Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-cjp7w Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-23T04:56:07.402Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Color naming, unique hues, and hue cancellation predicted from singularities in reflection properties

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 September 2006

DAVID L. PHILIPONA
Affiliation:
Laboratoire de Psychologie de la Perception, CNRS, Université Paris 5 René Descartes, Paris, France
J. KEVIN O'REGAN
Affiliation:
Laboratoire de Psychologie de la Perception, CNRS, Université Paris 5 René Descartes, Paris, France

Abstract

Psychophysical studies suggest that different colors have different perceptual status: red and blue for example are thought of as elementary sensations whereas yellowish green is not. The dominant account for such perceptual asymmetries attributes them to specificities of the neuronal representation of colors. Alternative accounts involve cultural or linguistic arguments. What these accounts have in common is the idea that there are no asymmetries in the physics of light and surfaces that could underlie the perceptual structure of colors, and this is why neuronal or cultural processes must be invoked as the essential underlying mechanisms that structure color perception. Here, we suggest a biological approach for surface reflection properties that takes into account only the information about light that is accessible to an organism given the photopigments it possesses, and we show that now asymmetries appear in the behavior of surfaces with respect to light. These asymmetries provide a classification of surface properties that turns out to be identical to the one observed in linguistic color categorization across numerous cultures, as pinned down by cross cultural studies. Further, we show that data from psychophysical studies about unique hues and hue cancellation are consistent with the viewpoint that stimuli reported by observers as special are those associated with this singularity-based categorization of surfaces under a standard illuminant. The approach predicts that unique blue and unique yellow should be aligned in chromatic space while unique red and unique green should not, a fact usually conjectured to result from nonlinearities in chromatic pathways.

Type
SURFACE COLOR PERCEPTION
Copyright
© 2006 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Berlin, B. & Kay, P. (1969). Basic Color Terms: Their Universality and Evolution. Berkeley, California: University of California Press.
Boynton, R.M. & Olson, C.X. (1987). Locating basic colors in the OSA space. Color Research and Application 12, 94105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brainard, D.H., Roorda, A., Yamauchi, Y., Calderone, J.B., Metha, A., Neitz, M., Neitz, J., Williams, D.R., & Jacobs, G.H. (2000). Functional consequences of the relative numbers of L and M cones. Journal of the Optical Society of America A 17, 607614.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buchsbaum, G. & Gottschalk, A. (1984). Chromaticity coordinates of frequency-limited functions. Journal of the Optical Society of America 1, 885887.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chalmers, D. (1996). The Conscious Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Chiao, C.-C., Cronin, T.W., & Osorio, D. (2000). Color signals in natural scenes: Characteristics of reflectance spectra and effects of natural illuminants. Journal of the Optical Society of America A 17, 218224CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chichilnisky, E. & Wandell, B.A. (1999). Trichromatic opponent-color classification. Vision Research 39, 34443458.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chittka, L. & Menzel, R. (1992). The evolutionary adaptation of flower colors and the insect pollinators' color vision systems. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 171, 171181.Google Scholar
Crick, F. & Koch, C. (2003). A framework for consciousness. Nature Neuroscience 6, 119126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Finlayson, G., Drew, M., & Funt, B.V. (1994). Spectral sharpening: Sensor transformations for improved color constancy. Journal of the Optical Society of America A 11, 15531563.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hering, E. (1920/1964). Outlines of a Theory of the Light Sense. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press.
Jameson, D. & Hurvich, L.M. (1955). Some quantitative aspects of an opponent colors theory. I. Chromatic responses and spectral saturation. Journal of the Optical Society of America 45, 546552.Google Scholar
Judd, D.B., MacAdam, D.L., & Wyszecki, G. (1964). Spectral distribution of typical daylight as a function of correlated color temperature. Journal of the Optical Society of America 54, 10311040.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knoblauch, K., Sirovich, L., & Wooten, B.R. (1985). Linearity of hue cancellation in sex-linked dichromacy. Journal of the Optical Society of America A 2, 136146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knoblauch, K. & Shevell, S.K. (2001). Relating cone signals to color appearance: Failure of monotonicity in yellow/blue. Visual Neuroscience 18, 901906.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuehni, R.G. (2004). Variability in unique hue selection: A surprising phenomenon. Color Research and Application 29, 158162.Google Scholar
Levine, J. (1983). Materialism and Qualia: The Explanatory Gap. Pacific Philosophical Quarterly 64, 354361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lotto, B.R. & Purves, D. (2002). A rationale for the structure of color space. Trends in Neuroscience 25, 8488CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maloney, L.T. (1999). Physics-based approaches to modeling surface color perception. In Color Vision: From Genes to Perception, eds. Maloney, L.T., Gegenfurtner, K.R. & Sharpe, L.T., pp. 387422. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
McCollough, C. (1965). Color adaptation of edge-detectors in the human visual system. Science 149, 11151116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mollon, J.D. & Jordan, G. (1997). On the nature of unique hues. In John Dalton's Colour Vision Legacy, eds. Dickinson, C., Murray, I. & Carden, D., pp. 381392. London, UK: Taylor & Francis.
Nascimento, S.M.C., Ferreira, F.P., & Foster, D.H. (2002). Statistics of spatial cone-excitation ratios in natural scenes. Journal of the Optical Society of America A 19, 14841490CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neitz, J., Carroll, J., Yamauchi, Y., Neitz, M., & Williams, D.R. (2002). Color perception is mediated by a plastic neural mechanism that remains adjustable in adults. Neuron 35, 783792.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Regan, J.K. & Noë, A. (2001). A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24, 9391011.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parkkinen, J.P.S., Hallikainen, J., & Jaaskelainen, T. (1989). Characteristic spectra of Munsell colors. Journal of the Optical Society of America A 6, 318322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Regier, T., Kay, P., & Cook, R.S. (2005). Focal colors are universal after all. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the U.S.A. 102, 83868391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romero, J., Garcia-Beltran, A., & Hernandez-Andres, J. (1997). Linear bases for representation of natural and artificial illuminants. Journal of the Optical Society of America A 14, 10071014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruderman, D.R., Cronin, T.W., & Chiao, C.-C. (1998). Statistics of cone responses to natural images: Implications for visual coding. Journal of the Optical Society of America A 15, 20362045.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stiles, W.S. & Burch, J.M. (1959). NPL colour-matching investigation: Final report. Optica Acta 6, 126.Google Scholar
Stockman, A. & Sharpe, L.T. (2000). Spectral sensitivities of the middle- and long-wavelength sensitive cones derived from measurements in observers of known genotype. Vision Research 40, 17111737.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sugita, Y. (2004). Experience in early infancy is indispensable for color perception. Current Biology 14, 12671271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valberg, A. (2001). Unique hues: An old problem for a new generation. Vision Research 41, 16451657.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wandell, B. (1997). Foundations of Vision. Sunderland, Massachusetts: Sinauer Associates.
Webster, M.A., Miyahara, E., Malkoc, G., & Raker, V.E. (2000). Variations in normal color vision. I. Cone-opponent axes. Journal of the Optical Society of America A 17, 15351545.Google Scholar
Westland, S., Shaw, A.J., & Owens, H.C. (2000). Colour statistics of natural and man-made surfaces. Sensor Review 20, 5055.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zeki, S. (2003). The disunity of consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Science 7, 214218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar