Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-2lccl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T22:55:28.630Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

SECOND AND THIRD LANGUAGE LEARNERS’ SENSITIVITY TO JAPANESE PITCH ACCENT IS ADDITIVE

AN INFORMATION-BASED MODEL OF PITCH PERCEPTION

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 March 2019

Seth Wiener*
Affiliation:
Carnegie Mellon University
Seth Goss
Affiliation:
Emory University
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Seth Wiener, Department of Modern Languages, Carnegie Mellon University, 160 Baker Hall, 5000 Forbes Avenue, Pittsburgh, PA 15213. E-mail: sethw1@cmu.edu

Abstract

This study examines second (L2) and third (L3) language learners’ pitch perception. We test the hypothesis that a listener’s discrimination of and sensitivity (d’) to Japanese pitch accent reflects how pitch cues inform all words a listener knows in an additive, nonselective manner rather than how pitch cues inform words in a selective, Japanese-only manner. Six groups of listeners performed a speeded ABX discrimination task in Japanese. Groups were defined by their L1, L2, and L3 experience with the target language’s pitch cues (Japanese), a language with less informative pitch cues (English), or a language with more informative pitch cues (Mandarin Chinese). Results indicate that sensitivity to pitch is better modeled as a function of pitch’s informativeness across all languages a listener speaks. These findings support cue-centric views of perception and transfer, demonstrate potential advantageous transfer of tonal-L1/L2 speakers, and highlight the cumulative role that pitch plays in language learning.

Type
Research Report
Open Practices
Open materials
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

The authors thank Joy Maa and Zhe Gao for their help with the experiment and the anonymous reviewers for their incredibly valuable and insightful feedback on earlier versions of the manuscript.

The experiment in this article earned an Open Materials badge for transparent practices. The materials are available at https://osf.io/9328s/.

References

REFERENCES

Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 148.Google Scholar
Beckman, M. E. (1986). Stress and non-stress accent. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Fortis Publications.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beckman, M. E., & Pierrehumbert, J. B. (1986). Intonational structure in Japanese and English. Phonology, 3, 255309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bice, K., & Kroll, J. F. (2015). Native language change during early stages of second language learning. NeuroReport, 26, 966971.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2018). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer [Computer software]. Version 6.0.39. Retrieved from http://www.praat.org/.Google Scholar
Bohn, O. S., & Best, C. T. (2012). Native-language phonetic and phonological influences on perception of American English approximants by Danish and German listeners. Journal of Phonetics, 40, 109128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burnham, D., & Mattock, K. (2007). The perception of tones and phones. In Bohn, O-S. & Munro, M. J. (Eds.), Language experience in second language speech learning: In honor of James Emil Flege (pp. 259280). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chandrasekaran, B., Sampath, P. D., & Wong, P. C. (2010). Individual variability in cue-weighting and lexical tone learning. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 128, 456465.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chang, C. B. (2018). Perceptual attention as the locus of transfer to nonnative speech perception. Journal of Phonetics, 68, 85102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chang, C. B., & Mishler, A. (2012). Evidence for language transfer leading to a perceptual advantage for non-native listeners. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 132, 27002710.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chang, Y. H. S., Yao, Y., & Huang, B. H. (2017). Effects of linguistic experience on the perception of high-variability non-native tones. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 141, EL120126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cruttenden, A. (1997). Intonation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cutler, A., & Otake, T. (1999). Pitch accent in spoken-word recognition in Japanese. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 105, 18771888.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dijkstra, T., & Van Heuven, W. J. (2002). The architecture of the bilingual word recognition system: From identification to decision. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 5, 175197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E. (1995). Second language speech learning: Theory, findings, and problems. In Strange, W. (Ed.), Speech perception and linguistic experience: Issues in cross language research (pp. 233272). Baltimore, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Flynn, S., Foley, C., & Vinnitskaya, I. (2004). The cumulative-enhancement model for language acquisition: Comparing adults’ and children’s patterns of development in first, second and third language acquisition of relative clauses. International Journal of Multilingualism, 1, 316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goss, S. (2018). A critical pedagogy of lexical accent in L2 Japanese: Insights into research and practice. Japanese Language and Literature, 52, 126.Google Scholar
Goss, S., & Tamaoka, K. (2015). Predicting lexical accent perception in native Japanese speakers: An investigation of acoustic pitch sensitivity and working memory. Japanese Psychological Research, 57, 143154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, P., & MacLeod, C. J. (2016). SIMR: An R package for power analysis of generalized linear mixed models by simulation. Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 7, 493498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gussenhoven, C. (2004). The phonology of tone and intonation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hallé, P. A., Chang, Y. C., & Best, C. T. (2004). Identification and discrimination of Mandarin Chinese tones by Mandarin Chinese vs. French listeners. Journal of Phonetics, 32, 395421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hao, Y. C. (2012). Second language acquisition of Mandarin Chinese tones by tonal and non-tonal language speakers. Journal of Phonetics, 40, 269279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hasegawa, Y., & Hata, K. (1992). Fundamental frequency as an acoustic cue to accent perception. Language and Speech, 35, 8798.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hautus, M. J., & Meng, X. (2002). Decision strategies in the ABX (matching-to-sample) psychophysical task. Perception and Psychophysics, 64, 89106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirata, Y. (1999). Acquisition of Japanese rhythm and pitch accent by English native speakers (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Chicago.Google Scholar
Holt, L. L., & Lotto, A. J. (2006). Cue weighting in auditory categorization: Implications for first and second language acquisition. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 119, 30593071.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F., & Stewart, E. (1994). Category interference in translation and picture naming: Evidence for asymmetric connections between bilingual memory representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 149174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ladd, D. R. (2008). Intonational phonology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leather, J. (1987). F0 pattern inference in the perceptual acquisition of second language tone. In James, A. & Leather, J. (Eds.), Sound patterns in second language acquisition (pp. 5981). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Foris Publications.Google Scholar
Lenth, R. V. (2016). Least-squares means: The R package lsmeans. Journal of Statistical Software, 69, 133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mandell, J. (2018). Tonometric [computer software]. Retrieved from https://tonometric.com.Google Scholar
Maniwa, K. (2002). Acoustic and perceptual evidence of complete neutralization of word-final tonal specification in Japanese. Kansas Working Papers in Linguistics, 26, 93112.Google Scholar
McNicol, D. (2005). A primer of signal detection theory. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Minematsu, N., Hirano, H., Nakamura, N., & Oikawa, K. (2016). Improvement of naturalness of learners’ spoken Japanese by practicing with the web-based prosodic reading tutor, Suzuki-kun. Speech prosody 2016 (pp. 257261). Boston, MA.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ortega-Llebaria, M., Nemoga, M., & Presson, N. (2017). Long-term experience with a tonal language shapes the perception of intonation in English words: How Chinese-English bilinguals perceive “Rose?” vs. “Rose.” Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20, 367383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Otake, T., & Cutler, A. (1999). Perception of suprasegmental structure in a non-native dialect. Journal of Phonetics, 27, 229253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pelzl, E., Lau, E., Guo, T., & DeKeyser, R. (2018). Advanced second language learners’ perception of lexical tone contrasts. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263117000444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perrachione, T. K., Lee, J., Ha, L. Y., & Wong, P. C. (2011). Learning a novel phonological contrast depends on interactions between individual differences and training paradigm design. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 130, 461472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Qin, Z., & Jongman, A. (2016). Does second language experience modulate perception of tones in a third language? Language and Speech, 59, 318338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saito, K., & Wu, X. (2014). Communicative focus on form and second language suprasegmental learning: Teaching Cantonese learners to perceive Mandarin tones. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 36, 647680.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schaefer, V., & Darcy, I. (2014). Lexical function of pitch in the first language shapes cross-linguistic perception of Thai tones. Laboratory Phonology, 5, 489522.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shibata, T., & Hurtig, R. (2008). Prosody acquisition by Japanese learners. In Han, Z. (Ed.), Understanding second language process (pp. 176204). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Shibata, T., & Shibata, R. (1990). Akusento wa doo’ongo o dono teido bembetsu shiuruka: Nihongo, eigo chugokugo no ba’ai [Is word-accent significant in differentiating homonyms in Japanese, English, and Chinese?]. Keiryoo Kokugogaku [Mathematical Linguistics], 17, 317327.Google Scholar
Shook, A., & Marian, V. (2016). The influence of native-language tones on lexical access in the second language. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 139, 31023109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shport, I. A. (2008). Acquisition of Japanese pitch accent by American learners. In Heinrich, P. & Sugita, Y. (Eds.), Japanese as a foreign language in the age of globalization (pp. 165187). Munich, Germany: Iudicium Publishing.Google Scholar
Shport, I. A. (2015). Perception of acoustic cues to Tokyo Japanese pitch-accent contrasts in native Japanese and naïve English listeners. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 138, 307318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shport, I. A. (2016). Training English listeners to identify pitch-accent patterns in Tokyo Japanese. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 38, 739769.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
So, C. K., & Best, C. T. (2010). Cross-language perception of non-native tonal contrasts: Effects of native phonological and phonetic influences. Language and Speech, 53, 273293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
So, C. K., & Best, C. T. (2014). Phonetic influences on English and French listeners’ assimilation of Mandarin tones to native prosodic categories. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 36, 195221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spivey, M. J., & Marian, V. (1999). Cross talk between native and second languages: Partial activation of an irrelevant lexicon. Psychological Science, 10, 281284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stagray, J. R., & Downs, D. (1993 ). Differential sensitivity for frequency among speakers of a tone and a nontone language. Journal of Chinese Linguistics, 21, 143163.Google Scholar
Surendran, D., & Levow, G. A. (2004). The functional load of tone in Mandarin is as high as that of vowels. In Bel, B. & Marlien, I. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 2nd International Conference on Speech Prosody (pp. 99102). Nara, Japan.Google Scholar
Surendran, D., & Niyogi, P. (2006). Quantifying the functional load of phonemic oppositions, distinctive features, and suprasegmentals. In Thomsen, O. N. (Ed.), Competing models of linguistic change: Evolution and beyond (pp. 4358). Amsterdam, The Netherlands, and Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tamaoka, K., Saito, N., Kiyama, S., Timmer, K., & Verdonschot, R. G. (2014). Is pitch accent necessary for comprehension by native Japanese speakers? An ERP investigation. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 27, 3140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tatsuno, Y., & Sakai, K. L. (2005). Language-related activations in the left prefrontal regions are differentially modulated by age, proficiency, and task demands. Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 16371644.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tremblay, A., Broersma, M., & Coughlin, C. E. (2017). The functional weight of a prosodic cue in the native language predicts the learning of speech segmentation in a second language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21, 113.Google Scholar
Van Lancker, D. (1980). Cerebral lateralization of pitch cues in the linguistic signal. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 13, 201277.Google Scholar
Vance, T. (2008). The sounds of Japanese. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wang, X., Wang, J., & Malins, J. (2017). Do you hear “feather” when listening to “rain”? Lexical tone activation during unconscious translation: Evidence from Mandarin-English bilinguals. Cognition, 169, 1524.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, Y., Spence, M. M., Jongman, A., & Sereno, J. A. (1999). Training American listeners to perceive Mandarin tones. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 106, 36493658.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wayland, R. P., & Guion, S. G. (2004). Training English and Chinese listeners to perceive Thai tones: A preliminary report. Language Learning, 54, 681712.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wayland, R. P., & Li, B. (2008). Effects of two training procedures in cross-language perception of tones. Journal of Phonetics, 36, 250267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wedel, A., Kaplan, A., & Jackson, S. (2013). High functional load inhibits phonological contrast loss: A corpus study. Cognition, 128, 179186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wong, P. C., & Perrachione, T. K. (2007). Learning pitch patterns in lexical identification by native English-speaking adults. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 565585.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wu, X., Kawase, S., & Wang, Y. (2017). Effects of acoustic and linguistic experience on Japanese pitch accent processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20, 931946.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wu, X., Munro, M. J., & Wang, Y. (2014). Tone assimilation by Mandarin and Thai listeners with and without L2 experience. Journal of Phonetics, 46, 86100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wu, X., Tu, J. Y., & Wang, Y. (2012). Native and nonnative processing of Japanese pitch accent. Applied Psycholinguistics, 33, 623641.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wu, Y. J., Cristino, F., Leek, C., & Thierry, G. (2013). Non-selective lexical access in bilinguals is spontaneous and independent of input monitoring: Evidence from eye tracking. Cognition, 129, 418425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yip, M. (2002). Tone. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Wiener and Goss supplementary material

Table S1

Download Wiener and Goss supplementary material(File)
File 18 KB