Hostname: page-component-5c6d5d7d68-qks25 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-08-17T13:54:13.017Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Poorer color discrimination by females when tested with pseudoisochromatic plates containing vanishing designs on neutral backgrounds

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 July 2008

RIGMOR C. BARAAS*
Affiliation:
Department of Optometry & Visual Science, Buskerud University College, Kongsberg, Norway
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: R.C. Baraas, Department of Optometry & Visual Science, Buskerud University College, P.O. Box 251, 3603 Kongsberg, Norway. E-mail: rigmor.baraas@hibu.no

Abstract

It might be expected that normal trichromatic females would perform as well as normal trichromatic males of the same age when tested with standard clinical color-vision tests that use pseudoisochromatic vanishing designs on neutral gray backgrounds such as the Hardy-Rand-Rittler (HRR) pseudoisochromatic plates and the Neitz Test of Color Vision (NTCV). Here 2966 children aged 6–13 years from four municipalities in Norway were tested in their school classrooms with the NTCV. Children who made errors on the test were retested. 187 males and 152 females made one or more errors on retest, and each was tested individually on the Richmond HRR Fourth Edition. 8% of the males were defined as color deficient when a double criterion for failing was applied, that is, one or more errors on the NTCV and two or more errors on the HRR. The calculated frequency of color-deficient females (homozygotes) for the same criterion is 0.42%. By contrast, 3% of females failed the criterion that gave a stable population of color-deficient males. This result is considered in relation to reports of female carriers of color-vision deficiency having problems with the Ishihara test and of females having poorer color discrimination than males.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Asenjo, A.B., Rim, J. & Oprian, D.D. (1994). Molecular determinants of human red/green color discrimination. Neuron 12, 11311138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bailey, J.E., Neitz, M., Tait, D.M. & Neitz, J. (2004). Evaluation of an updated HRR color vision test. Visual Neuroscience 21, 431436.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belcher, S.J., Greenshields, K.W. & Wright, W.D. (1958). Colour vision survey using the Ishihara, Dvorine, Bostrom and Kugelberg, Bostrom, and American-Optical Hardy-Rand-Rittler tests. British Journal of Ophthalmology 42, 355359.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Birch, J. (1997). Clinical use of the American Optical Company (Hardy, Rand and Rittler) pseudoisochromatic plates for red-green colour deficiency. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics 17, 248254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cole, B.L., Lian, K.-Y. & Lakkis, C. (2006). The new Richmond HRR pseudoisochromatic test for colour vision is better than the Ishihara test. Clinical and Experimental Optometry 89, 7380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crone, R.A. (1959). Spectral sensitivity in color-defective subjects and heterozygous carriers. American Journal of Ophthalmology 48, 231238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crone, R.A. (1961). Quantitative diagnosis of defective color vision. A comparative evaluation of the Ishihara test, the Farnsworth dichotomous test and the Hardy-Rand-Rittler polychromatic plates. American Journal of Ophthalmology 51, 298305.Google ScholarPubMed
Dain, S.J. (2004). Colorimetric analysis of four editions of the Hardy-Rand-Rittler pseudoisochromatic tests. Visual Neuroscience 21, 437443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feig, K. & Ropers, H.H. (1978). On the incidence of unilateral and bilateral colour blindness in heterozygous females. Human Genetics 41, 313323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hardy, L.H., Rand, G. & Rittler, M.C. (1954). H-R-R polychromatic plates. Journal of the Optical Society of America 44, 509523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, A.R. & Aspinall, P.A. (1982). Pass/fail criteria in colour vision tests and their effect on decision confidence. Documenta Ophthalmologica Proceedings Series 33, 157162.Google Scholar
Hill, A.R., Heron, G., Lloyd, M. & Lowther, P. (1982). An evaluation of some colour vision tests for children. Documenta Ophthalmologica Proceedings Series 33, 183187.Google Scholar
Hood, S.M., Mollon, J.D., Purves, L. & Jordan, G. (2006). Color discrimination in carriers of color deficiency. Vision Research 46, 28942900.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jordan, G. & Mollon, J.D. (1993). A study of women heterozygous for colour deficiencies. Vision Research 33, 14951508.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jorgensen, A.L., Philip, J., Raskind, W.H., Matsushita, M., Christensen, B., Dreyer, V. & Motulsky, A.G. (1992). Different patterns of X inactivation in MZ twins discordant for red-green color-vision deficiency. American Journal of Human Genetics 51, 291298.Google ScholarPubMed
Lakowski, R. (1969). Theory and practice of colour vision testing: A review. Part 2. British Journal of Industrial Medicine 26, 265288.Google Scholar
Lyon, M.F. (1972). X-chromosome inactivation and developmental patterns in mammals. Biological Reviews Cambridge Philosophical Society 47, 135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merbs, S.L. & Nathans, J. (1992). Absorption spectra of human cone pigments. Nature 356, 433435.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nathans, J., Piantanida, T.P., Eddy, R.L., Shows, T.B. & Hogness, D.S. (1986a). Molecular genetics of inherited variation in human color vision. Science 232, 203210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nathans, J., Thomas, D. & Hogness, D.S. (1986b). Molecular genetics of human color vision: the genes encoding blue, green, and red pigments. Science 232, 193202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neitz, J., Neitz, M. & Kainz, P.M. (1996). Visual pigment gene structure and the severity of color vision defects. Science 274, 801804.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neitz, J., Summerfelt, P. & Neitz, M. (2001). The Neitz Test of Color Vision Manual. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
Neitz, M., Kraft, T.W. & Neitz, J. (1998). Expression of L cone pigment gene subtypes in females. Vision Research 38, 32213225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neitz, M. & Neitz, J. (2001). A new mass screening test for color-vision deficiencies in children. Color Research and Application 26, S239S249.3.0.CO;2-L>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neitz, M., Neitz, J. & Jacobs, G.H. (1991). Spectral tuning of pigments underlying red-green color vision. Science 252, 971974.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pardo, P.J., Pérez, A.L. & Suero, M.I. (2007). An example of sex-linked color vision difference. Color Research and Application 32, 433439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paulson, H.M. (1971). Color vision testing in the United States Navy. In Visual Science. Proceedings of the 1968 International Symposium, ed. Pierce, J.R. & Levene, J.R., pp. 164176. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Rand, G. & Rittler, M.C. (1956). An evaluation of the AO H-R-R pseudoisochromatic plates: A test for detecting, classifying, and estimating the degree of defective color vision. American Medical Association Archives of Ophthalmology 56, 736742.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodríguez-Carmona, M., Sharpe, L.T., Harlow, J.A. & Barbur, J.L. (2008). Sex-related differences in chromatic sensitivity. Visual Neuroscience 25, This issue.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharpe, L.T., Stockman, A., Jägle, H., Knau, H., Klausen, G., Reitner, A. & Nathans, J. (1998). Red, green, and red-green hybrid pigments in the human retina: correlations between deduced protein sequences and psychophysically measured spectral sensitivities. The Journal of Neuroscience 18, 1005310069.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharpe, L.T., Stockman, A., Jägle, H. & Nathans, J. (1999). Opsin genes, cone photopigments, color vision, and color blindness. In Color vision, ed. Gegenfurtner, K.R. & Sharpe, L.T., pp. 351. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sloan, L.L. & Habel, A. (1956). Tests for color deficiency based on the pseudoisochromatic principle: A comparative study of several new tests. American Medical Association Archives of Ophthalmology 55, 229239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vos, J.J., Verkaik, W. & Boogaard, J. (1972). The significance of the TMC and HRR color-vision-tests as to red-green defectiveness. American Journal of Optometry and Archives of American Academy of Optometry 49, 847859.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waaler, G.H.M. (1927). Über die erblichkeitsverhältnisse der verschiedenen arten von angeborener rotgrünblindheit. Acta Ophthalmologica 5, 309345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walls, G.L. (1959). How good is the H-R-R test for color blindness. American Journal of Optometry and Archives of American Academy of Optometry 36, 169193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winderickx, J., Lindsey, D.T., Sanocki, E., Teller, D.Y., Motulsky, A.G. & Deeb, S.S. (1992). Polymorphism in red photopigment underlies variation in colour matching. Nature 356, 431433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed