. World Psychiatry, 20(2), 200221. doi: 10.1002/wps.20869CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gal, R. (1986). The selection, classification and placement process. In A portrait of the Israeli Soldier. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, Inc.Google Scholar
Harvey, P. D., Bosia, M., Cavallaro, R., Howes, O. D., Kahn, R. S., Leucht, S., … Vita, A. (2022). Cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia: An expert group paper on the current state of the art. Schizophrenia Research: Cognition, 29, 100249. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scog.2022.100249.Google Scholar
Harvey, P. D., Koren, D., Reichenberg, A., & Bowie, C. R. (2006). Negative symptoms and cognitive deficits: What is the nature of their relationship? Schizophrenia Bulletin, 32(2), 250258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harvey, P. D., & Strassnig, M. T. (2019). Cognition and disability in schizophrenia: Cognition-related skills deficits and decision-making challenges add to morbidity. World Psychiatry, 18(2), 165167. doi: 10.1002/wps.20647CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahn, R. S. (2020). On the origins of schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 177(4), 291297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahn, R. S., & Keefe, R. S. (2013). Schizophrenia is a cognitive illness: Time for a change in focus. JAMA Psychiatry, 70(10), 11071112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keefe, R. S., Buchanan, R. W., Marder, S. R., Schooler, N. R., Dugar, A., Zivkov, M., & Stewart, M. (2013). Clinical trials of potential cognitive-enhancing drugs in schizophrenia: What have we learned so far? Schizophrenia Bulletin, 39(2), 417435.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Littlefield, A. K., Lane, S. P., Gette, J. A., Watts, A. L., & Sher, K. J. (2021). The “Big everything”: Integrating and investigating dimensional models of psychopathology, personality, personality pathology, and cognitive functioning. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 12(2), 103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacCabe, J. H., Brébion, G., Reichenberg, A., Ganguly, T., McKenna, P. J., Murray, R. M., & David, A. S. (2012). Superior intellectual ability in schizophrenia: Neuropsychological characteristics. Neuropsychology, 26(2), 181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacCabe, J. H., Lambe, M. P., Cnattingius, S., Sham, P. C., David, A. S., Reichenberg, A., … Hultman, C. M. (2010). Excellent school performance at age 16 and risk of adult bipolar disorder: National cohort study. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 196(2), 109115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marek, R. J., Anderson, J. L., Tarescavage, A. M., Martin-Fernandez, K., Haugh, S., Block, A. R., … Ben-Porath, Y. S. (2020). Elucidating somatization in a dimensional model of psychopathology across medical settings. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 129(2), 162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClure, G., Hawes, D. J., & Dadds, M. R. (2016). Borderline personality disorder and neuropsychological measures of executive function: A systematic review. Personality and Mental Health, 10(1), 4357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moura, B. M., van Rooijen, G., Schirmbeck, F., Wigman, H., Madeira, L., Harten, P. V., … Marcelis, M. (2021). A network of psychopathological, cognitive, and motor symptoms in schizophrenia Spectrum disorders. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 47(4), 915926. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbab002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oomen, P., Begemann, M., Brand, B., de Haan, L., Veling, W., Koops, S., … Sommer, I. (2021). Longitudinal clinical and functional outcome in distinct cognitive subgroups of first-episode psychosis: A cluster analysis. Psychological Medicine, 111. doi: 10.1017/S0033291721004153Google ScholarPubMed
Paus, T., Keshavan, M., & Giedd, J. N. (2008). Why do many psychiatric disorders emerge during adolescence? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(12), 947957. doi: 10.1038/nrn2513CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Polak, A. R., Witteveen, A. B., Reitsma, J. B., & Olff, M. (2012). The role of executive function in posttraumatic stress disorder: A systematic review. Journal of affective disorders, 141(1), 1121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reichenberg, A. (2005). Cognitive impairment as a risk factor for psychosis. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 7(1), 3138. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2005.7.1/areichenbergCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reichenberg, A., Weiser, M., Rapp, M. A., Rabinowitz, J., Caspi, A., Schmeidler, J., … Harvey, P. D. (2005). Elaboration on premorbid intellectual performance in schizophrenia: Premorbid intellectual decline and risk for schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(12), 12971304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rock, P. L., Roiser, J., Riedel, W. J., & Blackwell, A. (2014). Cognitive impairment in depression: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 44(10), 20292040.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rotstein, A., Goldenberg, J., Fund, S., Levine, S. Z., & Reichenberg, A. (2021). Capturing adolescents in need of psychiatric care with psychopathological symptoms: A population-based cohort study. European Psychiatry, 64(1), E76. doi: 10.1192/j.eurpsy.2021.2251CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russo, M., Murray, R., & Reichenberg, A. (2013). Cognitive impairment and symptom dimensions in psychosis. In Harvey, P. D. (Ed.), Cognitive impairment in schizophrenia. Characteristics, assessment, and treatment (pp. 6984). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Southward, M., Cheavens, J., & Coccaro, E. (2022). Defining the p-factor: An empirical test of five leading theories. Psychological Medicine, 112. doi: 10.1017/S0033291722001635Google ScholarPubMed
Toulopoulou, T., Picchioni, M., Rijsdijk, F., Hua-Hall, M., Ettinger, U., Sham, P., & Murray, R. (2007). Substantial genetic overlap between neurocognition and schizophrenia: Genetic modeling in twin samples. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64(12), 13481355. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.12.1348CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tripoli, G., Quattrone, D., Ferraro, L., Gayer-Anderson, C., Rodriguez, V., La Cascia, C., … Tarricone, I. (2021). Jumping to conclusions, general intelligence, and psychosis liability: Findings from the multi-centre EU-GEI case–control study. Psychological Medicine, 51(4), 623633.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (1955). Manual for the Wechsler adult intelligence scale. New York: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Weiser, M., Frenkel, O., Fenchel, D., Tzur, D., Sandin, S., Janecka, M., … Reichenberg, A. (2021). Familial clustering of psychiatric disorders and low IQ. Psychological Medicine, 17. doi: 10.1017/S0033291721004852Google ScholarPubMed
Weiser, M., Reichenberg, A., Rabinowitz, J., Knobler, H., Lubin, G., Yazvitzky, R., … Davidson, M. (2004). Cognitive performance of male adolescents is lower than controls across psychiatric disorders: A population-based study. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 110(6), 471475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiser, M., Reichenberg, A., Rabinowitz, J., Nahon, D., Kravitz, E., Lubin, G., … Noy, S. (2007). Impaired reading comprehension and mathematical abilities in male adolescents with average or above general intellectual abilities are associated with comorbid and future psychopathology. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 195(11), 883890.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wraw, C., Deary, I. J., Der, G., & Gale, C. R. (2016). Intelligence in youth and mental health at age 50. Intelligence, 58, 6979.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zanelli, J., Mollon, J., Sandin, S., Morgan, C., Dazzan, P., Pilecka, I., … Fearon, P. (2019). Cognitive change in schizophrenia and other psychoses in the decade following the first episode. American Journal of Psychiatry, 176(10), 811819.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zanelli, J., Morgan, K., Dazzan, P., Morgan, C., Russo, M., Pilecka, I., … Jones, P. B. (2013). Gender differences in neuropsychological performance across psychotic disorders–a multi-centre population based case–control study. PloS one, 8(10), e77318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. models of the structure of psychopathology with and without cognition.Note. Abbreviations. P, A general factor of psychopathology; Ex, Externalizing; In, Internalizing; TD, Thought Disorder; SA, Substance Abuse; CD, Conduct Disorder (i.e. Hostility); IS, Interpersonal Sensitivity; De, Depression; An, Anxiety; PA, Phobic Anxiety; OC, Obsessive-Compulsive, Ps, Psychoticism; PI, Paranoid Ideation; IQ, Intelligence Quotient; VU, Verbal Understanding; VS, Visual-Spatial problem-solving abilities; MR, Mathematical reasoning; Ca, Categorization abilities.The numbers represent the standardized loadings beta estimates. Green lines represent positive estimates, whereas red lines represent negative estimates. All factor loadings are statistically significant. Due to multicollinearity, the standardized loadings estimates can exceed the bounds of (−1,1) (Deegan, 1978).

Figure 1

Figure 2. Bivariate Pearson correlations between the study variables.

Figure 2

Figure 3. (a)The percentage of high, average, and low cognition for distinct levels of psychopathology. (b) The percentage of high, average, and low psychopathology for distinct levels of cognition.

Figure 3

Table 1. Fit indices for models of psychopathology with and without cognition

Supplementary material: File

Rotstein et al. supplementary material
Download undefined(File)
File 229.6 KB