Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-5g6vh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T19:10:57.983Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The p factor of psychopathology and personality in middle childhood: genetic and gestational risk factors

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 February 2023

Line C. Gjerde*
Affiliation:
Department of Mental Disorders, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Oslo, Norway Promenta Research Center, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
Espen Moen Eilertsen
Affiliation:
Promenta Research Center, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway Centre for Fertility and Health, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Oslo, Norway
Tom A. McAdams
Affiliation:
Promenta Research Center, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway Social, Genetic & Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College, London, UK
Rosa Cheesman
Affiliation:
Promenta Research Center, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
Terrie E. Moffitt
Affiliation:
Promenta Research Center, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway Social, Genetic & Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College, London, UK Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, USA
Avshalom Caspi
Affiliation:
Promenta Research Center, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway Social, Genetic & Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College, London, UK Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, USA
Thalia C. Eley
Affiliation:
Promenta Research Center, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway Social, Genetic & Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College, London, UK
Espen Røysamb
Affiliation:
Promenta Research Center, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway Department of Child Development, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Oslo, Norway
Tom H. Rosenström
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Logopedics, Faculty of Medicine, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland
Eivind Ystrom
Affiliation:
Department of Mental Disorders, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Oslo, Norway Promenta Research Center, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway School of Pharmacy, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
*
Author for correspondence: Line C. Gjerde, E-mail: Line.Gjerde@fhi.no
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

A joint, hierarchical structure of psychopathology and personality has been reported in adults but should also be investigated at earlier ages, as psychopathology often develops before adulthood. Here, we investigate the joint factor structure of psychopathology and personality in eight-year-old children, estimate factor heritability and explore external validity through associations with established developmental risk factors.

Methods

Phenotypic and biometric exploratory factor analyses with bifactor rotation on genetically informative data from the Norwegian Mother, Father, and Child Cohort (MoBa) study. The analytic sub-sample comprised 10 739 children (49% girls). Mothers reported their children's symptoms of depression (Short Moods and Feelings Questionnaire), anxiety (Screen for Anxiety Related Disorders), attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder inattention and hyperactivity, oppositional-defiant disorder, conduct disorder (Parent/Teacher Rating Scale for Disruptive Behavior Disorders), and Big Five personality (short Hierarchical Personality Inventory for Children). Developmental risk factors (early gestational age and being small for gestational age) were collected from the Medical Birth Registry.

Results

Goodness-of-fit indices favored a p factor model with three residual latent factors interpreted as negative affectivity, positive affectivity, and antagonism, whereas psychometric indices favored a one-factor model. ADE solutions fitted best, and regression analyses indicated a negative association between gestational age and the p factor, for both the one- and four-factor solutions.

Conclusion

Correlations between normative and pathological traits in middle childhood mostly reflect one heritable and psychometrically interpretable p factor, although optimal fit to data required less interpretable residual latent factors. The association between the p factor and low gestational age warrants further study of early developmental mechanisms.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

In psychopathology, comorbidity is common. Around half of people who meet diagnostic criteria for one disorder simultaneously meet criteria for other disorders (Newman, Moffitt, Caspi, & Silva, Reference Newman, Moffitt, Caspi and Silva1998). The need to understand comorbidity in mental health has inspired research on the structure of psychopathology using factor-analytic methods. A two-factor model, encompassing an internalizing factor characterized by negative mood states and behavioral inhibition, and an externalizing factor, characterized by behavioral disinhibition explain cross-disorder correlations well in samples of both children (Achenbach, Reference Achenbach1992) and adults (Krueger, Reference Krueger1999). However, the extensive cross-correlation between the internalizing and externalizing spectra themselves (Cosgrove et al., Reference Cosgrove, Rhee, Gelhorn, Boeldt, Corley, Ehringer and Hewitt2011; Lahey et al., Reference Lahey, Rathouz, Van Hulle, Urbano, Krueger, Applegate and Waldman2008) has made the notion of a continuous general factor of psychopathology (often referred to as p; Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Houts, Belsky, Goldman-Mellor, Harrington, Israel and Moffitt2014) increasingly popular in summarizing and explaining liability to psychopathology (although other approaches to comorbidity exist – such as severity and directionality assessments [Marceau & Neiderhiser, Reference Marceau and Neiderhiser2022)].

Cross-correlations and one overarching p factor of psychopathology suggest that categorical nosologies of psychopathology fall short of capturing the complexity in psychopathology. As a response, the Hierarchical Taxonomy of Psychopathology (HiTOP; Kotov et al., Reference Kotov, Krueger, Watson, Achenbach, Althoff, Bagby and Zimmerman2017) works toward an alternative nosology based on a dimensional model of psychopathology. Following this work, there is a growing consensus about the importance of personality (characteristic ways of thinking, feeling, and behaving) particularly in the form of the Big Five framework (Goldberg, Reference Goldberg1990; McCrae & Costa, Reference McCrae and Costa1987), for psychopathology (Widiger et al., Reference Widiger, Sellbom, Chmielewski, Clark, DeYoung, Kotov and Wright2019). First, the HiTOP superspectra align closely with the Big Five personality dimensions (neuroticism, extraversion, openness, agreeableness, and conscientiousness; Kotov et al., Reference Kotov, Krueger, Watson, Achenbach, Althoff, Bagby and Zimmerman2017; Widiger et al., Reference Widiger, Sellbom, Chmielewski, Clark, DeYoung, Kotov and Wright2019), and p factors of personality and psychopathology correlate strongly (McCabe, Oltmanns, & Widiger, Reference McCabe, Oltmanns and Widiger2022). Second, personality contributes substantially to different life outcomes (Ozer & Benet-Martínez, Reference Ozer and Benet-Martínez2006) including common mental disorders (e.g. Kotov, Gamez, Schmidt, & Watson, Reference Kotov, Gamez, Schmidt and Watson2010). Third, the HiTOP postulates inclusion of personality traits assessment to predict future psychopathology (Widiger et al., Reference Widiger, Sellbom, Chmielewski, Clark, DeYoung, Kotov and Wright2019), recently demonstrated by Waszczuk et al., who found that personality traits better predicted future psychopathology than previous psychiatric diagnoses (Waszczuk et al., Reference Waszczuk, Hopwood, Luft, Morey, Perlman, Ruggero and Kotov2021).

We and others have previously shown that correlations between personality and psychopathology can be rotated to a general behavioral risk factor (McCabe et al., Reference McCabe, Oltmanns and Widiger2022; Rosenström et al., Reference Rosenström, Gjerde, Krueger, Aggen, Czajkowski, Gillespie and Ystrom2018). However, attempts to investigate the joint factorial structure of psychopathology and personality have only been preliminary in childhood (Shields, Giljen, España, & Tackett, Reference Shields, Giljen, España and Tackett2021). The p factor in childhood is poorly understood (Levin-Aspenson, Watson, Clark, & Zimmerman, Reference Levin-Aspenson, Watson, Clark and Zimmerman2020). Some find that p is predominantly linked with internalizing symptoms (Lahey, Van Hulle, Singh, Waldman, & Rathouz, Reference Lahey, Van Hulle, Singh, Waldman and Rathouz2011; Tackett et al., Reference Tackett, Lahey, van Hulle, Waldman, Krueger and Rathouz2013), others with externalizing symptoms and inattention (Moore et al., Reference Moore, Kaczkurkin, Durham, Jeong, McDowell, Dupont and Lahey2020; Olino et al., Reference Olino, Bufferd, Dougherty, Dyson, Carlson and Klein2018), particularly when personality is included (Slobodskaya, Reference Slobodskaya2014). Mixed findings could be due to variations in content sampling and the age span included (Levin-Aspenson et al., Reference Levin-Aspenson, Watson, Clark and Zimmerman2020). In this study, we focus our investigation on middle childhood (age 8 years), a period marked by dramatic changes in self-regulation, executive functions, and mentalization (DelGiudice, Reference DelGiudice, Halfon, Forrest, Lerner and Faustman2018). As personality traits are more easily identifiable than psychopathology in prepubertal children, the present research could identify potential personality trait antecedents of psychopathology that may ultimately be intervened on.

Critiques of the p factor put forward that the p factor is only descriptive, and not more than the sum of its parts (Fried, Greene, & Eaton, Reference Fried, Greene and Eaton2021). In the present study, we seek to convey that the p factor is a useful construct in understanding etiology, thus moving beyond mere description, if it (1) captures early genetic and environmental risk for psychopathology in childhood, (2) demonstrates basic psychometric properties (Bonifay, Lane, & Reise, Reference Bonifay, Lane and Reise2016), and (3) relates to putative early risk factors for psychopathology, in line with the nomological network thinking for construct validity (Cronbach & Meehl, Reference Cronbach and Meehl1955).

The p factor and personality traits are heritable (Allegrini et al., Reference Allegrini, Cheesman, Rimfeld, Selzam, Pingault, Eley and Plomin2020; Waldman, Poore, van Hulle, Rathouz, & Lahey, Reference Waldman, Poore, van Hulle, Rathouz and Lahey2016). There is also evidence of genetic correlations between psychopathology and personality (Blonigen, Hicks, Krueger, Patrick, & Iacono, Reference Blonigen, Hicks, Krueger, Patrick and Iacono2005; Bouchard & McGue, Reference Bouchard and McGue2003; Czajkowski et al., Reference Czajkowski, Aggen, Krueger, Kendler, Neale, Knudsen and Reichborn-Kjennerud2018). However, the heritability of a common childhood p factor, with personality included, has not been estimated.

Gestational age and being small for gestational age (SGA) are associated with poorer functioning in several domains (Gluckman & Hanson, Reference Gluckman and Hanson2006; Wolke, Johnson, & Mendonça, Reference Wolke, Johnson and Mendonça2019). For instance, children born preterm or with low birth weight have significantly more internalizing and externalizing problems in childhood, adolescence, and young adulthood (Hack et al., Reference Hack, Youngstrom, Cartar, Schluchter, Taylor, Flannery and Borawski2004; Laerum et al., Reference Laerum, Reitan, Evensen, Lydersen, Brubakk, Skranes and Indredavik2019; Mathewson et al., Reference Mathewson, Chow, Dobson, Pope, Schmidt and Van Lieshout2017). SGA has been found to be associated with a p factor in adults when familial confounding is controlled for (Pettersson, Larsson, D'Onofrio, Almqvist, & Lichtenstein, Reference Pettersson, Larsson, D'Onofrio, Almqvist and Lichtenstein2019). One possible pathway from gestational risk factors to later psychopathology is through compromised brain development, for instance due to a lack of oxygen and nutrients during a critical period (Kapellou et al., Reference Kapellou, Counsell, Kennea, Dyet, Saeed, Stark and Edwards2006; Walhovd et al., Reference Walhovd, Fjell, Brown, Kuperman, Chung, Hagler and Dale2012), another is through social factors such as parenting (Wolke et al., Reference Wolke, Johnson and Mendonça2019).

Using a large, population-based birth cohort of eight-year-old children with measures on a broad range of psychopathology traits as well as on Big Five personality we aim to (1) explore the joint, hierarchical structure of psychopathology and personality traits in middle childhood; (2) estimate genetic and environmental contributions to the obtained latent variables; and (3) investigate associations between putative early risk factors (gestational age and SGA) and the obtained latent variables.

Methods

Sample

This study is part of the Norwegian Mother, Father and Child Cohort Study (MoBa), conducted by the Norwegian Institute of Public Health. MoBa is a prospective, ongoing pregnancy cohort study (Magnus et al., Reference Magnus, Birke, Vejrup, Haugan, Alsaker, Daltveit and Stoltenberg2016). Participants were recruited from 1999 to 2008 at a routine ultrasound examination offered to all pregnant women in Norway at gestational week ≈18. The total sample includes >114 500 children, >95 000 mothers and >75 000 fathers. In total, 41% of eligible women participated. The current study is based on the genetically informative subproject called the Intergenerational Transmission of Risk, where the wider kinship (e.g. twins, siblings, cousins) between participants in both the parent and the child generation has been identified (eAppendix 1). The present study consisted of 10 739 children (49% girls) with a relative also participating in the MoBa study. In the study sample there were 117 monozygotic twin relations, 4261 dizygotic twin and sibling relations, 108 half-sibling relations, 2354 cousin relations and 96 half-cousin relations. The additive genetic correlations between these types of relatives are 1.0, 0.5, 0.25, 0.125 and 0.0625, respectively. Non-additive genetic correlations are 1.0 for monozygotic twins, 0.25 for dizygotic twins and full siblings, and 0.00 for the rest of the relations. Among the relatives, there were 4420 shared-mother relations (necessary to model shared environmental influences as discussed in the biometric modeling procedure below).

Version 11 of the quality-assured MoBa data files were used, released in 2018. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants upon recruitment. The establishment and data collection in MoBa was previously based on a license from the Norwegian Data protection agency and approval from The Regional Committee for Medical Research Ethics, and is now based on regulations related to the Norwegian Health Registry Act. The current study was approved by The Regional Committee for Medical Research Ethics.

Measures

Depressive symptoms were reported by mothers using the 13-item Short Moods and Feelings Questionnaire ( Angold et al., Reference Angold, Costello, Messer, Pickles, Winder and Silver1995). Anxiety symptoms were reported by mothers using the five-item version of the Screen for Anxiety Related Disorders (SCARED; Birmaher et al., Reference Birmaher, Khetarpal, Brent, Cully, Balach, Kaufman and Neer1997). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), oppositional-defiant disorder (ODD), and conduct disorder (CD) symptoms were reported by mothers using the Parent/Teacher Rating Scale for Disruptive Behavior Disorders (RS-DBD; Silva et al., Reference Silva, Alpert, Pouget, Silva, Trosper, Reyes and Dummit2005). We analyzed inattention and hyperactivity in ADHD separately due to recent evidence on differential etiologies (Gustavson et al., Reference Gustavson, Torvik, Eilertsen, Ask, McAdams, Hannigan and Gjerde2021). Nine items were each used to measure ADHD inattention and hyperactivity, and eight items to measure ODD and CD, respectively. We created sum scores of the scale items for each of the six traits.

Big Five personality (neuroticism, extraversion, imagination, conscientiousness and benevolence/agreeableness) was reported by mothers using the short Hierarchical Personality Inventory for Children (HiPIC-30; Vollrath, Hampson, & Torgersen, Reference Vollrath, Hampson and Torgersen2016). Each personality trait was constructed using the sum of six items. More information on the psychopathology and personality scales (e.g. items and response categories) can be found in MoBa's instrument documentation (Jin, Reference Jin2016).

Measures of gestational age and birth weight were collected from the Medical Birth Registry, which contains information on all births in Norway from 1967 and onwards (Irgens, Reference Irgens2000). Gestational age was centered on 40 weeks. Birth weight was included as SGA. This was a binary variable scored 1 for those who weighed less than 2 standard deviations below expected birth weight and zero otherwise, as defined by Marsál (Marsál et al., Reference Marsál, Persson, Larsen, Lilja, Selbing and Sultan1996).

Statistical analyses

Horn's parallel analysis (Horn, Reference Horn1965) was conducted as an initial test of how many latent factors to include. We proceeded by fitting several bifactor exploratory factor analysis (EFA) models to the data, and evaluated each latent factor on both goodness-of-fit and psychometric indices. Regarding psychometric indices, we emphasized the H-index (H > 0.70), which is a measure of how well the latent variable is defined by its indicators (Rodriguez, Reise, & Haviland, Reference Rodriguez, Reise and Haviland2016). Other indices were also included for comprehensiveness (short descriptions in Table 1). These are discussed thoroughly elsewhere (Rodriguez et al., Reference Rodriguez, Reise and Haviland2016). Next, we selected the best fitting model(s) and ran biometric EFA versions of these to investigate etiology and criterion validity. With respect to the biometric modeling, we distinguish between additive genetic- (A), non-additive/dominance genetic- (D), common environmental- (C) and unique environmental influences (E). The correlation structure of A and D among individuals was specified according to the additive and non-additive genetic correlations derived from the pedigree structure (described above). We defined C as an environmental component shared among individuals with the same mother, and E was defined as an environmental component unique to the individual. The common factor model assumes that the responses relating to an individual (y) can be described as

$${\boldsymbol y} = {\vector \Lambda }{\boldsymbol \eta } + {\rm \epsilon }, \;$$

where ${\vector \Lambda }$ is the factor loading matrix, η a vector of common factors and ${\rm \epsilon }$ a vector of unique factors. In the biometric extension of the factor model we specified that the common and unique factors are a function of genetic and environmental components, e.g.:

$${\boldsymbol \eta } = {\boldsymbol A}_{\boldsymbol c} + {\boldsymbol D}_{\boldsymbol c} + {\boldsymbol C}_{\boldsymbol c} + {\boldsymbol E}_{\boldsymbol c}, \;$$
$${\rm \epsilon } = {\boldsymbol A}_{\boldsymbol u} + {\boldsymbol D}_{\boldsymbol u} + {\boldsymbol C}_{\boldsymbol u} + {\boldsymbol E}_{\boldsymbol u}.$$

Table 1. Psychometric and goodness of fit indices for different factor solutions on the phenotypic exploratory factor analysis models with bifactor rotation

Note: only factor loadings >0.2 was included in the indices. F1 = first residual/specific latent factor; F2 = second residual/specific latent factor; F3 = third residual/specific latent factor. H = H index, a construct replicability index where high values reflect that the factor is well defined by its indicators. Threshold commonly used is >0.70; ECV = Explained common variance, a measure of strength of the general factor (the value indicates the proportion of the total variance in the indicators explained by the general rather than the specific latent factors); Omega = a measure of reliability, indicating the proportion of variance attributable to both the general and specific factors together; OmegaH = Omega hierarchical, a measure of reliability that estimates the proportion of variance attributable to the general factor only; Factor determinacy = an index of trustworthiness of the latent factor, where a high values indicates that the predicted factor scores correspond well with the corresponding factor. Threshold commonly used is >0.90; PUC = Percent uncontaminated correlations, an indicator of how many percent of all correlations among indicators attributable to the general factor (Hancock & Mueller, Reference Hancock, Mueller, Cudeck, du Toit and Sörbom2001; Rodriguez et al., Reference Rodriguez, Reise and Haviland2016). AIC = Akaike's information criterium, a measure of a model's goodness of fit relative to other models, where parsimony is favored. The preferred model is the one with the lowest AIC value (Akaike, Reference Akaike1987); BIC = Bayesian information criterium, a relative goodness of fit index, similar to AIC, but with different penalizing of model complexity (Schwarz, Reference Schwarz1978); RMSEA = Root mean square error of approximation, an absolute goodness of fit index, that assesses how far a hypothesized model is from a perfect model (Steiger, Reference Steiger1990). Threshold commonly used is <0.05; CFI = Comparative fit index (Bentler, Reference Bentler1990), an absolute goodness of fit index, similar to RMSEA but often used in exploratory contexts. Threshold commonly used is >0.95.

Given the current pedigree it is statistically difficult to distinguish C from D effects. We therefore ran ACE and ADE models separately. The six symptom clusters and five personality traits were first residualized on child sex. Full information maximum likelihood was used to fit the models, and the factor loadings matrix was rotated using the Jennrich–Bentler orthogonal bifactor rotation (Jennrich & Bentler, Reference Jennrich and Bentler2011) with the function bifactorT in the GPArotation package in R (Bernaards & Jennrich, Reference Bernaards and Jennrich2005). Here, a single general factor is isolated that explains covariance between all symptom clusters and traits, in addition to residual latent factors that are uncorrelated with the general factor and explain residual covariance between clusters of variables not accounted for by the general factor (Jennrich & Bentler, Reference Jennrich and Bentler2011). Genetic and environmental sources of variance on the rotated common factors were estimated, along with genetic and environmental residual variance for each trait. We first estimated a full model, in which A, C/D and E influences were allowed both on the latent factors and the observed traits. We then tested fixing the C/D effects on the residuals of the observed traits to zero, while retaining them on the latent factors. The most restricted model was a model where C/D was fixed to zero both on latent factors and observed-trait residuals. The nested sub-models were compared to the full models using Akaike's information criterion (AIC; Akaike, Reference Akaike1987). As bifactor rotation solutions have been criticized for being unstable, we also simulated how stable the best fitting solution was in this dataset (eAppendix 2).

To investigate their associations with gestational age and birth weight, the general psychopathology factor as well as the residual latent factors was each regressed onto gestational age and SGA in a joint model including the best fitting biometric structure. Gestational age was allowed both a linear and quadratic association with the latent factors. The modeling procedures were conducted in R, using the svcmr package (code available at https://github.com/espenmei/svcmr).

Results

Model fitting

A correlation matrix of the traits is shown in Fig. 1. Horn's parallel test indicated three factors (online Supplementary Fig. S1). The four-factor model (Fig. 2a) had a superior fit according to the goodness-of-fit indices (Table 1) and was also highly stable in this dataset (eAppendix 2). However, only a one-factor model (Fig. 2b) satisfied psychometric criteria for interpretability (e.g. H > 0.7; Table 1).

Fig. 1. Correlations between included variables.

Note: Gray cross indicates correlations not significant (p > 0.05).

Fig. 2. Best fitting EFA models.

Note: Loadings below 0.20 are not shown in Fig. 2a. A = Broad-sense genetic influences (including both additive and non-additive/dominance effects); E = unique environmental influences and measurement error.

Twin- and sibling correlations indicated that an ADE model would fit the data best (online Supplementary Fig. S2). This was confirmed by goodness-of-fit indices for both the four- and one-factor solution (Table 2). After bifactor rotation on the four-factor solution, a p factor (F1) was isolated. Similarly to the one-factor solution all symptom clusters and neuroticism had positive loadings and the other personality dimensions had negative loadings on this general factor (online Supplementary Table S1). Three residual latent factors also emerged: a negative affectivity factor (F2), with loadings on depression and anxiety symptoms and neuroticism, along with a positive affectivity factor (F3; loading onto extraversion, imagination and ADHD hyperactivity symptoms), and a less clear antagonism factor (F4) that resembled rule-breaking behavior (positive loadings on CD, oppositional defiant disorder and conscientiousness, and negative loadings on benevolence and inattention). Variance explained by the p factor and residual factors along with variance unique to the traits for both models are shown in online Supplementary Table S2. In the four-factor solution, the p factor explained most variance in ADHD inattention (70%; only 12% was unique to the trait), whereas for the one-factor model it was oppositional defiant disorder (57%; 43% unique to the trait).

Table 2. Model fit statistics from bifactor exploratory factor analyses

Best fitting models are shown in bold. −2LL = two times the negative log likelihood – an estimate of how well the model fits the data; ep = number of estimated parameters included in the model; AIC = Akaike's information criterion – an indicator of how well the model fits the data that also penalizes complex models; df = degrees of freedom; ΔLL = the difference in log likelihood compared to the full model; p = probability value for rejecting the null hypothesis. cACE sACE = Additive genetic (A), shared environmental/shared mother effects (C) and unique environmental (E) effects on both common factors (c) and specific traits (s); cACE sAE = shared environmental/shared mother effects only on common factors and not specific traits; cADE sADE = Additive genetic (A), non-additive/dominance effects (D) and unique environmental (E) effects on both common factors (c) and specific traits (s); cADE sAE = non-additive/dominance effects only on common factors and not specific traits; cAE sAE = only additive genetic and unique environmental effects on both common factors and specific traits.

Genetic and environmental contributions

The narrow-sense heritability of the p factor in the four-factor solution was 0.70, and dominance effects accounted for 0.05, giving a broad-sense heritability of 0.75. For the one-factor solution, only additive genetic influences contributed to the heritability (0.82). For the residual latent factors, the narrow-sense heritabilities were 0.17 for negative affectivity, 0.56 for positive affectivity, and 0.02 for antagonism, and dominance effects accounted for 0.49, 0.17, 0.22, giving broad-sense heritabilities of 0.67, 0.73, and 0.24, respectively. The rest of the variance in p and the residual latent factors was accounted for by unique environmental influences and measurement error. Residual broad-sense heritability spanned from 0.04 for neuroticism to 0.46 for anxiety (mean = 0.22; online Supplementary Table S3). Corresponding numbers for the one-factor solution were 0.11 ADHD inattention and 0.63 for imagination (mean = 0.37; online Supplementary Table S3). The rest of the variance was explained by unique environmental influences and measurement error.

As finding evidence for D over C in models of a wide range of psychopathology and personality traits was unexpected, and these traits were rated by mothers, it is possible the dominance effects reflect rater bias to some extent (Derks, Hudziak, & Boomsma, Reference Derks, Hudziak, Boomsma and Kim2009). We therefore conducted sensitivity analyses on mono- and dizygotic twin pairs only to get an indication of whether sibling interaction or rater bias (Simonoff et al., Reference Simonoff, Pickles, Hervas, Silberg, Rutter and Eaves1998) could explain dominance effects (eAppendix 3). This was done using the method of adding an extra parameter that allows for feedback loops between siblings (Carey, Reference Carey1986) on univariate biometric models of each trait separately as well as on a sum-score of the traits to resemble a p factor. We then compared goodness-of-fit between ADE models v. AE models with the added sibling feedback parameter. For most of the phenotypes, the AE + sibling feedback parameter fitted the data best.

Associations between gestational age, SGA, p and residual latent factors

In the four-factor solution, children born SGA scored statistically significantly higher on negative affectivity compared to children not classified as SGA (β = 0.26, s.e. = 0.077, p = 0.001; online Supplementary Table S4). For p there was no difference in scores for SGA compared to non-SGA children (p = 0.838), nor for the two residual latent factors positive affectivity (p = 0.908) and antagonism (p = 0.160). In the one-factor solution, there was no association between p and SGA.

Low gestational age had a curvilinear, negative association with p that flattened as gestational age approached term in both the four-factor (p = 0.036) and one-factor solution (p = 0.002). For instance, children born in gestational week 28 were predicted to score ≈0.4 standard deviations (s.d.) higher on p compared to children born in gestational week 40 (Fig. 3 and online Supplementary Fig. S3). This pattern was very similar for both the one- and four-factor solution. For the four-factor solution, gestational age had a positive, curvilinear statistically significantly association with two of the three residual latent factors: positive affectivity (p = 0.046), and antagonism (p = 0.012). Children born in gestational week 28 were predicted a ≈0.42 s.d. lower score on antagonism compared to children born full term (week 40). There was no evidence of an interaction between gestational age and SGA on the factors.

Fig. 3. Predicted latent factor scores for different levels of gestational age. The figure shows the association between gestational age and p, as well as between gestational age and the residual latent factors (F2–F4), expressed as predicted factor scores for different levels of gestational age. Factors have mean = 0 and standard deviation of 1. The vertical lines index gestational age 40 (term), 37 (early term), 32 (very preterm) and 28 weeks (extremely preterm). Dotted lines are predicted factor scores for children born small for gestational age.

Discussion

The present study provides insight into the nature of psychopathology risk in middle childhood. A p factor could be recovered in eight-year-old children when personality was included in the structure, in line with what has been shown in adults (Kotov et al., Reference Kotov, Krueger, Watson, Achenbach, Althoff, Bagby and Zimmerman2017; Rosenström et al., Reference Rosenström, Gjerde, Krueger, Aggen, Czajkowski, Gillespie and Ystrom2018) and a spectrum model of psychopathology and personality (Widiger, Reference Widiger2011). According to our findings, the p factor in middle childhood is characterized by high scores on inattention, oppositional defiant behavior, and hyperactivity as well as low scores on conscientiousness and agreeableness (Fig. 2, online Supplementary Table S2). There are to our knowledge no other comprehensive, factorial studies on the joint structure of psychopathology and personality in middle childhood.

The p factor recovered in the present study fulfilled all criteria we defined for being a useful construct (capturing genetic and environmental risk, demonstrating psychometric properties for interpretability, and criterion validity). The p factor was also robust, as it was almost identical in the one- and four factor solution on loading pattern, heritability, and strength and direction of association with early putative risk factors.

Our findings contribute to the debate on what constitutes the core of the p factor in middle childhood. Some find it to be defined by internalizing aspects (Lahey et al., Reference Lahey, Van Hulle, Singh, Waldman and Rathouz2011; Waldman et al., Reference Waldman, Poore, van Hulle, Rathouz and Lahey2016), some by externalizing and autism aspects (Allegrini et al., Reference Allegrini, Cheesman, Rimfeld, Selzam, Pingault, Eley and Plomin2020; Martel et al., Reference Martel, Pan, Hoffmann, Gadelha, do Rosario, Mari and Salum2017; Moore et al., Reference Moore, Kaczkurkin, Durham, Jeong, McDowell, Dupont and Lahey2020; Neumann et al., Reference Neumann, Pappa, Lahey, Verhulst, Medina-Gomez, Jaddoe and Tiemeier2016), and some by borderline personality traits which sit in between internalizing and externalizing spectra (Gluschkoff, Jokela, & Rosenström, Reference Gluschkoff, Jokela and Rosenström2021). Our study adds to the literature by linking established developmental risk factors and personality in an etiologically important age period to a model of the p factor. Here, the constellation of associations between normative personality traits and the p factor resembled that of normative personality and borderline personality disorder (Samuel & Widiger, Reference Samuel and Widiger2008), as observed for adults (Rosenström et al., Reference Rosenström, Gjerde, Krueger, Aggen, Czajkowski, Gillespie and Ystrom2018). In the four- and one-factor models, the strongest and second strongest loadings, respectively, on the p factor was for ADHD inattention. ADHD has particularly strong etiological links to borderline personality disorder (Kuja-Halkola et al., Reference Kuja-Halkola, Lind Juto, Skoglund, Rück, Mataix-Cols, Pérez-Vigil and Larsson2021). Furthermore, our p factor was associated with early gestational age that is also a risk factor for ADHD inattention (Ask et al., Reference Ask, Gustavson, Ystrom, Havdahl, Tesli, Askeland and Reichborn-Kjennerud2018). Thus, we argue that the p factor may be a natural model for psychopathology that sits between traditional internalizing and externalizing spectra rather than being their re-expression.

It is worth commenting on why both the one- and four-factor solutions were included. Goodness-of-fit is often used when the main aim is to explore structure rather than to construct measurement instruments. In confirmatory modeling and when robust constructs or measures are of interest, the recommendation is to also include psychometric fit indices (Rodriguez et al., Reference Rodriguez, Reise and Haviland2016) to ensure that included residual latent factors are interpretable and replicable. In the present study, none of the models performed well for all latent factors on both goodness-of-fit and psychometric indices. Thus, we used two models to show that the p factor was the same across the models and attained good performance on all indices. As we studied the correlation structure of 11 quite different psychopathology and personality traits, measured with different scales using an ESEM approach (Asparouhov & Muthén, Reference Asparouhov and Muthén2009), we did not expect a clean psychometric measurement model. The ESEM strategy has been created precisely because such clean structures are often infeasible when underlying structures are of interest. Previous studies on the hierarchical structure of psychopathology usually need two residual latent factors in addition to p (e.g. Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Houts, Belsky, Goldman-Mellor, Harrington, Israel and Moffitt2014; Lahey et al., Reference Lahey, Van Hulle, Singh, Waldman and Rathouz2011). When personality is added, no less residual factors should be needed. From a structural viewpoint, our multifactor model makes sense, and hopefully also appeal to some applied researchers that may take interest in the evidence for psychometrically valid scale constructs (as discussed for instance in Lahey, Moore, Kaczkurkin, & Zald, Reference Lahey, Moore, Kaczkurkin and Zald2021).

In the four-factor solution, three residual latent factors in addition to p were necessary to explain covariance in the data. We interpreted these as a negative affectivity (F2), a positive affectivity (F3), and a less clear antagonism factor (F4). Contrary to the p factor, reliability estimates for these factors were sub-optimal (Hancock & Mueller, Reference Hancock, Mueller, Cudeck, du Toit and Sörbom2001; Rodriguez et al., Reference Rodriguez, Reise and Haviland2016) and their interpretation is more imprecise. We therefore refrain from closer interpretation of their content.

The high broad-sense heritability of the p factor (75-82%) indicates that early etiology of psychiatric burden is driven by genetic risks. This is in line with previous studies on p in childhood and adolescent samples (Allegrini et al., Reference Allegrini, Cheesman, Rimfeld, Selzam, Pingault, Eley and Plomin2020; Lahey et al., Reference Lahey, Van Hulle, Singh, Waldman and Rathouz2011; Waldman et al., Reference Waldman, Poore, van Hulle, Rathouz and Lahey2016), although the influence of genes seems to be higher in our study. However, it is unusual to find evidence for non-additive genetic effects in etiological studies of the hierarchical structure of childhood psychopathology (e.g. Lahey et al., Reference Lahey, Van Hulle, Singh, Waldman and Rathouz2011). As we have included personality, the finding of D-effects makes sense as such effects have been found for personality traits and ADHD (Derks et al., Reference Derks, Hudziak, Boomsma and Kim2009; Keller, Coventry, Heath, & Martin, Reference Keller, Coventry, Heath and Martin2005). Yet, as all our included traits had substantial D-effects, this finding may to some extent reflect rater contrast effects (Simonoff et al., Reference Simonoff, Pickles, Hervas, Silberg, Rutter and Eaves1998). This suspicion was supported by the sensitivity analyses (eAppendix 3), making this topic a feasible possibility for further study.

Understanding how personality relates to psychopathology can be valuable in clinical settings, since personality traits can be measured in young children before the onset of psychopathology. We have previously shown that when modeling the joint structure of psychopathology and personality in adults, all Big Five traits (except openness) load onto the p factor (Rosenström et al., Reference Rosenström, Gjerde, Krueger, Aggen, Czajkowski, Gillespie and Ystrom2018). The personality profile that best reflected p was a high score on neuroticism as well as low scores on conscientiousness and agreeableness. In this sample of eight-year-olds, the findings were similar, but instead low scores on conscientiousness and benevolence were most characteristic for p. Conscientiousness and benevolence even had higher loadings on p than many of the psychopathology traits. This finding extends those of previous studies where neuroticism is typically found to be most important, but is not surprising considering the centrality of poor self-regulation on developmental psychopathology (Nigg, Reference Nigg2017). Perhaps children presenting with behavior that resembles a profile of low conscientiousness and benevolence, along with high neuroticism should be followed more closely than children with a less risk-prone personality profile to prevent psychopathology. Our study cannot answer whether these traits predict risk for later psychopathology as the measurements were conducted at the same time-point, but personality has been shown to be relatively stable in childhood (Lamb, Chuang, Wessels, Broberg, & Hwang, Reference Lamb, Chuang, Wessels, Broberg and Hwang2002).

The p factor was negatively associated with gestational age, indicating that prematurely born children scored higher on general psychopathology risk. This finding, along with the high heritability and interpretability, supports the notion that p is a clinically relevant construct. We can only speculate on the mechanisms behind the association between p and gestational age. It is known that preterm birth compromises brain development (Davis et al., Reference Davis, Buss, Muftuler, Head, Hasso, Wing and Sandman2011) and is associated with smaller brain volume (Nosarti et al., Reference Nosarti, Al-Asady, Frangou, Stewart, Rifkin and Murray2002). It is biologically plausible that being born with an immature nervous system increases the risk of developing psychopathology (Nosarti et al., Reference Nosarti, Reichenberg, Murray, Cnattingius, Lambe, Yin and Hultman2012). An immature nervous system may be more vulnerable to stressors, and it may be harder for parents to correctly interpret the cues from their preterm babies.

There are notable strengths in our study, such as the large sample size and the rich measurements of both psychopathology and personality traits. Some limitations also need to be acknowledged. First, all included traits were reported by mothers, rendering shared method bias possible (Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Lee, & Podsakoff, Reference Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Lee and Podsakoff2003). Our sensitivity analyses on a subset of the data indicated that rater bias was likely. We recommend that all researchers wanting to conduct family studies on MoBa data have this in mind, although further studies are required to conclude.

The anxiety measure had a low Cronbach's α value (0.48) and had a lower association with p than expected. This is possibly due to this instrument being constructed to measure a variety of anxiety disorders but could also be due to unreliability.

The recruitment rate in MoBa is low (41%; Magnus et al., Reference Magnus, Irgens, Haug, Nystad, Skjaerven and Stoltenberg2006), and it has been found that women in MoBa differ from other childbearing women in Norway on several exposures and outcomes (Nilsen et al., Reference Nilsen, Vollset, Gjessing, Skjaerven, Melve, Schreuder and Magnus2009). It is possible that women with severe psychopathology symptoms did not participate, and that the children of these mothers differ from children of participating mothers on psychopathology or personality traits. However, the children, which are the focus of the present study, have not self-selected into the study, which may give less bias in this generation than in the parent generation. When recruitment rates are low, bias typically occur in estimates of prevalence, and not in estimates of associations (Nilsen et al., Reference Nilsen, Vollset, Gjessing, Skjaerven, Melve, Schreuder and Magnus2009).

Analysis indicated that gestational age was associated with both the p factor and residual factors. However, the standard errors of these estimates were high, indicating that these estimates are uncertain. Many variables are associated with SGA and gestational age, such as characteristics about the mothers (weight, medical history, smoking, etc.; McCowan & Horgan, Reference McCowan and Horgan2009). Thus, the mechanism explaining the correlation between the p factor and gestational age require further study.

The bifactor rotation of the included traits provides just one of many possible factor structures of childhood psychopathology and personality. As this was an exploratory study, we did not test other solutions. The bifactor rotation is a common and recommended practice for studies on the hierarchical structure of psychopathology (Levin-Aspenson et al., Reference Levin-Aspenson, Watson, Clark and Zimmerman2020).

To sum up, this study extends previous findings on the nature and etiology of general psychopathology in middle childhood. Personality can be meaningfully placed within a joint structure of psychopathology risk in this age group. The psychometric properties, high heritability of p, and its associations with established developmental risk factors lend support to the usefulness of this construct.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291723000077.

Acknowledgements

The work was supported by a grant from the Medicine, Health Sciences and Biology Programme at the Norwegian Research Council (Grant Numbers 231105, 262177 and 288083). TE is part-funded by a program grant from the UK Medical Research Council (MR/V012878/1), and by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR), the Biomedical Research Centre at South London, Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King's College London. The views expressed are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health. TAM is supported by a Wellcome Trust Senior Research Fellowship (Grant Number 220382/Z/20/Z). TR is supported by the Academy of Finland (Grant Numbers 334057 and 335901). ER is supported by the Norwegian Research Council (grant 320709 and 31483). The Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study is supported by the Norwegian Ministry of Health and Care Services and the Ministry of Education and Research. We are grateful to all the participating families in Norway who take part in this on-going cohort study.

Conflict of interest

The authors have no conflicts of interest.

Footnotes

This article has been updated since its original publication. A notice detailing the changes can be found here: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291723000879

*

These authors have contributed equal as senior/last authors.

References

Achenbach, T. M. (1992). Manual for the child behavior checklist/2-3 and 1992 profile. Burlington, VT: Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont.Google Scholar
Akaike, H. (1987). Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrica, 52, 317332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allegrini, A. G., Cheesman, R., Rimfeld, K., Selzam, S., Pingault, J.-B., Eley, T. C., & Plomin, R. (2020). The p factor: Genetic analyses support a general dimension of psychopathology in childhood and adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 61(1), 3039. doi:https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13113CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Angold, A., Costello, E. J., Messer, S. C., Pickles, A., Winder, F., & Silver, D. (1995). Development of a short questionnaire for use in epidemiological studies of depression in children and adolescents. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 5(4), 237249.Google Scholar
Ask, H., Gustavson, K., Ystrom, E., Havdahl, K. A., Tesli, M., Askeland, R. B., & Reichborn-Kjennerud, T. (2018). Association of gestational age at birth with symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children. JAMA Pediatrics, 172(8), 749756. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2018.1315CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. (2009). Exploratory structural equation modeling. Structural Equation Modeling, 16, 397438. doi:10.1080/10705510903008204CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 107(2), 238246. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernaards, C. A., & Jennrich, R. I. (2005). Gradient projection algorithms and software for arbitrary rotation criteria in factor analysis. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 65(5), 676696. doi:10.1177/0013164404272507CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Birmaher, B., Khetarpal, S., Brent, D., Cully, M., Balach, L., Kaufman, J., & Neer, S. M. (1997). The screen for child anxiety related emotional disorders (SCARED): Scale construction and psychometric characteristics. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(4), 545553. doi:10.1097/00004583-199704000-00018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blonigen, D. M., Hicks, B. M., Krueger, R. F., Patrick, C. J., & Iacono, W. G. (2005). Psychopathic personality traits: Heritability and genetic overlap with internalizing and externalizing psychopathology. Psychological Medicine, 35(5), 637648. doi:10.1017/s0033291704004180CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonifay, W., Lane, S. P., & Reise, S. P. (2016). Three concerns with applying a bifactor model as a structure of psychopathology. Clinical Psychological Science, 5(1), 184186. doi:10.1177/2167702616657069CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bouchard, T. J. Jr., & McGue, M. (2003). Genetic and environmental influences on human psychological differences. Journal of Neurobiology, 54(1), 445. doi:10.1002/neu.10160CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carey, G. (1986). Sibling imitation and contrast effects. Behavior Genetics, 16(3), 319341.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A., Houts, R. M., Belsky, D. W., Goldman-Mellor, S. J., Harrington, H., Israel, S., … Moffitt, T. E. (2014). The p factor: One general psychopathology factor in the structure of psychiatric disorders? Clinical Psychological Science, 2(2), 119137. doi:10.1177/2167702613497473CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cosgrove, V. E., Rhee, S. H., Gelhorn, H. L., Boeldt, D., Corley, R. C., Ehringer, M. A., … Hewitt, J. K. (2011). Structure and etiology of co-occurring internalizing and externalizing disorders in adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39(1), 109123. doi:10.1007/s10802-010-9444-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cronbach, L. J., & Meehl, P. E. (1955). Construct validity in psychological tests. Psychological Bulletin, 52(4), 281302. doi:10.1037/h0040957CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Czajkowski, N., Aggen, S. H., Krueger, R. F., Kendler, K. S., Neale, M. C., Knudsen, G. P., … Reichborn-Kjennerud, T. (2018). A twin study of normative personality and DSM-IV personality disorder criterion counts: Evidence for separate genetic influences. American Journal of Psychiatry, 175(7), 649656. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2017.17050493CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, E. P., Buss, C., Muftuler, L. T., Head, K., Hasso, A., Wing, D. A., … Sandman, C. A. (2011). Children's brain development benefits from longer gestation. Frontiers in Psychology, 2(1). doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DelGiudice, M. (2018). Middle childhood: An evolutionary-developmental synthesis. In Halfon, N., Forrest, C. B., Lerner, R. M., & Faustman, E. M. (Eds.), Handbook of life course health development (pp. 95107). Cham: Springer International Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Derks, E. M., Hudziak, J. J., & Boomsma, D. I. (2009). Genetics of ADHD, hyperactivity, and attention problems. In Kim, Y. (Ed.), Handbook of behavior genetics (pp. 361378). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fried, E. I., Greene, A. L., & Eaton, N. R. (2021). The p factor is the sum of its parts, for now. World Psychiatry, 20(1), 6970. doi:10.1002/wps.20814CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gluckman, P. D., & Hanson, M. A. (2006). The consequences of being born small – an adaptive perspective. Hormone Research, 65(Suppl 3), 514. doi:10.1159/000091500CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gluschkoff, K., Jokela, M., & Rosenström, T. (2021). General psychopathology factor and borderline personality disorder: Evidence for substantial overlap from two nationally representative surveys of US adults. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 12(1), 86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, L. R. (1990). An alternative “description of personality”: The big-five factor structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59(6), 12161229. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.59.6.1216CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gustavson, K., Torvik, F. A., Eilertsen, E. M., Ask, H., McAdams, T. A., Hannigan, L. J., … Gjerde, L. C. (2021). Genetic and environmental contributions to co-occurring ADHD and emotional problems in school-aged children. Developmental Psychology, 57(8), 13591371. doi:10.1037/dev0001229CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hack, M., Youngstrom, E. A., Cartar, L., Schluchter, M., Taylor, H. G., Flannery, D., … Borawski, E. (2004). Behavioral outcomes and evidence of psychopathology among very low birth weight infants at age 20 years. Pediatrics, 114(4), 932940. doi:10.1542/peds.2003-1017-LCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hancock, G. R., & Mueller, R. O. (2001). Rethinking construct reliability within latent variable systems. In Cudeck, R., du Toit, S., & Sörbom, D. (Eds.), Structural equation modeling: Present and future – A festschrift in honor of Karl Jöreskog (pp. 195-216). Lincolnwood, IL: Scientific Software International.Google Scholar
Horn, J. L. (1965). A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor analysis. Psychometrika, 30(2), 179185. doi:10.1007/bf02289447CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Irgens, L. M. (2000). The medical birth registry of Norway. Epidemiological research and surveillance throughout 30 years. Acta Obstetricia Et Gynecologica Scandinavica, 79(6), 435439. doi:10.1034/j.1600-0412.2000.079006435.xGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jennrich, R. I., & Bentler, P. M. (2011). Exploratory Bi-factor analysis. Psychometrika, 76(4), 537549. doi:10.1007/s11336-011-9218-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jin, F. (2016). Questions Documentation 8-year Questionnaire when the child was 8 years old. The Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study (MoBa). Mother Questionnaire. Retrieved from https://www.fhi.no/globalassets/dokumenterfiler/studier/den-norske-mor-far-og-barn--undersokelsenmoba/instrumentdokumentasjon/instrument-documentation-q-8year.pdf.Google Scholar
Kapellou, O., Counsell, S. J., Kennea, N., Dyet, L., Saeed, N., Stark, J., … Edwards, A. D. (2006). Abnormal cortical development after premature birth shown by altered allometric scaling of brain growth. PLoS Medicine, 3(8), e265. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.0030265CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, M. C., Coventry, W. L., Heath, A. C., & Martin, N. G. (2005). Widespread evidence for non-additive genetic variation in Cloninger's and Eysenck's personality dimensions using a twin plus sibling design. Behavior Genetics, 35(6), 707721. doi:10.1007/s10519-005-6041-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kotov, R., Gamez, W., Schmidt, F., & Watson, D. (2010). Linking “big” personality traits to anxiety, depressive, and substance use disorders: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136(5), 768821. doi:10.1037/a0020327CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kotov, R., Krueger, R. F., Watson, D., Achenbach, T. M., Althoff, R. R., Bagby, R. M., … Zimmerman, M. (2017). The hierarchical taxonomy of psychopathology (HiTOP): A dimensional alternative to traditional nosologies. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 126(4), 454477. doi:10.1037/abn0000258CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krueger, R. F. (1999). The structure of common mental disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56(10), 921926.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuja-Halkola, R., Lind Juto, K., Skoglund, C., Rück, C., Mataix-Cols, D., Pérez-Vigil, A., … Larsson, H. (2021). Do borderline personality disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder co-aggregate in families? A population-based study of 2 million Swedes. Molecular Psychiatry, 26(1), 341349. doi:10.1038/s41380-018-0248-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laerum, A. M. W., Reitan, S. K., Evensen, K. A. I., Lydersen, S., Brubakk, A. M., Skranes, J., & Indredavik, M. S. (2019). Psychiatric symptoms and risk factors in adults born preterm with very low birthweight or born small for gestational age at term. BMC Psychiatry, 19(1), 223. doi:10.1186/s12888-019-2202-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lahey, B. B., Moore, T. M., Kaczkurkin, A. N., & Zald, D. H. (2021). Hierarchical models of psychopathology: Empirical support, implications, and remaining issues. World Psychiatry, 20(1), 5763. doi:https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20824CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lahey, B. B., Rathouz, P. J., Van Hulle, C., Urbano, R. C., Krueger, R. F., Applegate, B., … Waldman, I. D. (2008). Testing structural models of DSM-IV symptoms of common forms of child and adolescent psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36(2), 187206. doi:10.1007/s10802-007-9169-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lahey, B. B., Van Hulle, C. A., Singh, A. L., Waldman, I. D., & Rathouz, P. J. (2011). Higher-order genetic and environmental structure of prevalent forms of child and adolescent psychopathology. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68(2), 181189. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.192CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lamb, M. E., Chuang, S. S., Wessels, H., Broberg, A. G., & Hwang, C. P. (2002). Emergence and constructvalidation of the big five factors in early childhood: A longitudinal analysis of their ontogeny in Sweden. Child Development, 73(5), 15171524. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00487CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levin-Aspenson, H. F., Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Zimmerman, M. (2020). What is the general factor of psychopathology? Consistency of the p factor across samples. Assessment, 28(4), 10351049. doi:10.1177/1073191120954921CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Magnus, P., Birke, C., Vejrup, K., Haugan, A., Alsaker, E., Daltveit, A. K., … Stoltenberg, C. (2016). Cohort profile update: The Norwegian mother and child cohort study (MoBa). International Journal of Epidemiology, 45(2), 382388. doi:10.1093/ije/dyw029.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Magnus, P., Irgens, L. M., Haug, K., Nystad, W., Skjaerven, R., & Stoltenberg, C. (2006). Cohort profile: The Norwegian mother and child cohort study (MoBa). International Journal of Epidemiology, 35(5), 11461150. doi:10.1093/ije/dyl170CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marceau, K., & Neiderhiser, J. (2022). Generalist genes and specialist environments for adolescent internalizing and externalizing problems: A test of severity and directionality. Development and Psychopathology, 34(1), 379386. doi:10.1017/s0954579420001108CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marsál, K., Persson, P. H., Larsen, T., Lilja, H., Selbing, A., & Sultan, B. (1996). Intrauterine growth curves based on ultrasonically estimated foetal weights. Acta Paediatrica, 85(7), 843848. doi:10.1111/j.1651-2227.1996.tb14164.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martel, M. M., Pan, P. M., Hoffmann, M. S., Gadelha, A., do Rosario, M. C., Mari, J. J., … Salum, G. A. (2017). A general psychopathology factor (p factor) in children: Structural model analysis and external validation through familial risk and child global executive function. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 126(1), 137148. doi:10.1037/abn0000205CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mathewson, K. J., Chow, C. H., Dobson, K. G., Pope, E. I., Schmidt, L. A., & Van Lieshout, R. J. (2017). Mental health of extremely low birth weight survivors: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 143(4), 347383. doi:10.1037/bul0000091CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCabe, G. A., Oltmanns, J. R., & Widiger, T. A. (2022). The general factors of personality disorder, psychopathology, and personality. Journal of Personality Disorders, 36(2), 129156. doi:10.1521/pedi_2021_35_530CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCowan, L., & Horgan, R. P. (2009). Risk factors for small for gestational age infants. Best Practice and Research Clinical Obstetrics and Gynaecology, 23(6), 779793. doi:10.1016/j.bpobgyn.2009.06.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1987). Validation of the five-factor model of personality across instruments and observers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(1), 8190. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.52.1.81CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, T. M., Kaczkurkin, A. N., Durham, E. L., Jeong, H. J., McDowell, M. G., Dupont, R. M., … Lahey, B. B. (2020). Criterion validity and relationships between alternative hierarchical dimensional models of general and specific psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 129(7), 677688. doi:10.1037/abn0000601CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neumann, A., Pappa, I., Lahey, B. B., Verhulst, F. C., Medina-Gomez, C., Jaddoe, V. W., … Tiemeier, H. (2016). Single nucleotide polymorphism heritability of a general psychopathology factor in children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 55(12), 10381045.e1034. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2016.09.498CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newman, D. L., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., & Silva, P. A. (1998). Comorbid mental disorders: Implications for treatment and sample selection. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107(2), 305311. doi:10.1037/0021-843x.107.2.305CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nigg, J. T. (2017). Annual research review: On the relations among self-regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(4), 361383. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12675CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nilsen, R. M., Vollset, S. E., Gjessing, H. K., Skjaerven, R., Melve, K. K., Schreuder, P., … Magnus, P. (2009). Self-selection and bias in a large prospective pregnancy cohort in Norway. Paediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology, 23(6), 597608. doi:10.1111/j.1365-3016.2009.01062.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nosarti, C., Al-Asady, M. H. S., Frangou, S., Stewart, A. L., Rifkin, L., & Murray, R. M. (2002). Adolescents who were born very preterm have decreased brain volumes. Brain, 125(7), 16161623. doi:10.1093/brain/awf157CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nosarti, C., Reichenberg, A., Murray, R. M., Cnattingius, S., Lambe, M. P., Yin, L., … Hultman, C. M. (2012). Preterm birth and psychiatric disorders in young adult life. Archives of General Psychiatry, 69(6), 610617. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.1374CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olino, T. M., Bufferd, S. J., Dougherty, L. R., Dyson, M. W., Carlson, G. A., & Klein, D. N. (2018). The development of latent dimensions of psychopathology across early childhood: Stability of dimensions and moderators of change. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 46(7), 13731383. doi:10.1007/s10802-018-0398-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ozer, D. J., & Benet-Martínez, V. (2006). Personality and the prediction of consequential outcomes. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 401421. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190127CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pettersson, E., Larsson, H., D'Onofrio, B., Almqvist, C., & Lichtenstein, P. (2019). Association of fetal growth with general and specific mental health conditions. Jama Psychiatry, 76(5), 536543. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.4342CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J. Y., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(5), 879903. doi:10.1037/0021-9101.88.5.879CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodriguez, A., Reise, S. P., & Haviland, M. G. (2016). Evaluating bifactor models: Calculating and interpreting statistical indices. Psychological Methods, 21(2), 137150. doi:10.1037/met0000045CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenström, T., Gjerde, L. C., Krueger, R. F., Aggen, S. H., Czajkowski, N. O., Gillespie, N. A., … Ystrom, E. (2018). Joint factorial structure of psychopathology and personality. Psychological Medicine, 49(13), 21582167. doi:10.1017/S0033291718002982.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Samuel, D. B., & Widiger, T. A. (2008). A meta-analytic review of the relationships between the five-factor model and DSM-IV-TR personality disorders: A facet level analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 28(8), 13261342. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2008.07.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwarz, G. (1978). Estimating the dimension of a model. The Annals of Statistics, 6(2), 461464, 464. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1214/aos/1176344136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shields, A. N., Giljen, M., España, R. A., & Tackett, J. L. (2021). The p factor and dimensional structural models of youth personality pathology and psychopathology. Current Opinion in Psychology, 37, 2125. doi:10.1016/j.copsyc.2020.06.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silva, R. R., Alpert, M., Pouget, E., Silva, V., Trosper, S., Reyes, K., & Dummit, S. (2005). A rating scale for disruptive behavior disorders, based on the DSM-IV item pool. Psychiatric Quarterly, 76(4), 327339. doi:10.1007/s11126-005-4966-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Hervas, A., Silberg, J. L., Rutter, M., & Eaves, L. (1998). Genetic influences on childhood hyperactivity: Contrast effects imply parental rating bias, not sibling interaction. Psychological Medicine, 28(4), 825837. doi:10.1017/s0033291798006886CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slobodskaya, H. R. (2014). The hierarchical structure of personality and common psychopathology in childhood. Journal of Research in Personality, 53, 3646. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2014.08.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steiger, J. H. (1990). Structural model evaluation and modification: An interval estimation approach. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 25(2), 173180. doi:10.1207/s15327906mbr2502_4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tackett, J. L., Lahey, B. B., van Hulle, C., Waldman, I., Krueger, R. F., & Rathouz, P. J. (2013). Common genetic influences on negative emotionality and a general psychopathology factor in childhood and adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122(4), 11421153. doi:10.1037/a0034151CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vollrath, M. E., Hampson, S. E., & Torgersen, S. (2016). Constructing a short form of the hierarchical personality inventory for children (HiPIC): The HiPIC-30. Personality and Mental Health, 10(2), 152165. doi:10.1002/pmh.1334CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waldman, I. D., Poore, H. E., van Hulle, C., Rathouz, P. J., & Lahey, B. B. (2016). External validity of a hierarchical dimensional model of child and adolescent psychopathology: Tests using confirmatory factor analyses and multivariate behavior genetic analyses. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125(8), 10531066. doi:10.1037/abn0000183CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walhovd, K. B., Fjell, A. M., Brown, T. T., Kuperman, J. M., Chung, Y., Hagler, D. J., … Dale, A. M. (2012). Long-term influence of normal variation in neonatal characteristics on human brain development. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(49), 2008920094. doi:10.1073/pnas.1208180109CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waszczuk, M. A., Hopwood, C. J., Luft, B. J., Morey, L. C., Perlman, G., Ruggero, C. J., … Kotov, R. (2021). The prognostic utility of personality traits versus past psychiatric diagnoses: Predicting future mental health and functioning. Clinical Psychological Science, 10(4), 734751. doi:10.1177/21677026211056596CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Widiger, T. A. (2011). Personality and psychopathology. World Psychiatry, 10(2), 103106. doi:https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2051-5545.2011.tb00024.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Widiger, T. A., Sellbom, M., Chmielewski, M., Clark, L. A., DeYoung, C. G., Kotov, R., … Wright, A. G. C. (2019). Personality in a hierarchical model of psychopathology. Clinical Psychological Science, 7(1), 7792. doi:10.1177/2167702618797105CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolke, D., Johnson, S., & Mendonça, M. (2019). The life course consequences of very preterm birth. Annual Review of Developmental Psychology, 1(1), 6992. doi:10.1146/annurev-devpsych-121318-084804CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Psychometric and goodness of fit indices for different factor solutions on the phenotypic exploratory factor analysis models with bifactor rotation

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Correlations between included variables.Note: Gray cross indicates correlations not significant (p > 0.05).

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Best fitting EFA models.Note: Loadings below 0.20 are not shown in Fig. 2a. A = Broad-sense genetic influences (including both additive and non-additive/dominance effects); E = unique environmental influences and measurement error.

Figure 3

Table 2. Model fit statistics from bifactor exploratory factor analyses

Figure 4

Fig. 3. Predicted latent factor scores for different levels of gestational age. The figure shows the association between gestational age and p, as well as between gestational age and the residual latent factors (F2–F4), expressed as predicted factor scores for different levels of gestational age. Factors have mean = 0 and standard deviation of 1. The vertical lines index gestational age 40 (term), 37 (early term), 32 (very preterm) and 28 weeks (extremely preterm). Dotted lines are predicted factor scores for children born small for gestational age.

Supplementary material: File

Gjerde et al. supplementary material

Gjerde et al. supplementary material

Download Gjerde et al. supplementary material(File)
File 157.8 KB