Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-vpsfw Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-18T15:02:19.478Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Preemption and the Learning of L2 Grammars

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 November 2008

William E. Rutherford
Affiliation:
University of Southern California

Abstract

Although most of the theoretical work in second language acquisition (SLA) over the years has led to advancement of theories of developing grammars, some recent SLA research has begun to investigate how those grammars are actually learned, relevant to current theories of learnability. This article (a) considers some of the claims for learnability principles that have been proposed within the first language (L1) context and the problems associated with their application to SLA, (b) examines four second language (L2) phenomena with respect to a promising variety of learnability theory labeled preemption, and (c) suggests in what ways research on learnability in SLA can contribute to the further development of learnability theory in general.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1989

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Adjémian, C. (1983). The transferability of lexical properties. In Gass, S. & Selinker, L. (Eds.), Language transfer in language learning (pp. 250268). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Andersen, R. (1983). Transfer to somewhere. In Gass, S. & Selinker, L. (Eds.), Language transfer in language learning (pp. 177201). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Andersen, R. (1984). The one-to-one principle of interlanguage construction. Language Learning, 34, 7795.Google Scholar
Bailey, N., Madden, C., & Krashen, S. (1974). Is there a “natural sequence” in adult second-language learning? Language Learning, 24, 235243.Google Scholar
Berwick, R. (1985). The acquisition of syntactic knowledge. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Berwick, R. (1987). Parsability and learnability. In MacWhinney, B. (Ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition (pp. 345365), Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Berwick, R., & Weinberg, A. (1984). The grammatical basis of linguistic performance. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bickerton, D. (1984). The language bioprogram hypothesis and second language acquisition. In Rutherford, W. (Ed.), Language universals and second language acquisition (pp. 141161). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Bley-Vroman, R. (1986). Hypothesis testing in second language acquisition theory. Language Learning, 36, 353376.Google Scholar
Bley-Vroman, R. (in press). The logical problem of foreign language learning. Linguistic Analysis.Google Scholar
Borer, H., & Wexler, K. (1987). The maturation of syntax. In Roeper, T. and Williams, E. (Eds.), Parameter setting (pp. 123172). Dordrecht: Reidei.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1987). Commentary. In MacWhinney, B. (Ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition (pp. 443465). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Braine, M. (1971). On two types of models of the internalization of grammars. In Slobin, D. (Ed.), The ontogenesis of grammar. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H. (1982). Spracherwerb in der Kindheit: Eine Untersuchung zur Entwicklung der Syntax bei Kleinkin-dern. Tübingen: Gunter Narr.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H. (1984). The acquisition of German word order: A test case for cognitive approaches to L2 development. In Andersen, R. (Ed.), Second languages: A cross-linguistic perspective (pp. 219242). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Clark, E. (1987). The principle of contrast: A constraint on language acquisition. In MacWhinney, B. (Ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition (pp. 133). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Corder, S. (1983). A role for the mother tongue. In Gass, S. & Selinker, L. (Eds.), Language transfer in language learning (pp. 8597). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Dulay, H., & Burt, M. (1974). Natural sequences in child second language acquisition. Language Learning, 24, 137153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dulay, H., Burt, M., & Krashen, S. (1982). Language two. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Faerch, C, & Kasper, G. (1987). Perspectives on language transfer. Applied Linguistics, 8, 111136.Google Scholar
Felix, S. (1984). The maturational aspects of Universal Grammar. In Davies, A., Criper, C., & Howatt, A. (Eds), Interlanguage (pp. 133161). Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Ferguson, C. (1984). Comments on paper by Bickerton. In Rutherford, W. (Ed.), Language universals and second language acquisition (pp. 162165). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Ferguson, C. (1985). Contrastive analysis: A linguistic hypothesis. In Jankowsky, K. (Ed.), Scientific and humanistic dimensions of language (pp. 199207). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flynn, S. (1987). A parameter-setting model of 12 acquisition. Dordrecht: Reidel.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1983). Modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. D., & Crain, S. (1987). Simplicity and generality of rules in language acquisition. In MacWhinney, B. (Ed.)., Mechanisms of language acquisition (pp. 3563). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gass, S., & Selinker, L. (Eds.). (1983). Language transfer in language learning. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Hawkins, J. (1987). Implicational universals as predictors of language acquisition. Linguistics, 25, 453473.Google Scholar
Hilles, S. (1986). Interlanguage and the pro-drop parameter. Second Language Research, 2, 3352.Google Scholar
Hyams, N. (1986). Language acquisition and the theory of parameters. Dordrecht: Reidel.Google Scholar
Jansen, B., Lalleman, J., & Muysken, P. (1981). The alternation hypothesis: Acquisition of Dutch word order by Turkish and Moroccan foreign workers. Language Learning, 31, 315336.Google Scholar
Jordens, P. (1983). Discourse functions in interlanguage morphology. In Gass, S. & Selinker, L. (Eds.), Language transfer in language learning (pp. 327357). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Kean, M.-L. (1984). Second language acquisition and grammatical theory: A matter of projection and marking. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Kellerman, E. (1983). Now you see it, now you don't. In Gass, S. & Selinker, L. (Eds.), Language transfer in language learning (pp. 112134). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Kellerman, E. (1984). The empirical evidence for the influence of LI in interlanguage. In Davies, A., Criper, C., & Howatt, A. (Eds.), Interlanguage (pp. 98122). Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Klein, W. (1986). Second language acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Long, M. (1985). Input and second language acquisition theory. In Gass, S. & Madden, C. (Eds.), Input in second language acquisition (pp. 377393). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (1987). The competition model. In MacWhinney, B. (Ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition (pp. 249308). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Naro, A. (1978). A study on the origins of pidginization. Language, 54, 314347.Google Scholar
Phinney, M. (1987). The pro-drop parameter in second language acquisition. In Roeper, T. & Williams, E. (Eds.), Parameter setting (pp. 221238). Dordrecht: Reidel.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1984). Language learnability and language development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1986). Productivity and conservatism in language acquisition. In Demopoulos, W. & Marras, A. (Eds.), Language learning and concept acquisition (pp. 5479). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1987). The bootstrapping problem in language acquisition. In MacWhinney, B. (Ed.), Mechanisms of anguage acquisition (pp. 399441). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Roeper, T, Lapointe, S., Bing, J., & Tavakolian, S. (1981). A lexical approach to language acquisition. In Tavakolian, S. (Ed.), Language acquisition and linguistic theory (pp. 3558). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Rutherford, W. (1987). Second language grammar: Learning and teaching. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Safir, K. (1987). Comments on Wexler and Manzini. In Roeper, T. & Williams, E. (Eds.), Parameter setting (pp. 7789). Dordrecht: Reidel.Google Scholar
Scarcella, R. (1984). Cohesion in the writing development of native and non-native English speakers. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Southern California, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Schachter, J. (1983). A new account of language transfer. In Gass, S. & Selinker, L. (Eds.), Language transfer in language learning (pp. 98111). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Schwartz, B. (1987). The epistemological status of second language acquisition. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Southern California, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Selinker, L. (1972). Interlanguage. International Review of Applied Linguistics, 10, 209231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Selinker, L, Swain, M., & Dumas, G. (1975). The interlanguage hypothesis extended to children. Language Learning, 25, 139152.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. (1985). The cross-cultural study of language acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Stowell, T. (1981). Origins of phrase structure. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, M.I.T., Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Wexler, K., & Culicover, P. (1980). Formal principles of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wexler, K., & Manzini, M. (1987). Parameters and learnability in binding theory. In Roeper, T. & Williams, E. (Eds.), Parameter setting (pp. 4176). Dordrecht: Reidel.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, L. (1985a). The acquisition of parameterized grammars: Subjacency in second language acquisition. Second Language Research, 1, 117.Google Scholar
White, L. (1985b). The “Pro-Drop” parameter in second language acquisition. Language Learning, 35, 4762.Google Scholar
White, L. (1987a). A note on Phinney. In Roeper, T. & Williams, E. (Eds.), Parameter setting (pp. 239246). Dordrecht: Reidel.Google Scholar
White, L. (1987b, February). Universal grammar: Is it just a new name for old problems? Paper presented at the Second Language Research Forum, University of Southern California, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
White, L. (in press). The principle of adjacency in second language acquisition. In Gass, S. & Schachter, J. (Eds.), Second language acquisition: A linguistic perspective. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Zobl, H. (1987a, February). Categorial distinctions and the problem of overgeneration. Paper presented at the Second Language Research Forum, University of Southern California, Los Angeles.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zobl, H. (1987b). Configurationality and the Subset Principle: The acquisition of V1 by Japanese learners of English. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar