Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-qxdb6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T07:51:35.261Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Letter to the Editor: Multifaceted impairments of impulsivity in cannabis users?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 July 2013

JOHANNES WREGE
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Basel, Switzerland
STEFAN BORGWARDT*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Basel, Switzerland
*
Author for correspondence: Stefan Borgwardt, Professor for Neuropsychiatry, Department of Psychiatry (UPK), University of Basel, c/o University Hospital, Petersgraben 4, 4031 Basel, Switzerland. (Email: Stefan.Borgwardt@upkbs.ch)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Correspondence
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

With great interest, we read the article by Huddy et al. (Reference Huddy, Clark, Harrison, Ron, Moutoussis, Barnes and Joyce2013) recently published by Psychological Medicine. The authors aimed to shed empirical light on the co-morbidity of first episode psychosis (FEP) and cannabis consumption with respect to two subdomains of impulsivity – response inhibition and reflection impulsivity. It was shown that patients with FEP had significantly greater impairment in response inhibition but not in reflection impulsivity compared to healthy controls. By contrast, patients with current cannabis use had greater reflection impulsivity but no impairments in response inhibition.

Over the past years an increasing number of studies have shown that impaired aspects of impulsivity like response inhibition are found across multiple substances with different pharmacological effects (Verdejo-Garcia et al. Reference Verdejo-Garcia, Lawrence and Clark2008; Solowij et al. Reference Solowij, Jones, Rozman, Davis, Ciarrochi, Heaven, Pesa, Lubman and Yücel2012) and across several neuropsychiatric diseases (Fontanelle et al. Reference Fontanelle, Oostermeijer, Harrison, Pantelis and Yücel2011). In particular, there is a growing body of evidence for impaired impulsivity FEP on the one hand and complex associations of different aspects of impulsivity with cannabis consumption on the other. Interestingly, the study by Huddy et al. (Reference Huddy, Clark, Harrison, Ron, Moutoussis, Barnes and Joyce2013) fails to show differences in response inhibition in cannabis users compared to drug-naive and discontinued users. It was concluded that abnormal reflection impulsivity is associated with substance use in psychosis but not psychosis itself; the opposite relationship may hold for response inhibition.

The findings by Huddy et al. are in line with previous evidence of non-acute studies with different abstinence periods starting from 17 hours to more than a year. No impairments of cognitive inhibition among recreational (Griffith-Lendering et al. Reference Griffith-Lendering, Huijbregts, Vollebergh and Swaab2012) and long-term users (Pope et al. Reference Pope, Gruber, Hudson, Huestis and Yurgelund-Todd2002) have been shown. Interestingly, performance of the Stroop task was affected by marijuana use only in individuals with lower cognitive reserves (Bolla et al. Reference Bolla, Brown, Eldreth, Tate and Cadet2002) while cannabis users in another study did not differ significantly from controls, but were vulnerable to task complexity with increasing demands creating more sources of interference (Solowij et al. Reference Solowij, Stephens, Roffman and Babor2002). Moreno et al. (Reference Moreno, Estevez, Zaldivar, Montes, Gutierrez-Ferre, Esteban, Sanchez Santed and Flores2012) did find significantly different inhibitory control in recreational users with Stroop and Go/No-Go; and heavy users in Pope & Yurgelund-Todd (Reference Pope and Yurgelund-Todd1996) exhibited more errors of inhibition than light users. In contrast, clear impairments among cannabis users of this aspect of impulsivity have been shown after acute administration (McDonald et al. Reference McDonald, Schleifer, Richards and de Wit2003; Metrik et al. Reference Metrik, Kahler, Reynolds, McGeary, Monti, Haney, De Wit and Rohsenow2012).

While reflection inhibition in FEP in Huddy et al. (Reference Huddy, Clark, Harrison, Ron, Moutoussis, Barnes and Joyce2013) is not significantly impaired compared to healthy controls, this is the case for cannabis users compared to non-users. Co-morbid continued usage in FEP shows earlier onset of psychosis and cannabis consumption, increased daily usage, more abuse of other drugs and tends to increase positive symptoms, interpretable as being more prone to jumping to conclusions. Reflection inhibition is a failure of pre-decisional information sampling and evaluation of situations (Solowij et al. Reference Solowij, Jones, Rozman, Davis, Ciarrochi, Heaven, Pesa, Lubman and Yücel2012). Thus, it putatively influences decision making, which is impaired among acutely intoxicated cannabis users (Ramaekers et al. Reference Ramaekers, Kauert, van Ruitenbeek, Theunissen, Schneider and Moeller2006; Vadhan et al. Reference Vadhan, Hart, van Gorp, Gunderson, Haney and Foltin2007) as well as after a time of abstinence (Griffith-Lendering et al. Reference Griffith-Lendering, Huijbregts, Vollebergh and Swaab2012; Moreno et al. Reference Moreno, Estevez, Zaldivar, Montes, Gutierrez-Ferre, Esteban, Sanchez Santed and Flores2012). However, whether impulsivity is the consequence of prolonged drug abuse or simply a personality trait leading to discontinuation difficulties, is still open to debate.

Although neuroimaging studies have provided compelling evidence for cannabis-related effects on brain structure and functioning (Martín-Santos et al. Reference Martín-Santos, Fagundo, Crippa, Atakan, Bhattacharyya, Allen, Fusar-Poli, Borgwardt, Seal, Busatto and McGuire2010), it still remains to be seen whether there are any specific effects on response inhibition. In conclusion, we suggest more studies like Huddy and colleagues (2013) as well as longitudinal and prospective designs are needed to investigate whether specific aspects of impulsivity could be considered neuropsychiatric endophenotypes of comorbid psychiatric disorders.

Declaration of Interest

None.

References

Bolla, KI, Brown, K, Eldreth, D, Tate, K, Cadet, JL (2002). Dose-related neurocognitive effects of marijuana use. Neurology 59, 13371343.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fontanelle, LF, Oostermeijer, S, Harrison, B, Pantelis, C, Yücel, M (2011). Obsessive-compulsive disorder, impulse control disorders and drug addiction. Drugs 71, 827840.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffith-Lendering, MF, Huijbregts, SC, Vollebergh, WA, Swaab, H (2012). Motivational and cognitive inhibitory control in recreational cannabis users. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 34, 688697.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huddy, VC, Clark, L, Harrison, I, Ron, MA, Moutoussis, M, Barnes, TRE, Joyce, EM (2013). Reflection impulsivity and response inhibition in first-episode psychosis: relationship to cannabis use. Psychological Medicine. Published online: 23 January 2013. doi:10.1017/S0033291712003054.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martín-Santos, R, Fagundo, AB, Crippa, JA, Atakan, Z, Bhattacharyya, S, Allen, P, Fusar-Poli, P, Borgwardt, S, Seal, M, Busatto, GF, McGuire, P (2010). Neuroimaging in cannabis use: a systematic review of the literature. Psychological Medicine 40, 383398.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDonald, J, Schleifer, L, Richards, JB, de Wit, H (2003). Effects of THC on behavioral measures of impulsivity in humans. Neuropsychopharmacology 28, 13561365.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Metrik, J, Kahler, CW, Reynolds, B, McGeary, JE, Monti, PM, Haney, M, De Wit, H, Rohsenow, DJ (2012). Balanced placebo design with marijuana: pharmacological and expectancy effects on impulsivity and risk taking. Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 223, 489499.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moreno, M, Estevez, AF, Zaldivar, F, Montes, JM, Gutierrez-Ferre, VE, Esteban, L, Sanchez Santed, F, Flores, P (2012). Impulsivity differences in recreational cannabis users and binge drinkers in a university population. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 124, 355362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pope, HG Jr., Gruber, AJ, Hudson, JI, Huestis, MA, Yurgelund-Todd, D (2002). Cognitive measures in long-term cannabis users. Journal of Clinical Pharmacology 42, 4147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pope, HG Jr., Yurgelund-Todd, D (1996). The residual cognitive effects of heavy marijuana use in college students. Journal of the American Medical Association 275, 521527.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramaekers, JG, Kauert, G, van Ruitenbeek, P, Theunissen, EL, Schneider, E, Moeller, MR (2006). High-potency marijuana impairs executive function and inhibitory motor control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 31, 22962303.Google ScholarPubMed
Solowij, N, Jones, KA, Rozman, ME, Davis, SM, Ciarrochi, J, Heaven, PC, Pesa, N, Lubman, DI, Yücel, M (2012). Reflection impulsivity in adolescent cannabis users: a comparison with alcohol-using and non-substance-using adolescents. Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 219, 575586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solowij, N, Stephens, R, Roffman, RA, Babor, T (2002). Does marijuana use cause long-term cognitive deficits? Journal of the American Medical Association 287, 26532654.Google ScholarPubMed
Vadhan, NP, Hart, CL, van Gorp, WG, Gunderson, EW, Haney, M, Foltin, RW (2007). Acute effects of smoked marijuana on decision making, as assessed by a modified gambling task, in experienced marijuana users. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 29, 357364.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verdejo-Garcia, A, Lawrence, AJ, Clark, L (2008). Impulsivity as a vulnerability marker for substance-use disorders: Review of findings from high-risk research, problem gamblers and genetic association studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 32, 777810.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed