Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-pfhbr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-11T02:17:24.471Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Response to the commentary by Bankart and Brugha

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 December 2021

Yi-Ling Chien*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, National Taiwan University Hospital and College of Medicine, Taipei, Taiwan
*
Author for correspondence: Yi-Ling Chien, E-mail: ylchien@hotmail.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Invited Letter Rejoinder
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Dear Editor,

In response to the commentary by Bankart and Brugha (Reference Bankart and Brugha2021), we appreciate the opportunity to address the concerns raised by the authors.

To clarify, the research summarized in Chien et al. (Reference Chien, Tsai, Chen, Yang, Gau, Soong and Chiu2021) was a single-site study, utilizing an evidence-based, manualized intervention protocol (Laugeson, Reference Laugeson2017), which was translated into Mandarin Chinese and culturally modified with the developer's permission, and ultimately published in Mandarin Chinese (ISBN 978-986-357-130-8). Treatment was delivered to fidelity by the intervention team following the published intervention protocol.

The trial is registered on ClinicalTrial.gov (NCT05054829). The study protocol was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of National Taiwan University Hospital (no. 201612185RINC). The primary outcome of the study was social functioning assessed by the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS). The significance level was set at p < 0.05. The outcome was evaluated by as-treated analysis.

As for “minimum clinically important” effect size, there is a lack of research standards to date, particularly for psychotherapeutic approaches (Cuijpers, Turner, Koole, van Dijke & Smit, Reference Cuijpers, Turner, Koole, van Dijke and Smit2014). Lemieux et al. (Reference Lemieux, Beaton, Hogg-Johnson, Bordeleau and Goodwin2007) demonstrated that multiplying the pooled baseline standard deviation (s.d.) by 0.2 as the minimal clinically important difference corresponds to the smallest effect size. Cuijpers et al. (Reference Cuijpers, Turner, Koole, van Dijke and Smit2014) suggested a tentative clinical relevance cutoff of standardized mean difference (effect size) 0.24 for psychotherapeutic intervention. As for social skill training in autism spectrum disorder (ASD), previous studies suggested that social skill training improves social deficits in ASD children, with effect sizes in the medium range (d = 0.47–0.51). A recent meta-analysis of in-person and technological interventions showed that the effect sizes of face-to-face social skills training are in the medium to high range (g = 0.81) (Soares et al., Reference Soares, Bausback, Beard, Higinbotham, Bunge and Gengoux2021). Yet, effect sizes may vary with assessment tools and clinical populations, such as age and intellect. Unlike the abundant treatment trials for ASD children, adult intervention research is still limited. Thus, more research is certainly needed to define the clinical relevance cutoffs for social skills training outcomes in ASD adults. Adopting two clinical importance criteria proposed by Cuijpers et al. (Reference Cuijpers, Turner, Koole, van Dijke and Smit2014) (effect size d > 0.24) and Lemieux et al. (Reference Lemieux, Beaton, Hogg-Johnson, Bordeleau and Goodwin2007) (mean differences > pooled baseline s.d. multiplied by 0.2; i.e., SRS total scores: self-reported 28.7 × 0.2 = 5.74, coach-reported 22.1 × 0.2 = 4.42), social functioning measured on the SRS total and social communication subscale in this study (effect size: self-reported d = 0.41>0.24, coach-reported d = 0.73>0.24; mean difference: self-reported 11.7 > 5.74, coach-reported 15.8 > 4.42) were considered as clinical important differences following the social skill training.

With regard to the randomization procedures, simple randomization (rather than block randomization) was applied within each stratum (i.e. male or female). For allocation of the participants, we used a computer-generated list of random numbers. Participants of each sex were randomly assigned to the PEERS intervention group or treatment-as-usual control group following simple randomization procedures. As for implementation, the research team enrolled participants based on eligibility criteria (Chien et al., Reference Chien, Tsai, Chen, Yang, Gau, Soong and Chiu2021), while a trained research assistant generated the allocation sequence and randomized participants using the above procedures.

As for the adjustment made to the familywise error rate, for the main findings of mixed model summarized in original Table 2 (Chien et al., Reference Chien, Tsai, Chen, Yang, Gau, Soong and Chiu2021), if Bonferroni correction was adopted (significance threshold 0.05/15 = 0.0033), self-reported SRS social emotion, SRS total scores, Empathy Quotient total scores, social skills knowledge, and communication and social reciprocity of the autism diagnostic observation schedule passed the corrected threshold. If adjusted by false discovery rate (FDR), most domains remained significant (FDR q < 0.05) except self-reported AQ total scores, SRS social awareness and coach-reported SRS total scores.

For sample size and power calculation, in the mixed model with repeated measures, a total sample of 82 had statistical power at 0.8 to detect group differences with an effect size d > 0.27 (α < 0.05). For paired t test, 41 pairs of follow-up data had statistical power at 0.8 to detect group differences with effect size d > 0.45. Thus, this sample size may be adequate to detect medium to large effects, but is underpowered to detect small effect differences (e.g. d < 0.27 in mixed model or d < 0.45 in paired t test). Thus, we agree that the findings may be presented as preliminary and provide the grounds for a larger, definitive study.

We appreciate the comments, and would like to echo that autism researchers work in collaboration with people with autism to engage in a joint effort to seek greater consensus on intervention outcome prioritization.

Conflict of interest

The author declares no competing interest.

References

Bankart, J., & Brugha, T. (2021). Social skills interventions in young adults with autism spectrum disorder: Comment on Chien et al. (2021). Psychological Medicine, 12, 12. doi: 10.1017/S0033291721003998, Online ahead of print.Google Scholar
Chien, Y. L., Tsai, W. C., Chen, W. H., Yang, C. L., Gau, S. S., Soong, W. T., … Chiu, Y. N. (2021). Effectiveness, durability, and clinical correlates of the PEERS social skills intervention in young adults with autism spectrum disorder: The first evidence outside North America. Psychological Medicine, 111. doi: 10.1017/S0033291721002385, Online ahead of print.Google ScholarPubMed
Cuijpers, P., Turner, E., Koole, S., van Dijke, A., & Smit, F. (2014). What is the threshold for a clinically relevant effect? The case of major depressive disorders. Depression and Anxiety, 31(5), 374378. doi: 10.1002/da.22249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laugeson, E. (2017). PEERS® for young adults: Social skills training for adults with autism spectrum disorder and other social challenges. New York: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lemieux, J., Beaton, D. E., Hogg-Johnson, S., Bordeleau, L. J., & Goodwin, P. J. (2007). Three methods for minimally important difference: No relationship was found with the net proportion of patients improving. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 60(5), 448455. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2006.08.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soares, E. E., Bausback, K., Beard, C. L., Higinbotham, M., Bunge, E. L., & Gengoux, G. W. (2021). Social skills training for autism spectrum disorder: A meta-analysis of in-person and technological interventions. Journal of Technology in Behavioral Science, 6, 166180. doi: 10.1007/s41347-020-00177-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed