Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-sh8wx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-19T23:58:12.675Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A test of the repression hypothesis in agoraphobics

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 July 2009

Willem A. Arrindell*
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Psychology, Academic Hospital of the State University of Groningen, The Netherlands
Paul M. G. Emmelkamp
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Psychology, Academic Hospital of the State University of Groningen, The Netherlands
*
1 Address for correspondence: Mr Willem A. Arrindell, Department of Clinical Psychology, Academic Hospital of the State University of Groningen, Oostersingel 59, 9713 EZ Groningen, The Netherlands.

Synopsis

In a recent study Turner et al. (1983) employed Bell & Byrne's (1978) Repression-Sensitization (R–S) scale to test the hypothesis that agoraphobics utilize repression as a method of avoiding anxiety-arousing forms of cognition. However, no support was found for this view. Rather, the R–S scores indicated general sensitization and attention to negative affect. In this paper it is argued that, on both theoretical and psychometric grounds, the R–S scale cannot be considered a suitable measure of the repression–sensitization process. A study was carried out in which a number of other defensivity measures were used in addition to the R–S scale. Weak support was found for Goldstein & Chambless’ (1978) view that a defensive cognitive style is characteristic of agoraphobic patients when compared with non-phobic psychiatric controls.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1985

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Appels, A. (1975). Screenen als methode voor preventie in de geestelijke gezondheidszorg; tevens handleiding bij de Delftse Vragenlijst. Swets & Zeitlinger: Amsterdam.Google Scholar
Bell, P. A. & Byrne, D. (1978). Repression–sensitization. In Dimensions of Personality (ed. London, H. and Exner, J. E. Jr), pp. 449485. Wiley: New York.Google Scholar
Carlson, R. W. (1979). Dimensionality of the repression sensitization scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology 35, 7884.3.0.CO;2-U>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chambless, D. L. (1982). Characteristics of agoraphobics. In Agoraphobia. Multiple Perspectives on Theory and Treatment (ed. Chambless, D. L. and Goldstein, A. J.), pp. 118. Wiley: New York.Google Scholar
Chambless, D. L. & Goldstein, A. J. (1980). Anxieties: agoraphobia and hysteria. In Women and Psychotherapy. An Assessment of Research and Practice (ed. Brodsky, A. M. and Hare-Mustin, R. T.), pp. 113134. Guilford Press: New York.Google Scholar
Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (revised edn). Academic Press: New York.Google Scholar
Crowne, D. P. & Marlowe, D. (1964). The Approval Motive. A Study in Evaluative Dependence. Wiley: New York.Google Scholar
Evans, R. G. (1982). Clinical relevance of the Marlowe–Crowne Scale: a review and recommendations. Journal of Personality Assessment 46, 415425.Google Scholar
Fenichel, O. (1945). The Psychoanalytic Theory of Neurosis. Norton: New York.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1963). Analysis of a phobia in a five-year-old boy. In The Sexual Enlightenment of Children. Collier: New York.Google Scholar
Goldstein, A. J. & Chambless, D. L. (1978). A reanalysis of agoraphobia. Behavior Therapy 9, 4759.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heilbrun, A. B. Jr (1978). Projective and repressive styles of processing a versive information. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 46, 156164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Highland, A. C. (1980). Confounding of the Repression–Sensitization Scale, controlled for Social Desirability, with the IPAT Anxiety Scale. Psychological Reports 47, 10031006.Google Scholar
Millham, J. & Jacobson, L. I. (1978). The need for approval. In Dimensions of Personality (ed. London, H. and Exner, J. E. Jr), pp. 365390. Wiley: New York.Google Scholar
Nemiah, J. (1977). Alexithymia: theoretical considerations. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics 28, 199206.Google Scholar
Rimé, B. & Bonami, M. (1979). Overt and covert personality traits associated with coronary heart disease. British Journal of Medical Psychology 52, 7784.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salzman, L. (1982). Obsessions and agoraphobia. In Agoraphobia. Multiple Perspectives on Theory and Treatment (ed. Chambless, D. L. and Goldstein, A. J.), pp. 1942. Wiley: New York.Google Scholar
Seif, M. N. & Atkins, A. L. (1979). Some defensive and cognitive aspects of phobias. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 88, 4251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turner, R. M., Giles, T. R. & Marafiote, R. (1983). Agoraphobics: a test of the repression hypothesis. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 22, 7576.Google Scholar
van Rooijen, L. & Smoor-van Son, L. (1978). De VROPSOM-O en een Sociale Wenselijkheidsschaal: Resultaten uit een Eenzaamheidsonderzoek. Onderzoeksmemorandum RM–PS 78−02. Vakgroep Sociale Psychologie der Vrije Universiteit: Amsterdam.Google Scholar
Weinberger, D. A., Schwartz, G. E. & Davidson, R. J. (1979). Low-anxious, high-anxious, and repressive coping styles: psychometric patterns and behavioral and physiological responses to stress. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 88, 369380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed