Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home

WHEN GENDER AND LOOKING GO HAND IN HAND: Grammatical Gender Processing In L2 Spanish

  • Paola E. Dussias (a1), Jorge R. Valdés Kroff (a2), Rosa E. Guzzardo Tamargo (a3) and Chip Gerfen (a4)

Abstract

In a recent study, Lew-Williams and Fernald (2007) showed that native Spanish speakers use grammatical gender information encoded in Spanish articles to facilitate the processing of upcoming nouns. In this article, we report the results of a study investigating whether grammatical gender facilitates noun recognition during second language (L2) processing. Sixteen monolingual Spanish participants (control group) and 18 English-speaking learners of Spanish (evenly divided into high and low Spanish proficiency) saw two-picture visual scenes in which items matched or did not match in gender. Participants’ eye movements were recorded while they listened to 28 sentences in which masculine and feminine target items were preceded by an article that agreed in gender with the two pictures or agreed only with one of the pictures. An additional group of 15 Italian learners of Spanish was tested to examine whether the presence of gender in the first language (L1) modulates the degree to which gender is used during L2 processing. Data were analyzed by comparing the proportion of eye fixations on the objects in each condition. Monolingual Spanish speakers looked sooner at the referent on different-gender trials than on same-gender trials, replicating results reported in past literature. Italian-Spanish bilinguals exhibited a gender anticipatory effect, but only for the feminine condition. For the masculine condition, participants waited to hear the noun before identifying the referent. Like the Spanish monolinguals, the highly proficient English-Spanish speakers showed evidence of using gender information during online processing, whereas the less proficient learners did not. The results suggest that both proficiency in the L2 and similarities between the L1 and the L2 modulate the usefulness of morphosyntactic information during speech processing.

Copyright

Corresponding author

*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Paola E. Dussias, 237 Burrowes Building, Department of Spanish, Italian & Portuguese, Penn State University, University Park, PA 16802; E-mail: pdussias@psu.edu

Footnotes

Hide All

The writing of this article was supported in part by the National Science Foundation (NSF) grant BCS-0821924 to Paola E. Dussias and Chip Gerfen, by NSF grants BCS-0955090 and OISE-0968369 to Judith F. Kroll and Paola E. Dussias, and by a National Science Foundation graduate research fellowship to Jorge Valdés Kroff. We would like to thank the audience at Penn State’s Center for Language Science and the audience at the 2010 Second Language Research Forum for helpful comments. All errors are our own.

Footnotes

References

Hide All
Ainsworth-Darnell, K., Shulman, H. G., & Boland, J. E. (1998). Dissociating brain responses to syntactic and semantic anomalies: Evidence from Event-Related Potentials. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 112130.
Alameda, J. R., & Cuetos, F. (1995). Diccionario de las unidades lingüísticas del castellano: Volumen I: Orden alfabético / Volumen II: Orden por frecuencias [Dictionary of the linguistic units of Spanish: Volume I: Ordered alphabetically / Volume II: Ordered by frequencies]. Oviedo, Spain: Servicio de publicaciones de la Universidad de Oviedo.
Allopenna, P. D., Magnuson, J. S., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1998). Tracking the time course of spoken word recognition: Evidence for continuous mapping models. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 419439.
Altmann, G. T. M., & Kamide, Y. (1999). Incremental interpretation at verbs: Restricting the domain of subsequent reference. Cognition, 73, 247264.
Baayen, R. H. (2008). Analyzing linguistic data: A practical introduction to statistics using R. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Barber, H., & Carreiras, M. (2005). Grammatical gender and number agreement in Spanish: An ERP comparison. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 137153.
Bates, E., Devescovi, A., Hernández, A., & Pizzamiglio, L. (1996). Gender priming in Italian. Perception & Psychophysics, 85, 9921004.
Boersma, P. (2001). Praat, a system for doing phonetics by computer. Glot International, 5, 341345.
Bruhn de Garavito, J., & White, L. (2002). L2 acquisition of Spanish DPs: The status of grammatical features. In Pérez-Leroux, A. T. & Liceras, J. M. (Eds.), The acquisition of Spanish morphosyntax: The L1/L2 connection (pp. 151176). Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Carello, C., Lukatela, G., & Turvey, M. T. (1988). Rapid naming is affected by association but not by syntax. Memory & Cognition, 16, 187195.
Carroll, S. (1989). Second-language acquisition and the computational paradigm. Language Learning, 39, 535594.
Colé, P., & Segui, J. (1994). Grammatical incongruency and vocabulary types. Memory & Cognition, 22, 387394.
Cooper, R. (1974). The control of eye-fixation by the meaning of spoken language: A new methodology for the real-time investigation of speech perception, memory and language processing. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 84107.
Costa, A., Kovacic, D., Fedorenko, E., & Caramazza, A. (2003). The gender congruency effect and the selection of free-standing and bound morphemes: Evidence from Croatian. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 29, 12701282.
Cubelli, R., Lotto, L., Paolieri, D., Girelli, M., & Job, R. (2005). Grammatical gender is selected in bare noun production: Evidence from the picture-word interference paradigm. Journal of Memory and Language, 53, 4259.
Cudeck, R., & Klebe, K. J. (2002). Multiphase mixed-effects models for repeated measures data. Psychological Methods, 7, 4163.
Dahan, D., Swingley, D., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Magnuson, J. S. (2000). Linguistic gender and spoken-word recognition in French. Journal of Memory and Language, 42, 465480.
Dijkstra, T. (2005). Bilingual visual word recognition and lexical access. In Kroll, J. F. & de Groot, A. M. B. (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches (pp. 179201). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Dowens, M. G., Vergara, M., Barber, H. A., & Carreiras, M. (2010). Morphosyntactic processing in late second-language learners. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 18701887.
Duffield, N., & White, L. (1999). Assessing L2 knowledge of Spanish clitic placement: Converging methodologies. Second Language Research, 15, 133160.
Epstein, S. D., Flynn, S., & Martohardjono, G. (1996). Second language acquisition: Theoretical and experimental issues in contemporary research. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 19, 677758.
Faussart, C., Jakubowicz, C., & Costes, M. (1999). Gender and number processing in spoken French and Spanish. Rivista di Linguistica, 11, 75101.
Fernald, A., Perfors, A., & Marchman, V. A. (2006). Picking up speech in understanding: Speech processing efficiency and vocabulary growth across the 2nd year. Developmental Psychology, 42, 98116.
Fernández-García, M. (1999). Patterns of gender agreement in the speech of second language learners. In Gutiérrez-Rexach, J. & Martínez-Gil, F. (Eds.), Advances in Hispanic linguistics: Papers from the 2nd Hispanic Linguistics Symposium (pp. 315). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
Fischer, B. (1992). Saccadic reaction time: Implications for reading, dyslexia and visual cognition. In Rayner, K. (Ed.), Eye movements and visual cognition: Scene perception and reading (pp. 3145). New York: Springer Verlag.
Franceschina, F. (2001). Against an L2 morphological deficit as an explanation for the differences between native and non-native grammars. In Foster-Cohen, S. H. & Nizegorodcew, A. (Eds.), EUROSLA Yearbook: Vol. 1 (pp. 143158). Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Franceschina, F. (2005). Fossilized second language grammars: The acquisition of grammatical gender. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Friederici, A. D., & Jacobsen, T. (1999). Processing grammatical gender during comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28, 467484.
Foucart, A., & Frenck-Mestre, C. (2011). Grammatical gender processing in L2: Electrophysiological evidence of the effect of L1-L2 syntactic similarity. Bilingualism, 14, 379399.
Foucart, A., & Frenck-Mestre, C. (2012). Can late L2 learners acquire new grammatical features? Evidence from ERPs and eye-tracking. Journal of Memory and Language, 66, 226248.
Gess, R., & Herschensohn, J. (2001). Shifting the DP parameter: A study of Anglophone French L2ers. In Camps, J. & Wiltshire, C. R. (Eds.), Romance syntax, semantics and their L2 acquisition (pp. 105119). Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Gouvea, A. C., Phillips, C., Kazanina, N., & Poeppel, D. (2010). The linguistic processes underlying the P600. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25, 149188.
Grosjean, F., Dommergues, J.-Y., Cornu, E., Guillelmon, D., & Besson, C. (1994). The gender-marking effect in spoken word recognition. Perception & Psychophysics, 56, 590598.
Gurjanov, M., Lukatela, G., Lukatela, K., Savić, M., & Turvey, M. T. (1985). Grammatical priming of inflected nouns by the gender of possessive adjectives. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 11, 692701.
Hagoort, P., & Brown, C. M. (1999). Gender electrified: ERP evidence on the syntactic nature of gender processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28, 715728.
Hawkins, R., & Chan, C. Y. (1997). The partial availability of Universal Grammar in second language acquisition: The “failed functional features hypothesis.Second Language Research, 13, 187226.
Hawkins, R., & Franceschina, F. (2004). Explaining the acquisition and non-acquisition of determiner-noun gender concord in French and Spanish. In Prévost, P. & Paradis, J. (Eds.), The acquisition of French in different contexts (pp. 175205). Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Hernandez, A. E., Kotz, S. A., Hofmann, J., Valentin, V. V., Dapretto, M., & Bookheimer, S. Y. (2004). The neural correlates of grammatical gender decisions in Spanish. Brain Imaging, 15, 863866.
Jacobsen, T. (1999). Effects of grammatical gender on picture and word naming: Evidence from German. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28, 499514.
Jakubowicz, C., & Faussart, C. (1998). Gender agreement in the processing of spoken French. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 27, 597617.
Jescheniak, J. D. (1999). Gender priming in picture naming: Modality and baseline effects. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28, 729737.
Kreysa, H., & Knoeferle, P. (2011). Effects of speaker gaze on spoken language comprehension: Task matters. In Carlson, L., Hölscher, C., & Shipley, T. F. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 15571562). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
Kroll, J. F., & Stewart, E. (1994). Category interference in translation and picture naming: Evidence for asymmetric connections between bilingual memory representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 149174.
Levy, R. (2008). Expectation-based syntactic comprehension. Cognition, 106, 11261177.
Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A. (2007). Young children learning Spanish make rapid use of grammatical gender in spoken word recognition. Psychological Science, 18, 193198.
Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A. (2010). Real-time processing of gender-marked articles by native and non-native Spanish speakers. Journal of Memory and Language, 63, 447464.
McCarthy, C. (2007). Morphological variability in second language Spanish (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). McGill University, Montreal, Canada.
Montrul, S. (2004). Subject and object expression in Spanish heritage speakers: A case of morphosyntactic convergence. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 7, 125142.
Montrul, S., Foote, R., & Perpiñan, S. (2008). Gender agreement in adult second language learners and Spanish heritage speakers: The effects of age and context of acquisition. Language Learning, 58, 503553.
Moreno, E. M., & Kutas, M. (2005). Processing semantic anomalies in two languages: An electrophysiological exploration in both languages of Spanish-English bilinguals. Cognitive Brain Research, 22, 205220.
Morgan-Short, K., Sanz, C., Steinhauer, K., & Ullman, M. T. (2010). Acquisition of gender agreement in second language learners: An event-related potential study. Language Learning, 60, 154193.
Osterhout, L., & Holcomb, P. J. (1992). Event-related brain potentials elicited by syntactic anomaly. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 785806.
Potter, M. C., So, K.-F., Von Eckardt, B., & Feldman, L. B. (1984). Lexical and conceptual representation in beginning and proficient bilinguals. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 2338.
Sabourin, L., & Stowe, L. A. (2008). Second language processing: When are first and second languages processed similarly? Second Language Research, 24, 397430.
Sabourin, L., Stowe, L. A., & de Haan, G. J. (2006). Transfer effects in learning a second language grammatical gender system. Second Language Research, 22, 129.
Saslow, M. G. (1967). Latency for saccadic eye movement. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 57, 10301033.
Schiller, N. (2009, July). Grammatical gender in L2 from a processing perspective. Paper presented at the 7th International Symposium on Bilingualism, Utretch, Netherlands.
Schmidt, R. (1986). Was weiss der Artikel vom Hauptwort? Ein Beitrag zur Verarbeitung syntaktischer Beziehungen beim Lesen [What does the article know about the noun? A study of syntactic agreement processing during reading]. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie, 23, 150163.
Schwartz, B. D., & Sprouse, R. A. (1994). Word order and nominative case in non-native language acquisition: A longitudinal study of (L1 Turkish) German interlanguage. In Hoekstra, T. & Schwartz, B. D. (Eds.), Language acquisition studies in generative grammar (pp. 317368). Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Schwartz, B. D., & Sprouse, R. A. (1996). L2 cognitive states and the full transfer/full access model. Second Language Research, 12, 4072.
Tanenhaus, M. K., & Spivey-Knowlton, M. J. (1996). Eye-tracking. Language and Cognitive Processes, 11, 583588.
Tanenhaus, M. K., Spivey-Knowlton, M. J., Eberhard, K. M., & Sedivy, J. C. (1995). Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehension. Science, 286, 16321634.
Tanenhaus, M. K., & Trueswell, J. C. (2006). Eye movement and spoken language comprehension. In Traxler, M. J. & Gernsbacher, M. A. (Eds.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 835862). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Trueswell, J. C., Sekerina, I., Hill, N. M., & Logrip, M. L. (1999). The kindergarten-path effect: Studying on-line sentence processing in young children. Cognition, 73, 89134.
van Berkum, J. J. A. (1996). The psycholinguistics of grammatical gender. Nijmegen, Netherlands: Nijmegen University Press.
White, L. (1989). Linguistic universals, markedness and learnability: Comparing two different approaches. Second Language Research, 5, 127140.
White, L. (2003). Second language acquisition and Universal Grammar. New York: Cambridge University Press.
White, L., Valenzuela, E., Kozlowska-MacGregor, M., & Leung, Y.-K. I. (2004). Gender and number agreement in nonnative Spanish. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 105133.
Wicha, N. Y. Y., Orozco-Figueroa, A., Reyes, I., Hernández, A., Gavaldón de Barreto, L., & Bates, E. (2005). When zebras become painted donkeys: Grammatical gender and semantic priming interact during picture integration in a spoken Spanish sentence. Language and Cognitive Processes, 20, 553587.

Metrics

Altmetric attention score

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between <date>. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed