Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-r5zm4 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-24T10:02:26.027Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chromatic induction from surrounding stimuli under perceptual suppression

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 August 2014

KOJI HORIUCHI
Affiliation:
Graduate School of Information Sciences, Tohoku University, Sendai, Miyagi, Japan
ICHIRO KURIKI*
Affiliation:
Graduate School of Information Sciences, Tohoku University, Sendai, Miyagi, Japan Research Institute of Electrical Communication, Tohoku University, Sendai, Miyagi, Japan
RUMI TOKUNAGA
Affiliation:
Graduate School of Information Sciences, Tohoku University, Sendai, Miyagi, Japan Research Institute of Electrical Communication, Tohoku University, Sendai, Miyagi, Japan
KAZUMICHI MATSUMIYA
Affiliation:
Graduate School of Information Sciences, Tohoku University, Sendai, Miyagi, Japan Research Institute of Electrical Communication, Tohoku University, Sendai, Miyagi, Japan
SATOSHI SHIOIRI
Affiliation:
Graduate School of Information Sciences, Tohoku University, Sendai, Miyagi, Japan Research Institute of Electrical Communication, Tohoku University, Sendai, Miyagi, Japan
*
*Address correspondence to: Ichiro Kuriki, 2-1-1 Katahira, Aoba-ku, Sendai, Miyagi 980-8577, Japan. E-mail: ikuriki@riec.tohoku.ac.jp

Abstract

The appearance of colors can be affected by their spatiotemporal context. The shift in color appearance according to the surrounding colors is called color induction or chromatic induction; in particular, the shift in opponent color of the surround is called chromatic contrast. To investigate whether chromatic induction occurs even when the chromatic surround is imperceptible, we measured chromatic induction during interocular suppression. A multicolor or uniform color field was presented as the surround stimulus, and a colored continuous flash suppression (CFS) stimulus was presented to the dominant eye of each subject. The subjects were asked to report the appearance of the test field only when the stationary surround stimulus is invisible by interocular suppression with CFS. The resulting shifts in color appearance due to chromatic induction were significant even under the conditions of interocular suppression for all surround stimuli. The magnitude of chromatic induction differed with the surround conditions, and this difference was preserved regardless of the viewing conditions. The chromatic induction effect was reduced by CFS, in proportion to the magnitude of chromatic induction under natural (i.e., no-CFS) viewing conditions. According to an analysis with linear model fitting, we revealed the presence of at least two kinds of subprocesses for chromatic induction that reside at higher and lower levels than the site of interocular suppression. One mechanism yields different degrees of chromatic induction based on the complexity of the surround, which is unaffected by interocular suppression, while the other mechanism changes its output with interocular suppression acting as a gain control. Our results imply that the total chromatic induction effect is achieved via a linear summation of outputs from mechanisms that reside at different levels of visual processing.

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Akaike, H. (1974). A new look at the statistical model identification. IEEE Transactions on Automatic Control 19, 716723.Google Scholar
Barbur, J.L., de Cunha, D., Williams, C.B. & Plant, G. (2004). Study of instantaneous color constancy mechanisms in human vision. Journal of Electronic Imaging 13, 1528.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blake, R. & Fox, R. (1974). Adaptation to invisible gratings and the site of binocular rivalry suppression. Nature 249, 488490.Google Scholar
Blake, R. & Logothetis, N.K. (2002). Visual competition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 3, 1321.Google Scholar
Brainard, D.H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision 10, 433436.Google Scholar
Brenner, E. & Cornelissen, F.W. (1991). Spatial interactions in color vision depend on distances between boundaries. Naturwissenschaften 78, 7073.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brenner, E., Ruiz, J.S., Herráiz, E.M., Cornelissen, F.W. & Smeets, J.B.J. (2003). Chromatic induction and the layout of colours within a complex scene. Vision Research 43, 14131421.Google Scholar
Brown, R.O. & MacLeod, D.I.A. (1997). Color appearance depends on the variance of surround colors. Current Biology 7, 844849.Google Scholar
Buchsbaum, G. (1980). A spatial processor model for object colour perception. Journal of Franklin Institute 310, 126.Google Scholar
Cai, Y., Zhou, T. & Chen, L. (2008). Effects of binocular suppression on surround suppression. Journal of Vision 18, 110.Google Scholar
Carlson, T.A. & He, S. (2000). Visible binocular beats from invisible monocular stimuli during binocular rivalry. Current Biology 10, 10551058.Google Scholar
Chaparro, A., Stromeyer, C.F. III, Huang, E.P., Kronauer, R.E. & Eskew, R.T. (1993). Colour is what the eye sees best. Nature 361, 348350.Google Scholar
Chichilnisky, E.J. & Wandell, B.A. (1995). Photoreceptor sensitivity changes explain color appearance shifts induced by large uniform backgrounds in dichoptic matching. Vision Research 35, 239254.Google Scholar
Cowan, W.B. (1983). An inexpensive scheme for calibration of a colour monitor in terms of CIE standard coordinates. Computer Graphics 17, 315321.Google Scholar
de Monasterio, F.M. (1978). Center and surround mechanisms of opponent-color X and Y ganglion cells of retina of macaques. Journal of Neurophysiology 41, 14181434.Google Scholar
Fairchild, M.D. & Reniff, L. (1995). Time course of chromatic adaptation for color-appearance judgments. Journal of the Optical Society of America A 12, 824833.Google Scholar
Fairchild, M.D. (2005). Color Appearance Models (2nd ed.). Wiley IS&T, Chichester, UK.Google Scholar
Foster, D.H. (2011). Color constancy. Vision Research 51, 674700.Google Scholar
Gegenfurtner, K.R. (2003). Cortical mechanisms of colour vision. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 4, 563572.Google Scholar
Goltz, J. & MacLeod, D.I.A. (2002). Influence of scene statistics on colour constancy. Nature 415, 637640.Google Scholar
Hong, S.W. & Blake, R. (2009). Interocular suppression differently affects achromatic and chromatic mechanisms. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 71, 403411.Google Scholar
Hovis, J.K. (1989). Review of dichoptic color mixing. Optometry Vision Science 66, 181190.Google Scholar
Hurlbert, A. & Wolf, K. (2004). Color contrast: A contributory mechanism to color constancy. Progress in Brain Research 144, 145160.Google Scholar
Hurvich, C.M. & Tsai, C-L. (1989). Regression and time series model selection in small samples. Biometrika 76, 297307.Google Scholar
Ishihara, S. (1996). Ishihara’s Tests for Colour Blindness (38 plates edition). Handaya, Tokyo, Japan.Google Scholar
Johnson, E.N., Hawken, M.J. & Shapley, R. (2001). The spatial transformation of color in the primary visual cortex of the macaque monkey. Nature Neuroscience 4, 409416.Google Scholar
Kawabe, T. & Yamada, Y. (2009). Invisible motion contributes to simultaneous motion contrast. Consciousness and Cognition 18, 168175.Google Scholar
Kim, C.Y. & Blake, R. (2005). Psychophysical magic: Rendering the visible ‘invisible’. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9, 381388.Google Scholar
Kuriki, I. (2006). The loci of achromatic points in a real environment under various illuminant chromaticities. Vision Research 46, 30553066.Google Scholar
Kuriki, I. & MacLeod, D.I.A. (1998). Chromatic adaptation after effects on luminance and chromatic channels. In Dickinson, C.M., Murray, I.J. & Carden, D. (Eds.), John Dalton’s colour vision legacy (pp. 7383). Taylor and Francis, London, U.K.Google Scholar
Kuriki, I., Oguma, Y. & Uchikawa, K. (2000). Dynamics of asymmetric color matching. Optical Review 7, 249259.Google Scholar
Kuriki, I. & Uchikawa, K. (1996). Limitations of surface-color and apparent-color constancy. Journal of the Optical Society of America A 13, 16221636.Google Scholar
Land, E.H., Hubel, D.H., Livingstone, M.S., Perry, S.H. & Burns, M.M. (1983). Colour-generating interactions across the corpus callosum. Nature 303, 616618.Google Scholar
Land, E.H. & McCann, J.J. (1971). Lightness and retinex theory. Journal of the Optical Society of America 61, 111.Google Scholar
Lehmkuhle, S.W. & Fox, R. (1975). Effect of binocular rivalry suppression on the motion aftereffect. Vision Research 15, 855859.Google Scholar
Leopold, D.A. & Logothetis, N.K. (1996). Activity changes in early visual cortex reflect monkeys' percepts during binocular rivalry. Nature 379, 549553.Google Scholar
Linnell, K.J. & Foster, D.H. (2002). Scene articulation: Dependence of illuminant estimates on number of surfaces. Perception 31, 151159.Google Scholar
Livingstone, M.S. & Hubel, D.H. (1984). Specificity of intrinsic connections in primate primary visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience 4, 28302835.Google Scholar
Logothetis, N.K. & Schall, J.D. (1989). Neuronal correlates of subjective visual perception. Science 245, 761763.Google Scholar
Lumer, E.D., Friston, K.J. & Rees, G. (1998). Neural correlates of perceptual rivalry in the human brain. Science 280, 19301934.Google Scholar
MacAdam, D.L. (1974). Uniform color scales. Journal of the Optical Society of America 64, 16911702.Google Scholar
Moradi, F., Koch, C. & Shimojo, S. (2005). Face adaptation depends on seeing the face. Neuron 45, 169175.Google Scholar
Peirce, J.W., Solomon, S.G., Forte, J.D., Lennie, P. (2008). Cortical representation of color is binocular. Journal of Vision 8, 110.Google Scholar
Pelli, D.G. (1997). The video toolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision 10, 437442.Google Scholar
Polonsky, A., Blake, R., Braun, J. & Heeger, D.J. (2000). Neuronal activity in human primary visual cortex correlates with perception during binocular rivalry. Nature Neuroscience 3, 11531159.Google Scholar
Schein, S.J. & Desimone, R. (1990). Spectral properties of V4 neurons in the macaque. Journal of Neuroscience 10, 33693389.Google Scholar
Schwarz, G.E. (1978). Estimating the dimension of a model. Annals of Statistics 6, 461464.Google Scholar
Shevell, S.K. (1978). The dual role of chromatic backgrounds in color perception. Vision Research 18, 16491661.Google Scholar
Shevell, S.K. (2000). The time course of chromatic adaptation. Color Research and Application 26, S170S173.Google Scholar
Shevell, S.K. & Wei, J. (1998). Chromatic induction: Border contrast or adaptation to surrounding light? Vision Research 38, 15611566.Google Scholar
Shimono, K., Shioiri, S. & Yaguchi, H. (2009). Psychophysical evidence for a purely binocular color system. Vision Research 49, 202210.Google Scholar
Tong, F., Meng, M. & Blake, R. (2006). Neural bases of binocular rivalry. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10, 502511.Google Scholar
Ts'o, D.Y. & Gilbert, C.D. (1988). The organization of chromatic and spatial interactions in the primate striate cortex. Journal of Neuroscience 8, 17121727.Google Scholar
Tsuchiya, N. & Koch, C. (2005). Continuous flash suppression reduces negative afterimages. Nature Neuroscience 8, 10961101.Google Scholar
Valberg, A. & Lange-Malecki, B. (1990). “Colour constancy” in Mondrian patterns: A partial cancellation of physical chromaticity shifts by simultaneous contrast. Vision Research 30, 371380.Google Scholar
Wachtler, T., Sejnowski, T.J. & Albright, T.D. (2003). Representation of color stimuli in awake macaque primary visual cortex. Neuron 37, 681691.Google Scholar
Walraven, J. (1978). Discounting the background—the missing link in the explanation of chromatic induction. Vision Research 16, 289295.Google Scholar
Watanabe, M., Cheng, K., Murayama, Y., Ueno, K., Asamizuya, T., Tanaka, K. & Logothetis, N.K. (2011). Attention but not awareness modulates the bold signal in the human V1 during binocular suppression. Science 334, 829831.Google Scholar
Wichmann, F.A. & Hill, N.J. (2001a). The psychometric function: I. Fitting, sampling, and goodness of fit. Perception and Psychophysics 63, 12931313.Google Scholar
Wichmann, F.A. & Hill, N.J. (2001b). The psychometric function: II. Bootstrap-based confidence intervals and sampling. Perception and Psychophysics 63, 13141329.Google Scholar
Wiesenfelder, H. & Blake, R. (1990). The neural site of binocular rivalry relative to the analysis of motion in the human visual system. Journal of Neuroscience 10, 38803888.Google Scholar
Zaidi, Q., Yoshimi, B., Flanigan, N. & Canova, A. (1992). Lateral interactions within color mechanisms in simultaneous induced contrast. Vision Research 32, 16951707.Google Scholar