Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-skm99 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T17:51:28.616Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Object chaining and thought disorder in schizophrenic speech

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 July 2009

T. C. Manschreck*
Affiliation:
Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Harvard University, Boston, USA
B. Maher
Affiliation:
Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Harvard University, Boston, USA
M. T. Celada
Affiliation:
Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Harvard University, Boston, USA
M. Schneyer
Affiliation:
Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Harvard University, Boston, USA
R. Fernandez
Affiliation:
Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Harvard University, Boston, USA
*
1Address for correspondence: Dr Theo C. Manschreck, New Hampshire Hospital, Dartmouth Medical School, 105 Pleasant Street, Concord, New Hampshire 03301, USA.

Synopsis

The phenomenon of object chaining was investigated to determine its relationship to thought disorder in schizophrenia. Samples from thought-disordered schizophrenics (N = 12) and controls (10 non-thought-disordered schizophrenics and 10 normals) were analysed. Using the object subject ratio (OSR) to measure object chaining, we found higher OSRs in the speech of thought-disordered subjects than in that of subjects free of thought disorder. Object chaining correlated with low predictability of speech. We conclude that object chaining is associated with reduced speech comprehensibility and probably contributes to the judgement that thought disorder is present.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1991

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bleuler, E. (1911, 1950). Dementia Praecox or the Group of Schizophrenias, International Universities Press: New York.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1954). Language and Thought. Prentice-Hall: Englewood Cliffs, NJ.Google Scholar
Crow, T. J. (1980). Molecular pathology of schizophrenia: more than one disease process? British Medical Journal 280, 6668.Google Scholar
Kraepelin, E. (1919). Dementia Praecox and Paraphrenia (translated by Barclay, R. M.). Krieger: Huntington, NY. 1971.Google Scholar
Maher, B. (1983). Towards a Tentative Theory of Schizophrenic Language. Progress in Experimental Personality Research, Vol. XII, pp. 151. Academic Press, New York.Google Scholar
Maher, B., McKean, K. & McLaughlin, B. (1966). Studies in psychotic language. In The General Inquirer: A Computer Approach to Content Analysis. (ed. Stone, P. J., Dunphy, D. C., Smith, M. S. and Ogilvie, D. M.), pp. 469503. MIT Press: Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Maher, B. A., Manschreck, T. C., Hoover, T. M. & Weisstein, C. C. (1987). Thought disorder and measured features of language production in schizophrenia. In Positive and Negative Symptoms in Psychosis (ed. Harvey, P. and Walker, E.), pp. 195215. Erlbourn: Hillsboro, NJ.Google Scholar
Maher, B., Manschreck, T. C., Weisstein, C., Schneyer, M. & Okunieff, R. (1988). Cloze analysis in schizophrenic speech: scoring method and raters' education. Perceptual and Motor Skills 67, 911918.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manschreck, T. C., Hoover, T. M., Maher, B. A. & Ames, D. (1985). Repetition in schizophrenic speech. Language and Speech 28, 255268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manschreck, T. C., Maher, B., Rucklos, M. & White, M. (1979). The predictability of thought disordered speech in schizophrenic patients. British Journal of Psychiatry 134, 595601.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spitzer, R. & Endicott, J. (1977). Schedule of Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia (SADS), (2nd ed.). Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute: New York.Google Scholar