Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-p2v8j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-30T21:30:47.530Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Experimental studies of paranoid thinking in clinical and nonclinical populations: a systematic review and meta-analysis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 July 2023

Lyn Ellett*
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Southampton, UK
Filippo Varese
Affiliation:
Division of Psychology and Mental Health, School of Health Sciences, Manchester Academic Health Science Centre, University of Manchester, UK Complex Trauma and Resilience Research Unit, Greater Manchester Mental Health NHS Foundation Trust, UK
Jane Owens
Affiliation:
Division of Psychology and Mental Health, School of Health Sciences, Manchester Academic Health Science Centre, University of Manchester, UK NHS Lothian, Edinburgh, UK
Sonya Rafiq
Affiliation:
Division of Psychology and Mental Health, School of Health Sciences, Manchester Academic Health Science Centre, University of Manchester, UK Complex Trauma and Resilience Research Unit, Greater Manchester Mental Health NHS Foundation Trust, UK
Georgia Penn
Affiliation:
Division of Psychology and Mental Health, School of Health Sciences, Manchester Academic Health Science Centre, University of Manchester, UK
Katherine Berry
Affiliation:
Division of Psychology and Mental Health, School of Health Sciences, Manchester Academic Health Science Centre, University of Manchester, UK Complex Trauma and Resilience Research Unit, Greater Manchester Mental Health NHS Foundation Trust, UK
*
Corresponding author: Dr Lyn Ellett; Email: L.A.Ellett@soton.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Paranoia is common in clinical and nonclinical populations, consistent with continuum models of psychosis. A number of experimental studies have been conducted that attempt to induce, manipulate or measure paranoid thinking in both clinical and nonclinical populations, which is important to understand causal mechanisms and advance psychological interventions. Our aim was to conduct a systematic review and meta-analysis of experimental studies (non-sleep, non-drug paradigms) on psychometrically assessed paranoia in clinical and nonclinical populations. The review was conducted using PRISMA guidelines. Six databases (PsycINFO, PubMed, EMBASE, Web of Science, Medline and AMED) were searched for peer-reviewed experimental studies using within and between-subject designs to investigate paranoia in clinical and nonclinical populations. Effect sizes for each study were calculated using Hedge's g and were integrated using a random effect meta-analysis model. Thirty studies were included in the review (total n = 3898), which used 13 experimental paradigms to induce paranoia; 10 studies set out to explicitly induce paranoia, and 20 studies induced a range of other states. Effect sizes for individual studies ranged from 0.03 to 1.55. Meta-analysis found a significant summary effect of 0.51 [95% confidence interval 0.37–0.66, p < 0.001], indicating a medium effect of experimental paradigms on paranoia. Paranoia can be induced and investigated using a wide range of experimental paradigms, which can inform decision-making about which paradigms to use in future studies, and is consistent with cognitive, continuum and evolutionary models of paranoia.

Type
Review Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Paranoia, or persecutory ideation, occurs when a person believes they are under intentional threat of harm from others (Freeman & Garety, Reference Freeman and Garety2000). While paranoid delusions are often cited as the most commonly experienced form of delusional thinking within clinical populations, substantial evidence suggests that paranoid thinking commonly occurs in the general population (Garety, Everitt, & Hemsley, Reference Garety, Everitt and Hemsley1988; Ellett, Lopes, & Chadwick, Reference Ellett, Lopes and Chadwick2003; Bebbington et al., Reference Bebbington, McBride, Steel, Kuipers, Radovanovic, Brugha and Freeman2013; Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Loe, Kingdon, Startup, Molodynski, Rosebrock and Bird2021). This is consistent with current dimensional models of mental ill-health (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Houts, Belsky, Goldman-Mellor, Harrington, Israel and Moffitt2013). A number of psychosocial models have been proposed that place varying emphasis on the importance of developmental, cognitive, behavioural, affective and interpersonal factors involved in the formation and maintenance of persecutory beliefs. Cognitive models emphasise the importance of the interpretation of anomalous events, and how these interpretations are influenced by factors such as previous life experience, attentional and attributional biases and affective states (Garety, Kuipers, Fowler, Freeman, & Bebbington, Reference Garety, Kuipers, Fowler, Freeman and Bebbington2001; Morrison, Reference Morrison2001; Freeman, Garety, Kuipers, Fowler, & Bebbington, Reference Freeman, Garety, Kuipers, Fowler and Bebbington2002; Bentall, Corcoran, Howard, Blackwood, & Kinderman, Reference Bentall, Corcoran, Howard, Blackwood and Kinderman2011).

Psychological research using methodologies for inducing and/or measuring paranoid thinking in both clinical and nonclinical samples offers a unique approach to the study of paranoia. These methodologies have been used to test theoretical models of paranoia by investigating factors that buffer or attenuate experiences of paranoia (Ellett & Chadwick, Reference Ellett and Chadwick2007; Lincoln, Hohenhaus, & Hartmann, Reference Lincoln, Hohenhaus and Hartmann2013). However, they have been used most extensively in analogue samples, which provide a convenient and acceptable means of testing psychosocial models of paranoia given dimensional models of mental ill-health and paranoia in particular. Their use in clinical samples is relatively less well developed but an important area of research, most notably, in testing the role of potential buffers and protective factors in relation to paranoid thinking. Despite the popularity of paranoia induction methodologies, there is currently no meta-analysis of experimental studies of paranoia which can guide researchers in designing and evaluating research in this area. Two recent systematic reviews, one investigating the role of anxiety in paranoia and one investigating a range of causal mechanisms in delusions and hallucinations, included a review of studies using experimental procedures to manipulate paranoia (Bennetts, Stopa, & Newman-Taylor, Reference Bennetts, Stopa and Newman-Taylor2021; Brown, Waite, & Freeman, Reference Brown, Waite and Freeman2019). However, these reviews did not meta-analyse the effects of the experimental procedures on outcomes, which is important to establish the size of the effects. The aim of the current review was to summarise the existing literature on experimental paradigms that have been used to study paranoid thinking in both clinical and nonclinical populations. Given the proposed structure of paranoia as potentially including both persecutory ideation and/or social reference (Bebbington et al., Reference Bebbington, McBride, Steel, Kuipers, Radovanovic, Brugha and Freeman2013; Bell & O'Driscoll, Reference Bell and Driscoll2018), we chose to focus on the facet of paranoid thinking specifically. Additionally, given our main focus was to summarise the literature on experimental studies that measured paranoia as an outcome, we did not use a specific theoretical model to guide our decision-making about which paradigms to include, as this could have potentially limited the scope of studies included in the review. The current systematic review and meta-analysis will, therefore, address the following central questions: (1) What experimental paradigms have been used to induce or study paranoia?; (2) How effective are these experimental paradigms?; (3) What factors mediate and moderate the effect of experimental paradigms on paranoia?

Method

Six databases (PsycINFO, PubMed, EMBASE, Web of Science, Medline and AMED) were searched up to December 2021. Abstracts and titles were searched for the following: (persecution OR persecutory OR paranoid OR paranoia OR suspiciousness OR suspicious thoughts) AND (experimental OR manipulation OR manipulated OR induction OR induced OR paradigm).

Inclusion criteria for the review were as follows: (1) studies that used an experimental design to induce, manipulate or measure paranoid thinking; (2) clinical or nonclinical population and (3) studies that assessed paranoia using a psychometric measure. Exclusion criteria for the review were as follows: (1) studies that measured factors conceptually related to but different from paranoia, such as reasoning biases, attributions of harmful intent or other types of delusions or hallucinations, because the focus of the review was on the assessment of paranoia using psychometric measures; (2) studies where the sample included participants under 18 years old; (3) studies assessing suspiciousness of experimental procedures; (4) experimental studies of drug or sleep induced paranoia, or the effect of hearing deficits because the focus here was on psychological approaches; (5) studies that did not include a reliable and valid measure of paranoia, such as unstandardized visual analogue scales, interview-based ratings or single scale items; (6) studies that used correlational or cross-over designs due to the risk of carryover effects; (7) studies that did not provide sufficient data to enable us to calculate effects sizes (in the case of within subjects designs pre- and post-induction scores on a measure of paranoia and in the case of between subjects’ designs post-induction scores on measure of paranoia for an experimental and control group within or without adjustments for baseline scores) (Martin, Reference Martin1970; Cook & Perrin, Reference Cook and Perrin1971; Locascio & Snyder, Reference Locascio and Snyder1975; Zimbardo, Andersen, & Kabat, Reference Zimbardo, Andersen and Kabat1981; Horvat, Reference Horvat1986; Casanova, Katkovsky, & Hershberger, Reference Casanova, Katkovsky and Hershberger1988; Green, Nuechterlein, & Breitmeyer, Reference Green, Nuechterlein and Breitmeyer1995; Couzoulis-Mayfrank et al., Reference Couzoulis-Mayfrank, Heekeren, Neukirch, Stoll, Stock, Obradovic and Kovar2005; Kahn-Greene, Killgore, Kamimori, Balkin, & Killgore, Reference Kahn-Greene, Killgore, Kamimori, Balkin and Killgore2007; Mason, Morgan, Stefanovic, & Curran, Reference Mason, Morgan, Stefanovic and Curran2008). Where data were not reported in the papers, we attempted to obtain this from the authors.

Of the studies that fulfilled our inclusion criteria, only English language and peer-reviewed articles were included, and no restriction was placed on the year of publication. Following the exclusion of duplicate articles, each paper was assessed at either title, abstract or full text level to determine suitability. Some studies were excluded from the review for multiple reasons, making it difficult to allocate each study to a single reason for exclusion. However, the most common reasons for exclusion were not including a post-manipulation or validated measure of paranoia, not employing an experimental design, use of a biological paradigm (e.g. sleep and drug studies), irrelevant topic, not published in the English language and case studies. The review included 30 papers, and Fig. 1 summarises the search process.

Figure 1. Search results (Figure adapted from guidelines set out by the PRISMA group).

Effect size data extraction and statistical analyses

CMA Version 3 was used for all statistical analyses. All effect size computations were conducted using CMA except for the within-subject effects for which no pre–post correlation was available. For studies employing a between-subject/independent group design, effect sizes of the d-family were calculated using standard computational approaches from reported post-manipulation means, standard deviations and sample sizes (Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins, & Rothstein, Reference Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins and Rothstein2009). For studies employing a within-subject/paired group design, effect sizes were calculated based on pre- and post-manipulation means, standard deviations and sample size as well as the reported correlation coefficient between pre- and post-manipulation scores (Borenstein et al., Reference Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins and Rothstein2009). When the correlation coefficient was not reported, the review team requested the missing information by contacting the study authors. To maximise the number of effects included in the planned analyses, when study authors were unable to provide the missing correlation coefficient, we employed the effect size calculation approach (Hirst, Cragg, & Allen, Reference Hirst, Cragg and Allen2018). When studies reported the effect of paranoia induction manipulations in multiple, independent groups, effect sizes were calculated for each independent sample and included in the analyses as separate effects. In all cases, the Hedges' correction was applied to the computed effects ahead of statistical integration via a random-effect meta-analysis.

Effect sizes (i.e. Hedges' g) were integrated using the random effect meta-analysis. Due to the considerable heterogeneity in the design of the included studies, prior to the main analysis, we conducted a subgroup analysis to explore potential systematic differences in effect sizes of studies employing a within-subject/paired group design and those employing a between-subject/independent group design. All available effects were then considered into a single analysis to investigate the overall effect of paranoia induction procedures in the available literature. This principal analysis was followed-up through subgroup analyses to explore and summarise descriptive differences in the summary effects obtained by studies employing different paranoia induction procedures. Statistical heterogeneity was examined and quantified using the Q test and I 2 statistic, and influence analyses (one-study-removed analyses) were conducted to identify single studies associated with summary effects. Publication bias was assessed for through visual examination of funnel plots and the results of Egger's test. Egger's tests were conducted using the significance level of 0.10 instead of the conventional 0.05 due to limited studies in the meta-analysis resulting in limited power of statistical tests for funnel plot asymmetry (Egger, Smith, Schneider, & Minder, Reference Egger, Smith, Schneider and Minder1997). When appropriate, the Duval and Tweedie ‘Trim and Fill’ method was used to adjust the results for the potential impact of hypothetically missing studies (Duval & Tweedie, Reference Duval and Tweedie2000).

Results

Summary of studies

Thirty studies met criteria for inclusion in the review (total n = 3898). Preliminary data extracted from studies are presented in Table 1. Across studies, a range of paranoia measures (n = 4), induced states (n = 12) and methodological paradigms (n = 13) were used. Study designs included both between (n = 14) and within-subjects (n = 16), and studies employed a nonclinical sample (n = 26), a clinical and nonclinical sample (n = 3) or a clinical sample only (n = 1). Clinical samples included patients with psychotic disorders, persecutory delusions, anxiety disorders or people at risk of developing psychosis.

Table 1. Study Characteristics and Effect Sizes

Methodological paradigms

Two of the authors (L.E. and K.B.) independently grouped the studies based on methodological paradigm employed, resulting in 100% agreement for the following groupings: Cyberball (k = 7; Westermann, Kesting, & Lincoln, Reference Westermann, Kesting and Lincoln2012; Kesting, Bredenpohl, Klenke, Westermann, & Lincoln, Reference Kesting, Bredenpohl, Klenke, Westermann and Lincoln2013; Kaltsi, Bucci, & Morrison, Reference Kaltsi, Bucci and Morrison2018; Lincoln, Sundag, Schlier, & Karow, Reference Lincoln, Sundag, Schlier and Karow2018; Sundag, Ascone, & Lincoln, Reference Sundag, Ascone and Lincoln2018; Butler, Berry, Ellett, & Bucci, Reference Butler, Berry, Ellett and Bucci2019; Hepper, Wildschut, Sedikides, Robertson, & Routledge, Reference Hepper, Wildschut, Sedikides, Robertson and Routledge2021), imagery (k = 5; Bullock, Newman-Taylor, & Stopa, Reference Bullock, Newman-Taylor and Stopa2016; Bennetts, Stopa, & Newman-Taylor, Reference Bennetts, Stopa and Newman-Taylor2020; Sood & Newman-Taylor, Reference Sood and Newman-Taylor2020; Sood, Carnelley, & Newman-Taylor, Reference Sood, Carnelley and Newman-Taylor2021; Newman-Taylor, Sood, Rowe, & Carnelley, Reference Newman-Taylor, Sood, Rowe and Carnelley2021), attentional focus (k = 6; Bodner & Mikulincer, Reference Bodner and Mikulincer1998; Ellett & Chadwick, Reference Ellett and Chadwick2007; Prévost et al. Reference Prévost, Rodier, Lionnet, Brodeur, King and Debruille2011; Kingston & Ellett, Reference Kingston and Ellett2014; Flower, Newman-Taylor, & Stopa, Reference Flower, Newman-Taylor and Stopa2015; Hutton, Ellett, & Berry, Reference Hutton, Ellett and Berry2017), virtual reality (k = 2; Veling, Counotte, Pot-Kolder, van Os, & van der Gaag, Reference Veling, Counotte, Pot-Kolder, van Os and van der Gaag2016; Soflau & David, Reference Soflau and David2019), videos (k = 2; Lopes & Jaspal, Reference Lopes and Jaspal2015; Sellers, Wells, & Morrison, Reference Sellers, Wells and Morrison2018), situational recall of a loneliness experience (k = 1; Gollwitzer et al., Reference Gollwitzer, Wilczynska and Jaya2018); manipulated questionnaire with feedback (k = 1; Lamster, Nittel, Rief, Mehl, & Lincoln, Reference Lamster, Nittel, Rief, Mehl and Lincoln2017); exposure to anxiety pictures (k = 1; Lincoln, Lange, Burau, Exner, & Moritz, Reference Lincoln, Lange, Burau, Exner and Moritz2010), stories (k = 1; Boden & Berenbaum, Reference Boden and Berenbaum2007), cognitive coping strategy task (k = 1; Martinelli, Cavanagh, & Dudley, Reference Martinelli, Cavanagh and Dudley2013); exposure to urban environment (k = 1; Ellett, Freeman, & Garety, Reference Ellett, Freeman and Garety2008), goal induction (k = 1 Marr, Thau, Aquino, & Barclay, Reference Marr, Thau, Aquino and Barclay2012) and computer game (k = 1; Lopes & Pinto-Gouveia, Reference Lopes and Pinto-Gouveia2012).

Induced states

In total, 10 studies set out to explicitly induce paranoia, and 20 studies induced a range of other states, including social exclusion (k = 7); loneliness (k = 2); social stress (k = 2); attachment (k = 2); anxiety (k = 1); emotional awareness (k = 1); failure (k = 1); relationship threat (k = 1); metacognitive beliefs (k = 1); irrational beliefs (k = 1) and rumination (k = 1).

Paranoia measures

A range of measures were used across the studies to measure paranoia including original or state version of the Paranoia Checklist (Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Garety, Bebbington, Smith, Rollinson, Fowler and Dunn2005) (k = 14), state paranoia as originally reported in Bodner and Mikluciner (Reference Bodner and Mikulincer1998) (k = 8), state social paranoia scale (Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Pugh, Green, Valmaggia, Dunn and Garety2007) (n = 6), Paranoia Scale (Fenigstein & Vanable, Reference Fenigstein and Vanable1992) (n = 2) and a work-adapted version of the Paranoia Scale (Marr et al., Reference Marr, Thau, Aquino and Barclay2012) (n = 1) (note n for paranoia measures is 31 as one study used two different measures).

Quality appraisal of included studies

We used three criteria that we deemed important to the integrity of experimental paradigms to assess the quality of the studies included in the review: design, sensitivity of measures to change and representativeness of the sample. We then rated each study high, medium or low risk of bias in terms of each of these three criteria Design (high risk = groups not randomised; medium risk = groups randomised; low risk = within subjects design); Sensitivity to change, based on whether a state as opposed to a trait paranoia measure was used (high risk = trait measure used, low risk = state measure used) and representativeness of the sample (high risk = student population, medium risk = general population or clinical, not randomly selected, low risk = general population or clinical, randomly selected). See Table 2 for a breakdown of scoring.

Table 2. Quality Appraisal of Studies

Overall, the quality of studies included in the review was good, with the majority of the studies using either a randomised or within subjects design and nearly all studies using a state measure of paranoia. Studies were, however, weaker in terms of the representativeness of the sample using either student samples or non-randomly selected general population or clinical samples.

Meta-analysis findings

The subgroup analysis conducted ahead of planned primary analyses to investigate potential differences in effect sizes for studies with different designs indicated that the effects observed in between-subject/independent group studies [k = 16; Hedges' g = 0.58, 95% confidence interval (CI) 0.32–0.83] were not significantly different from those estimated for within-subject/paired group studies (k = 25; Hedges' g = 0.48, 95% CI 0.29–0.67; Q test = 0.39, p = 0.54). The analysis to evaluate the overall effect of paranoia induction procedures included effects extracted from 41 separate samples; the results are displayed in Fig. 2. The analysis found a significant summary effect of 0.51 [95% CI 0.37–0.66, p < 0.001]. Based on Cohen's (Reference Cohen1988) criteria, this indicates that experimental paradigms have, on average, a ‘medium’ effect on paranoia. However, the results of heterogeneity analyses highlighted high levels of statistical heterogeneity; Q(40) = 212.60, p < 0.001; I 2 = 81.19%. Despite the inclusion of studies with an uncharacteristic, statistically significant negative effect, one-study-removed analyses did not identify any study exerting undue influence on the estimated summary effect (Boden & Berenbaum, Reference Boden and Berenbaum2007; Lopes & Pinto-Gouveia, Reference Lopes and Pinto-Gouveia2012). Inspection of funnel plots and the results of Egger's test (p = 0.26) did not suggest that the findings were affected by possible publication or other selection bias, and the application of the Trim and Fill method was, therefore, deemed unnecessary.

Figure 2. Forrest plot of the main analysis considering the overall effect of paranoia induction.

The results of subgroup analyses that explored separately the summary effects of different types of methodological paradigm are reported in Table 3.

Table 3. Subgroup Analyses Considering Specific Paranoia Induction Manipulations

Note. Heterogeneity analysis is reported only for analyses with k > 5 due to likely bias in the estimation of statistical heterogeneity in meta-analyses with limited numbers of included studies.

Mediators and moderators

A summary of mediation and moderation effects reported across studies is shown in Table 4. A number of mediators and moderators of the effect of the experimental paradigms on paranoia were tested in five studies and included negative affect, self-esteem, jumping to conclusions reasoning bias, cognitive fusion and beliefs about self and others (Lincoln et al., Reference Lincoln, Lange, Burau, Exner and Moritz2010; Kesting et al., Reference Kesting, Bredenpohl, Klenke, Westermann and Lincoln2013; Gollwitzer, Wilczynska, & Jaya, Reference Gollwitzer, Wilczynska and Jaya2018; Sood & Newman-Taylor, Reference Sood and Newman-Taylor2020; Sood et al., Reference Sood, Carnelley and Newman-Taylor2021). Studies examining causal mechanisms found evidence that the effect of social exclusion on paranoia is mediated by self-esteem (small effect; Kesting et al., Reference Kesting, Bredenpohl, Klenke, Westermann and Lincoln2013); the impact of imagery on paranoia is mediated by cognitive fusion (large effect) and negative self and other beliefs (medium effect; Sood & Newman-Taylor, Reference Sood and Newman-Taylor2020; Sood et al., Reference Sood, Carnelley and Newman-Taylor2021); the association between anxiety and paranoia is mediated by an increase in the tendency to jump to conclusions (medium effect; Lincoln et al., Reference Lincoln, Lange, Burau, Exner and Moritz2010), and the effect of loneliness on paranoia is mediated by the negative affect (medium effect; Gollwitzer et al., Reference Gollwitzer, Wilczynska and Jaya2018).

Table 4. Summary of Mediation and Moderation Effects

Note. This table summarises two of the four conditions needed for a mediation effect to occur—when the IV has a significant effect on the DV in the absence of the mediator (direct effect of condition on paranoia column) and when the effect of the IV on the DV reduces when the mediator is added to the model (indirect effect column).

Psychosis proneness was investigated as moderators in three studies (Lincoln et al., Reference Lincoln, Lange, Burau, Exner and Moritz2010; Kesting et al., Reference Kesting, Bredenpohl, Klenke, Westermann and Lincoln2013; Lamster et al., Reference Lamster, Nittel, Rief, Mehl and Lincoln2017). Findings indicated that psychosis proneness moderated the effect of anxiety and loneliness paradigms on paranoia, such that anxiety resulted in higher paranoia among individuals with high existing vulnerability (Lincoln et al., Reference Lincoln, Lange, Burau, Exner and Moritz2010). Additionally, individuals with high psychosis proneness showed a larger reduction in paranoia resulting from a decrease in loneliness (Lamster et al., Reference Lamster, Nittel, Rief, Mehl and Lincoln2017). One study found that psychosis proneness did not moderate the effect of social exclusion on paranoia (Kesting et al., Reference Kesting, Bredenpohl, Klenke, Westermann and Lincoln2013).

Discussion

In this paper, we conducted a systematic review and meta-analysis of the literature on experimental studies of paranoia, measured using established psychometric scales, with the aim of identifying experimental paradigms (non-drug and non-sleep paradigms), evaluating their effectiveness and identifying mediators and moderators of paranoia. Thirteen experimental paradigms were employed, the most common being cyberball, attentional focus and imagery, and 12 states were induced, the most common being paranoia and social exclusion. A range of paranoia measures were also used across studies, the most common being a state-adapted version of the Paranoia Checklist.

A range of antecedents of paranoia were also identified in terms of the states that were intended to be induced by the paradigms. While a third of studies (n = 10) had the explicit aim of inducing paranoia, the remaining studies (n = 20) induced other states that may lead to paranoia. These included a range of beliefs (metacognitive and irrational), emotions (anxiety, social stress, loneliness and emotional awareness), situational factors (social exclusion, attachment, failure and relationship threat) and cognitive processes (rumination) consistent with both the ABC model used in cognitive behavioural therapy and current cognitive models of psychosis (Garety et al., Reference Garety, Kuipers, Fowler, Freeman and Bebbington2001; Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Garety, Kuipers, Fowler and Bebbington2002).

Findings from the meta-analysis indicated a medium effect size for the effect of experimental paradigms on paranoia across the 30 studies. As we might expect, results from the subgroup analyses tentatively suggest that experimental studies using clinical populations (exposure to urban environment and virtual reality) had the largest effect sizes. Additionally, in studies that used nonclinical populations only, attentional focus and anxiety induction paradigms had the largest cumulative effect sizes, although the subgroup analyses need to be interpreted cautiously given the small number of studies in some experimental groupings. Overall, the evidence from the studies reviewed suggests that paranoia can be induced using a range of different methodologies, which adds weight to the conceptualisation of paranoia being both a common and evolutionarily adaptive response to perceived threat (Ellett et al., Reference Ellett, Lopes and Chadwick2003) and is consistent with continuum models of psychosis (Strauss, Reference Strauss1969; van Os, Hanssen, Bijl, & Ravelli, Reference van Os, Hanssen, Bijl and Ravelli2000).

A number of factors were shown to mediate or moderate the effects of experimental paradigms on paranoid thinking. Consistent with current models of paranoid thinking (Garety et al., Reference Garety, Everitt and Hemsley1988; Morrison, Reference Morrison2001; Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Garety, Kuipers, Fowler and Bebbington2002; Bentall et al., Reference Bentall, Corcoran, Howard, Blackwood and Kinderman2011; Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Loe, Kingdon, Startup, Molodynski, Rosebrock and Bird2021), identified mediators included negative affect, self-esteem, jumping to conclusions reasoning bias, cognitive fusion and beliefs about self and others and moderators included psychosis proneness. Consistent with the stress-vulnerability model (Zubin & Spring, Reference Zubin and Spring1977), existing psychosis proneness was shown to exacerbate the effect of experimental paradigms on state paranoia in two studies. Interestingly, and again consistent with the stress-vulnerability model, the evidence also suggests that paranoia can also be induced in those with low trait disposition when environmental threat is high.

There are a number of limitations that warrant consideration, with both the individual studies included and the review overall. In terms of individual studies, effect size calculations were not possible for studies that did not report adequate data to allow this or for uncontrolled or correlational designs, in which the analyses presented did not pertain specifically to the effectiveness of induction(s). We attempted to contact authors for missing data but not all authors responded meaning that some potentially relevant studies could not be included. The issue of power of individual studies was addressed through the use of meta-analysis; however, it was noteworthy that many of the studies had small sample sizes. Additionally, we were not able to take into account the psychometric properties of the paranoia measures used across studies in our analyses, which means that the review is silent about which measures of paranoia are more or less sensitive to change. Findings from the meta-analysis indicated high levels of statistical heterogeneity, which need to be considered when interpreting the findings from this review. The review only included papers in the English language, such that there may be other studies that were not considered, and we were not able to determine whether the effectiveness of paranoia inductions differed between diagnostic groups. Furthermore, the focus of the review was solely on experimental studies that induce or manipulate paranoid thinking; therefore, some important areas were not included, such as factors associated with paranoia, and the findings do not generalise to biological inductions of paranoia. The review may also be subject to publication bias as the gray literature was not searched.

Recommendations for future research and clinical implications

The review highlights a number of areas for consideration in future research. There are some key methodological points that should be taken forward in future research, in particular, the inclusion of both baseline and post-manipulation measures of paranoia, and the measurement of affective states and cognitive processes that may also be activated via different methodological approaches. Although this review has highlighted that studies have started to examine the effect of affective states (e.g. anxiety) and cognitive processes (e.g. rumination) on paranoia, future studies might also determine the extent to which increases in paranoia occur in the presence v. absence of other affective states, and the extent to which increases in paranoia are accompanied by activation of theoretically relevant cognitive processes, such as worry. This research is important to identify the causal mechanisms that are implicated in paranoia. Additionally, although the review highlighted psychosis proneness as a moderator, the identification of additional moderators is needed in future research. The recent publication of the revised Green Paranoid Thoughts Scale (R-GPTS, Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Loe, Kingdon, Startup, Molodynski, Rosebrock and Bird2021) with severity cut offs will also allow more precision in sample selection and will facilitate the identification of nonclinical groups with elevated levels of trait paranoia for inclusion in future experimental studies. This is perhaps particularly important given the findings from the current review that individuals with existing vulnerability were more prone to the effects of experimental paradigms.

To date, the majority of experimental studies have relied almost exclusively on self-report measures, which may be subject to demand characteristics. Additional behavioural indices of paranoia have started to be identified and used in research studies (e.g. trust and distrust behaviours, behavioural or social avoidance and inequity aversion in economic contexts), and future research would be strengthened by the use of both self-report measures and behavioural indices of paranoia. In terms of the measures, it is also important to consider how sensitive paranoia scales are to change, particularly in non-clinical samples where data may be skewed. If scales are highly sensitive to change, it may be more difficult to determine whether a statistically significant change in a score is clinically or theoretically meaningful. Furthermore, although the paranoia scales have reasonable convergent validity with each other (possibly due to the fact that some of the scales were derived from each other), the issue of discriminant validity, construct validity and test re-test reliability is less well-researched.

Research has started to focus on identifying and experimentally testing factors that may buffer or attenuate paranoid thinking. To date, positive self-cognitions, self-affirmation, self-compassion, priming positive attachment and cognitive reappraisal have been shown to be effective (Ellett & Chadwick, Reference Ellett and Chadwick2007; Kingston & Ellett, Reference Kingston and Ellett2014; Lincoln, Stahnke, & Moritz, Reference Lincoln, Stahnke and Moritz2014; Bullock et al., Reference Bullock, Newman-Taylor and Stopa2016; Gollwitzer et al., Reference Gollwitzer, Wilczynska and Jaya2018). A related issue yet to be investigated is the stability of induced paranoia following manipulation. Although one study has shown that, once activated, paranoia is slow to dissipate, it would be interesting to determine the extent to which increases in paranoia reduce naturalistically, as has been shown cross-sectionally in relation to individual paranoid experiences or whether it is amenable to specific interventions, such as mindfulness, which have been shown to be helpful in clinical and nonclinical populations (Ellett & Chadwick, Reference Ellett and Chadwick2007; Chadwick, Hughes, Russell, Russell, & Dagnan, Reference Chadwick, Hughes, Russell, Russell and Dagnan2009; Ellett, Reference Ellett2013; Chadwick, Newman-Taylor, & Abba, Reference Chadwick, Newman-Taylor and Abba2005; Allen-Crooks & Ellett, Reference Allen-Crooks and Ellett2014; Shore, Strauss, Cavanagh, Hayward, & Ellett, Reference Shore, Strauss, Cavanagh, Hayward and Ellett2018; Ellett et al., Reference Ellett, Tarant, Kouimtsidis, Kingston, Vivarelli, Mendis and Chadwick2020). Future research might also usefully identify participants who are immune to paranoia induction in the context of threat, so that resiliency factors can be explored and fostered or enhanced, in individuals who experience paranoia across the continuum, consistent with a recent focus on strength-based approaches in psychological interventions (Chadwick, Reference Chadwick2006; Padesky & Mooney, Reference Padesky and Mooney2012).

While there is a growing literature on paranoia induction in nonclinical populations, there have been disproportionately fewer experimental studies conducted with clinical populations, which is a knowledge gap in the literature that should be addressed in future research. Additionally, a key limitation of experimental research is that it can lack ecological validity. Although research has started to examine the extent to which more naturalistic environments or interpersonal events give rise to paranoia in nonclinical populations (e.g. Hepper, Ellett, Kerley, & Kingston, Reference Hepper, Ellett, Kerley and Kingston2022; Ellett, Foxall, Wildschut, & Chadwick, Reference Ellett, Foxall, Wildschut and Chadwick2023), more research is needed in clinical populations, including identification of the specific characteristics of the environment that either intensify or attenuate paranoia. Finally, while some of the antecedents of paranoia have been investigated, there have been relatively few studies that have examined the behavioural consequences of paranoia.

There are also a number of important clinical implications from the reviewed studies that warrant consideration. For example, the ease with which paranoia can be induced in nonclinical samples may provide a normalising framework for people experiencing persecutory delusions and could be used to highlight factors involved in the development and maintenance of paranoia to individuals with these distressing experiences. Interestingly, some of the methodologies employed have started to be used and tested directly as the basis for psychological interventions, in particular, virtual reality (Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Bradley, Antley, Bourke, DeWeever, Evans and Clark2016; Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Lister, Waite, Yu, Slater, Dunn and Clark2019). Finally, studies in this area have begun to identify factors that attenuate or buffer paranoia, which could be tested and potentially translated for use with clinical populations.

Conclusion

The findings of this review demonstrate that paranoia can be induced using a wide range of experimental paradigms, consistent with continuum, cognitive and evolutionary models of paranoia, that can guide decision-making in future research. Key clinical implications are the possibility of using experimental paradigms to investigate factors that ameliorate paranoid thinking in the context of threat or existing vulnerabilities.

Conflict of interest

There are no conflicts of interest.

References

Allen-Crooks, R., & Ellett, L. (2014). Naturalistic change in nonclinical paranoid experiences. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 42(5), 634639. doi: 10.1017/S1352465813001148CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bebbington, P. E., McBride, O., Steel, C., Kuipers, E., Radovanovic, M., Brugha, T., … Freeman, D. (2013). The structure of paranoia in the general population. The British Journal Psychiatry, 202, 419427. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.112.119032CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bell, V., & Driscoll, C. (2018). The network structure of paranoia in the general population. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 53, 737744.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bennetts, A., Stopa, L., & Newman-Taylor, K. (2020). Does mental imagery affect paranoia, anxiety and core beliefs? A pilot experimental study in an analogue sample. Psychosis, 12(2), 182187. doi: 10.1080/17522439.2019.1697731CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bennetts, A., Stopa, L., & Newman-Taylor, K. (2021). What can experimental studies tell us about paranoia and anxiety? A systematic review with implications for theory and clinical practice. Psychosis, 14(2), 162175. doi: 10.1080/17522439.2019.1697731CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bentall, R. P., Corcoran, R., Howard, R., Blackwood, N., & Kinderman, P. (2011). Persecutory delusions: A review and theoretical integration. Clinical Psychology Review, 21(8), 11431192. doi: 10.1016/S0272-7358(01)00106-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boden, M., & Berenbaum, H. (2007). Emotional awareness, gender, and suspiciousness. Cognition and Emotion, 21, 268280. doi: 10.1080/02699930600593412CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bodner, E., & Mikulincer, M. (1998). Learned helplessness and the occurrence of depressive-like and paranoid-like responses: The role of attentional focus. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 10101023. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.4.1010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. (2009). An Introduction to Meta-analysis. New Jersey: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, P., Waite, F., & Freeman, D. (2019). ‘Twisting the lion's tail’: Manipulationist tests of causation for psychological mechanisms in the occurrence of delusions and hallucinations. Clinical Psychology Review, 68, 2537. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2018.12.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bullock, G., Newman-Taylor, K., & Stopa, L. (2016). The role of mental imagery in non-clinical paranoia. Journal of Behaviour Therapy & Experimental Psychiatry, 50, 264268. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2015.10.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butler, R., Berry, K., Ellett, L., & Bucci, S. (2019). An experimental investigation of the impact of critical and warm comments on state paranoia in a non-clinical sample. Journal of Behaviour Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 62, 3037. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2018.08.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casanova, G. M., Katkovsky, W., & Hershberger, W. A. (1988). Effects of impaired hearing and favorable vs. Unfavorable personal feedback on negative emotional reactions. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44, 982987. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1002/1097-4679(198811)44:6<982::AID-JCLP2270440621>3.0.CO;2-B3.0.CO;2-B>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A., Houts, R. M., Belsky, D. W., Goldman-Mellor, S. J., Harrington, H., Israel, S., … Moffitt, T. E. (2013). The p factor: One general psychopathology factor in the structure of psychiatric disorders? Clinical Psychology Science, 2(2), 119137. doi: 10.1177/2167702613497473CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chadwick, P. (2006). Person-based Cognitive Therapy for Distressing Psychosis. West Sussex, UK: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chadwick, P., Hughes, S., Russell, D., Russell, I., & Dagnan, D. (2009). Mindfulness groups for distressing voices and paranoia: A replication and randomized feasibility trial. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 37, 403412. doi: 10.1017/S1352465809990166CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chadwick, P., Newman-Taylor, K. & Abba, N. (2005). Mindfulness groups for people with psychosis. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 33, 351359. doi: 10.1017/S1352465805002158CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioural Sciences. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Cook, T. D., & Perrin, B. F. (1971). The effects of suspiciousness of deception and the perceived legitimacy of deception on task performance in an attitude change experiment. Journal of Personality, 39, 204224. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1971.tb00037.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Couzoulis-Mayfrank, E., Heekeren, K., Neukirch, A., Stoll, M., Stock, C., Obradovic, M., & Kovar, K. (2005). Psychological effects of (S-Ketamine and N,N-Dimethyltryptamine (DMT): A double-blind, cross-over study in healthy volunteers. Pharmacopsychiatry, 38, 301311. doi: 10.1055/s-2005-916185CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duval, S., & Tweedie, R. (2000). Trim and fill: A simple funnel-plot-based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics, 56(2), 455463. Retrieved from https://www.jstor.org/stable/2676988CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Egger, M., Smith, G. D., Schneider, M., & Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. British Medical Journal, 315, 629. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellett, L. (2013). Mindfulness for paranoid beliefs: Evidence from two case studies. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 41, 238242. doi: 10.1017/S1352465812000586CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellett, L., & Chadwick, P. (2007). Paranoid cognitions, failure, and focus of attention in college students. Cognition and Emotion, 21, 558576. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930600758155CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellett, L., Foxall, A., Wildschut, T., & Chadwick, P. (2023). Dispositional forgiveness buffers paranoia following interpersonal transgression. Journal of Personality, 91(3), 556565. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12755CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellett, L., Freeman, D., & Garety, P. A. (2008). The psychological effect of an urban environment on individuals with persecutory delusions: The Camberwell walk study. Schizophrenia Research, 99, 7784. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.10.027CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellett, L., Lopes, B., & Chadwick, P. (2003). Paranoia in a nonclinical population of college students. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 191, 425430. doi: 10.1097/01.NMD.0000081646.33030.EFCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellett, L., Tarant, E., Kouimtsidis, C., Kingston, J., Vivarelli, L., Mendis, J., & Chadwick, P. (2020). Group mindfulness-based therapy for persecutory delusions: A pilot randomised controlled trial. Schizophrenia Research, 222, 534536. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2020.04.023CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fenigstein, A., & Vanable, P. A. (1992). Paranoia and self-consciousness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62(1), 129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flower, L., Newman-Taylor, K., & Stopa, L. (2015). Cognitive control processes in paranoia: The impact of threat induction on strategic cognition and self-focused attention. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 43(1), 108118. doi: 10.1017/S1352465813000891CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, D., Bradley, J., Antley, A., Bourke, E., DeWeever, N., Evans, N., … Clark, D. M. (2016). Virtual reality in the treatment of persecutory delusions: Randomised controlled experimental study testing how to reduce delusional conviction. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 209(1), 6267. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.115.176438CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, D., Garety, P., Bebbington, P., Smith, B., Rollinson, R., Fowler, D., … Dunn, G. (2005). Psychological investigation of the structure of paranoia in a non-clinical population. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 186(5), 427435. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.186.5.427PCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freeman, D., & Garety, P. A. (2000). Comments on the content of persecutory delusions: Does the definition need clarification? British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 39, 407414. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1348/014466500163400CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, D., Garety, P. A., Kuipers, E., Fowler, D., & Bebbington, P. E. (2002). A cognitive model of persecutory delusions. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 41(4), 331347. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1348/014466502760387461CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, D., Lister, R., Waite, F., Yu, L. M., Slater, M., Dunn, G., & Clark, D. (2019). Automated psychological therapy using virtual reality (VR) for patients with persecutory delusions: Study protocol for a single-blind parallel-group randomised controlled trial (THRIVE). Trials, 20, 18. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1186/s13063-019-3198-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, D., Loe, B. S., Kingdon, D., Startup, H., Molodynski, A., Rosebrock, L., … Bird, J. C. (2021). The revised Green et al., Paranoid Thoughts Scale (R-GPTS): Psychometric properties, severity ranges, and clinical cut-offs. Psychological Medicine, 51(2), 244253. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719003155CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freeman, D., Pugh, K., Green, C., Valmaggia, L., Dunn, G., & Garety, P. (2007). A measure of state persecutory ideation for experimental studies. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 195(9), 781784. doi: 0.1097/NMD.0b013e318145a0a9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garety, P. A., Everitt, B. S., & Hemsley, D. R. (1988). The characteristics of delusions: A cluster analysis of deluded subjects. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 237, 112114. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00382375Google ScholarPubMed
Garety, P. A., Kuipers, E., Fowler, D., Freeman, D., & Bebbington, P. A. (2001). A cognitive model of the positive symptoms of psychosis. Psychological Medicine, 31, 189195. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291701003312CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gollwitzer, A., Wilczynska, M., & Jaya, E. S. (2018). Targeting the link between loneliness and paranoia via an interventionist-causal model framework. Psychiatry Research, 263, 101107. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2018.02.050CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, M. F., Nuechterlein, K. H., & Breitmeyer, B. (1995). Backward masking performance as an indicator of vulnerability to schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 15(1), 119. doi: 10.1016/0920-9964(95)95366-HCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hepper, E. G., Ellett, L., Kerley, D., & Kingston, J. L. (2022). Are they out to get me? Individual differences in nonclinical paranoia as a function of narcissism and defensive self-protection. Journal of Personality, 90(5), 727747. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12693CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hepper, E. G., Wildschut, T., Sedikides, C., Robertson, S., & Routledge, C. D. (2021). Time capsule: Nostalgia shields psychological wellbeing from limited time horizons. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 21(3), 644664. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/emo0000728CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirst, R. J., Cragg, L., & Allen, H. A. (2018). Vision dominates audition in adults but not children: A meta-analysis of the Colavita effect. Neuroscience and Biobehavioural Reviews, 94, 286301. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.07.012CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horvat, J. (1986). Detection of suspiciousness as a function of pleas for honesty. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 921924. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.50.5.921.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hutton, J., Ellett, L., & Berry, K. (2017). Adult attachment and paranoia: An experimental investigation. The Cognitive Behaviour Therapist, 10, e4. doi: 10.1017/S1754470X17000058CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahn-Greene, E. T., Killgore, D. B., Kamimori, G. H., Balkin, T. J., & Killgore, W. D. S. (2007). The effects of sleep deprivation on symptoms of psychopathology in healthy adults. Sleep Medicine, 8, 215221. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2006.08.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaltsi, M., Bucci, S., & Morrison, A. (2018). Experimental manipulation of metacognitive beliefs and paranoia in a non-clinical population. Psychosis, 10, 110. doi: 10.1080/17522439.2017.1413128CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kesting, M. L., Bredenpohl, M., Klenke, J., Westermann, S., & Lincoln, T. M. (2013). The impact of social stress on self-esteem and paranoid ideation. Journal of Behaviour Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 44, 122128. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2012.07.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kingston, J., & Ellett, L. (2014). Self-affirmation and nonclinical paranoia. Journal of Behaviour Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 45, 502550. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2014.07.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lamster, F., Nittel, C., Rief, W., Mehl, S., & Lincoln, T. (2017). The impact of loneliness on paranoia: An experimental approach. Journal of Behaviour Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 54, 5157. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2016.06.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lincoln, T., Hohenhaus, F., & Hartmann, M. (2013). Can paranoid thoughts be reduced by targeting negative emotions and self-esteem? An experimental investigation of a brief compassion-focussed intervention. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 37, 390402. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-012-9470-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lincoln, T. M., Lange, J., Burau, J., Exner, C., & Moritz, S. (2010). The effect of state anxiety on paranoid ideation and jumping to conclusions. An experimental investigation. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 36, 11401148. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbp029CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lincoln, T. M., Stahnke, J., & Moritz, S. (2014). The short-term impact of a paranoid explanation on self-esteem: An experimental study. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 38, 397406. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-014-9600-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lincoln, T. M., Sundag, J., Schlier, B., & Karow, A. (2018). The relevance of emotion regulation in explaining why social exclusion triggers paranoia in individuals at clinical high risk of psychosis. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 44(4), 757767. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbx135CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Locascio, J. J., & Snyder, C. R. (1975). Selective attention to threatening stimuli and field independence as factors in the etiology of paranoid behavior. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 84(6), 637. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.84.6.637CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lopes, B., & Jaspal, R. (2015). Paranoia predicts out-group prejudice: Preliminary experimental data. Mental Health, Religion and Culture, 18(5), 380395. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1080/13674676.2015.1065475CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lopes, B., & Pinto-Gouveia, P. (2012). How do non-clinical paranoid and socially anxious individuals react to failure? The role of hostility and state anxiety. Forensic Sciences Research, 3, 144152. doi: 10.4172/2157-7145.1000144Google Scholar
Marr, J. C., Thau, S., Aquino, K., & Barclay, L. J. (2012). Do I want to know? How the motivation to acquire relationship-threatening information in groups contributes to paranoid thought, suspicion behavior, and social rejection. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Process, 117, 285297. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2011.11.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, J. (1970). Suspicion and the experimental confederate: A study of role and credibility. Sociometry, 33, 178192. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.2307/2786328CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martinelli, C., Cavanagh, K., & Dudley, R. E. (2013). The impact of rumination on state paranoid ideation in a nonclinical sample. Behavior Therapy, 44, 385394. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2013.02.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mason, O. J., Morgan, C. J., Stefanovic, A., & Curran, H. V. (2008). The Psychotomimetic States Inventory (PSI): Measuring psychotic-type experiences from ketamine and cannabis. Schizophrenia Research, 103, 138142. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2008.02.020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morrison, A. P. (2001). The interpretation of intrusions in psychosis: An integrative cognitive approach to hallucinations and delusions. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 29(3), 257276. doi: 10.1017/S1352465801003010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newman-Taylor, K., Sood, M., Rowe, A. C., & Carnelley, K. B. (2021). The impact of repeated attachment priming on paranoia, mood and help-seeking intentions in an analogue sample. Brain Sciences, 11(10), 1257. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11101257CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Padesky, C. A., & Mooney, K. A. (2012). Strengths-based cognitive–behavioural therapy: A four-step model to build resilience. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 19(4), 283290. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1002/cpp.1795CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prévost, M., Rodier, M., Lionnet, C., Brodeur, M., King, S., & Debruille, J. B. (2011). Paranoid induction reduces N400s of healthy subjects with delusional-like ideation. Journal of Psychophysiology, 48(7), 937949. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01160.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sellers, R., Wells, A., & Morrison, A. P. (2018). An experimental manipulation of negative metacognitive beliefs in non-clinical paranoia: Effects on intrusions and state anxiety. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 9(3), 169185. doi: 10.5127/jep.062117CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shore, R., Strauss, C., Cavanagh, K., Hayward, M., & Ellett, L. (2018). A randomised controlled trial of a brief online mindfulness-based intervention on paranoia in a non-clinical sample. Mindfulness, 9, 294302. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-017-0774-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Soflau, R., & David, D. O. (2019). The impact of irrational beliefs on paranoid thoughts. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 47(3), 270286. doi: 10.1017/S1352465818000565CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sood, M., Carnelley, K., & Newman-Taylor, K. (2021). How does attachment imagery for paranoia work? Cognitive fusion and beliefs about self and others mediate the impact on paranoia and anxiety. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 94(4), 973–933. doi: 10.1111/papt.12354CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sood, M., & Newman-Taylor, K. (2020). Cognitive fusion mediates the impact of attachment imagery on paranoia and anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 44, 11501161. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-020-10127-yCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strauss, J. S. (1969). Hallucinations and delusions as points on continua function: Rating scale evidence. Archives of General Psychiatry, 21, 581586. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1969.01740230069010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sundag, J., Ascone, L., & Lincoln, T. M. (2018). The predictive value of early maladaptive schemas in paranoid responses to social stress. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 25(1), 6575. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1002/cpp.2128CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Os, J., Hanssen, M., Bijl, R. V., & Ravelli, A. (2000). Strauss (1969) revisited: A psychosis continuum in the general population? Schizophrenia Research, 45, 1120. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1016/S0920-9964(99)00224-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Veling, W., Counotte, J., Pot-Kolder, R., van Os, J., & van der Gaag, M. (2016). Childhood trauma, psychosis liability and social stress reactivity: A virtual reality study. Psychological Medicine, 46(16), 33393348. doi: 10.1017/S0033291716002208CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Westermann, S., Kesting, M. L., & Lincoln, T. M. (2012). Being deluded after being excluded? How emotion regulation deficits in paranoia-prone individuals affect state paranoia during experimentally induced social stress. Behavior Therapy, 43, 329340. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2011.07.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zimbardo, P. G., Andersen, S. M., & Kabat, L. G. (1981). Induced hearing deficit generates experimental paranoia. Science (New York, N.Y.), 212, 15291531. doi: 10.1126/science.7233242CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zubin, J., & Spring, B. (1977). Vulnerability: A new view of schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 86, 103126. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.86.2.103CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Search results (Figure adapted from guidelines set out by the PRISMA group).

Figure 1

Table 1. Study Characteristics and Effect Sizes

Figure 2

Table 2. Quality Appraisal of Studies

Figure 3

Figure 2. Forrest plot of the main analysis considering the overall effect of paranoia induction.

Figure 4

Table 3. Subgroup Analyses Considering Specific Paranoia Induction Manipulations

Figure 5

Table 4. Summary of Mediation and Moderation Effects