Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-fqc5m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-19T04:46:14.771Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Impaired awareness of motor intention in functional neurological disorder: implications for voluntary and functional movement

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 February 2017

K. Baek
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
N. Doñamayor
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
L. S. Morris
Affiliation:
Behavioural and Clinical Neurosciences Institute, Cambridge, UK
D. Strelchuk
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
S. Mitchell
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
Y. Mikheenko
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
S. Y. Yeoh
Affiliation:
School of Clinical Medicine, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
W. Phillips
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Addenbrooke's Hospital, Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
M. Zandi
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Addenbrooke's Hospital, Cambridge, Cambridge, UK Department of Molecular Neuroscience, UCL Institute of Neurology, London, UK National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery, UCLH NIHR Biomedical Research Centre, London, UK
A. Jenaway
Affiliation:
Cambridgeshire and Peterborough NHS Foundation Trust, Cambridge, UK
C. Walsh
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Addenbrooke's Hospital, Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
V. Voon*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK Behavioural and Clinical Neurosciences Institute, Cambridge, UK Cambridgeshire and Peterborough NHS Foundation Trust, Cambridge, UK NIHR Biomedical Research Council, Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
*
*Address for correspondence: V. Voon, M.D., Ph.D., Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Addenbrooke's Hospital, Level E4, Box 189, Cambridge CB2 0QQ, UK. (Email: vv247@cam.ac.uk)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Functional neurological disorders (FNDs), also known as conversion disorder, are unexplained neurological symptoms unrelated to a neurological cause. The disorder is common, yet poorly understood. The symptoms are experienced as involuntary but have similarities to voluntary processes. Here we studied intention awareness in FND.

Method

A total of 26 FND patients and 25 healthy volunteers participated in this functional magnetic resonance study using Libet's clock.

Results

FND is characterized by delayed awareness of the intention to move relative to the movement itself. The reporting of intention was more precise, suggesting that these findings are reliable and unrelated to non-specific attentional deficits. That these findings were more prominent with aberrant positive functional movement symptoms rather than negative symptoms may be relevant to impairments in timing for an inhibitory veto process. Attention towards intention relative to movement was associated with lower right inferior parietal cortex activity in FND, a region early in the processing of intention. During rest, aberrant functional connectivity was observed with the right inferior parietal cortex and other motor intention regions.

Conclusions

The results converge with observations of low inferior parietal activity comparing involuntary with voluntary movement in FND, emphasizing core deficiencies in intention. Heightened precision of this impaired intention is consistent with Bayesian theories of impaired top-down priors that might influence the sense of involuntariness. A primary impairment in voluntary motor intention at an early processing stage might explain clinical observations of slowed effortful voluntary movement, heightened self-directed attention and underlie functional movements. These findings further suggest novel therapeutic targets.

Type
Original Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017

Introduction

Functional neurological disorder (FND), or conversion disorder, refers to neurological symptoms in the absence of a neurological condition (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). The disorder is common (Carson et al. Reference Carson, Ringbauer, Stone, McKenzie, Warlow and Sharpe2000) and equally physically debilitating as Parkinson's disease, with greater impact on mental health and quality of life (Anderson et al. Reference Anderson, Gruber-Baldini, Vaughan, Reich, Fishman, Weiner and Shulman2007; Voon et al. Reference Voon, Cavanna, Coburn, Sampson, Reeve and LaFrance2016). Still, it remains poorly understood. Symptoms are experienced as involuntary but have similar physiological features as voluntary movements (Hallett, Reference Hallett2010). An early crucial study hypothesized that a disturbance of volition might underlie FND (Spence et al. Reference Spence, Crimlisk, Cope, Ron and Grasby2000). Here we focus on the awareness of motor intention as one aspect of volition, using Libet's clock paradigm (Libet et al. Reference Libet, Gleason, Wright and Pearl1983).

Libet et al. (Reference Libet, Gleason, Wright and Pearl1983) reported that awareness of the urge or intention to move (W judgement) preceded awareness of the movement itself (M judgement) by around 200 ms. Intention awareness was itself preceded by the Bereitschaftspotential, i.e. readiness potential, suggesting an unconscious initiation of the volitional process and constraining the potential for action control (Libet et al. Reference Libet, Gleason, Wright and Pearl1983; Libet, Reference Libet1999). This has been widely replicated (Lau et al. Reference Lau, Rogers, Haggard and Passingham2004; Brass & Haggard, Reference Brass and Haggard2007; Fried et al. Reference Fried, Mukamel and Kreiman2011). However, Libet emphasized the potential role for action control and the relevance of the interval between W and M judgements in the veto hypothesis: despite unconscious action initiation, action control would be plausible in that interval, allowing for a veto or inhibitory process (Libet, Reference Libet1999; Haggard & Libet, Reference Haggard and Libet2001). Delays in W relative to M judgement have been reported in Parkinson's disease (Tabu et al. Reference Tabu, Aso, Matsuhashi, Ueki, Takahashi, Fukuyama, Shibasaki and Mima2015) and Tourette's syndrome (Moretto et al. Reference Moretto, Schwingenschuh, Katschnig, Bhatia and Haggard2011), suggesting impairments in action control.

The supplementary motor complex (SMC) and inferior parietal cortex (IPC) have been identified as key regions underlying motor intention. Epilepsy patients reported the urge to move when supplementary and pre-supplementary motor areas (SMA, pre-SMA) were electrically stimulated (Fried et al. Reference Fried, Katz, McCarthy, Sass, Williamson, Spencer and Spencer1991), and single-neuron recordings showed increased firing as the W judgement approached (Fried et al. Reference Fried, Mukamel and Kreiman2011). Attention to intention with Libet's clock also activates the pre-SMA, intraparietal sulcus and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) (Lau et al. Reference Lau, Rogers, Haggard and Passingham2004). The IPC has been suggested to play a role upstream of the SMC in the development of motor intention with evidence from electrical stimulation, stroke lesion and motor imagery studies (Sirigu et al. Reference Sirigu, Daprati, Ciancia, Giraux, Nighoghossian, Posada and Haggard2004; Desmurget et al. Reference Desmurget, Reilly, Richard, Szathmari, Mottolese and Sirigu2009; Desmurget & Sirigu, Reference Desmurget and Sirigu2012).

Converging studies implicate impairments in explicit intentional processes in FND. An early study of functional paralysis demonstrated impaired dlPFC activity to attempted movement, which was linked to a disturbance of volition (Spence et al. Reference Spence, Crimlisk, Cope, Ron and Grasby2000). Functional compared with voluntary tremor has been associated with right temporoparietal junction (TPJ)/IPC hypoactivity, a region implicated in sensorimotor integration (Voon et al. Reference Voon, Gallea, Hattori, Bruno, Ekanayake and Hallett2010). The authors suggested a possible impairment of intentional or prediction processes, as sensory regions were intact. A follow-up study demonstrated SMA hypoactivity in FND subjects during motor preparation, as well as decreased dlPFC–SMA connectivity during internally generated v. externally cued action selection (Voon et al. Reference Voon, Brezing, Gallea and Hallett2011). Using Libet's clock, a small study reported that FND patients with functional tremor (n = 11) had significantly delayed W relative to M judgements compared with healthy volunteers (HV) (Edwards et al. Reference Edwards, Moretto, Schwingenschuh, Katschnig, Bhatia and Haggard2011), such that the timing of both judgements was indistinguishable, implying abnormalities in the conscious experience of action underlying functional movements.

In this experiment, we aimed to explore awareness of voluntary motor intention in a larger sample of FND patients with mixed symptoms and HV. We acquired functional magnetic resonance images (fMRI) while participants performed the task devised by Libet et al. (Reference Libet, Gleason, Wright and Pearl1983). We hypothesized that FND patients would have delayed motor intention awareness, and lower IPC and SMC activity compared with HV.

Method

Participants

A total of 26 FND patients and 25 HV took part in the study, and one FND patient and one HV were later excluded due to interruption of the experiment during the scan. FND subjects were referred by neurologists and psychiatrists from Addenbrooke's Hospital, Cambridge, and recruited through the FND Hope website (http://fndhope.org/). The diagnosis was based on Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth edition (DSM-5) diagnostic criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) and either made or confirmed by a neurologist from the FND clinic (W.P., M.Z.). Participants were screened by psychiatrists for psychiatric co-morbidities and to document symptom severity (S.M., V.V.). FND subjects with any other major neurological or psychiatric diagnosis, including current major depression greater than moderate severity [Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) > 17], psychotic or bipolar disorder and substance use disorder were excluded from the study. Current mild depression and elevated depression scores with no current major depression diagnosis were allowed. Recruitment was limited by subjects with head movements, who had ballistic movements, were unable to remain still in the scanner or had claustrophobia. HV were recruited via community advertisements. Participants gave written informed consent and were reimbursed for their time. All experimental procedures were approved by the University of Cambridge Research Ethics Committee.

Participants completed the BDI-II (Beck et al. Reference Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock and Erbaugh1961) and Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI; Spielberger et al. Reference Spielberger, Gorsuch, Lushene, Vagg and Jacobs1983). Pain, motor (paralysis or weakness, non-epileptic seizures, tremor, chorea, tics, gait abnormalities, dystonia, myoclonus) and sensory (somatosensory, vision, hearing) symptoms were recorded based on clinical interview by systematic enquiry, and rating of duration and severity [1 = mild (limited impact on daily functioning); 2 = moderate (noticeable impact on daily functioning with restriction of some activities); 3 = severe (marked impact on daily functioning with restriction of activity in multiple domains); 4 = very severe (impairment in all or virtually all domains of activity)].

Stimuli and procedure

In the Libet's clock task (Libet et al. Reference Libet, Gleason, Wright and Pearl1983; Lau et al. Reference Lau, Rogers, Haggard and Passingham2004) participants watched a red ball revolving around an unnumbered clock face at 2500 ms per cycle (Fig. 1 a). Participants were instructed to press a button with their left index finger after a random time interval after waiting for one cycle. They were asked to act as spontaneously as possible and in particular to avoid preselecting a position of the ball to trigger the button press. If there was no button press within three cycles the trial was recorded as missed.

Fig. 1. Libet's clock task. (a) Schematic representation of the task. The red ball revolved around the unnumbered clock face for a maximum of three cycles; participants had to make a button press after waiting one cycle. The ball continued moving for a random interval, after which participants returned the ball to its position when they had felt the urge (W judgement) or actually pressed the button (M judgement). (b) Estimated times of intention (W judgement), movement (M judgement) and difference between intention and movement (W-M) relative to the recorded button press for functional neurological disorder (FND) patients and healthy controls (HV). Values are means, with standard errors represented by the horizontal bars. ++ p = 0.017, * p = 0.009, ** p = 0.001. For a colour figure, see the online version of the paper.

The task consisted of two types of trials. In intention trials, participants were asked to attend to when they felt the urge to press the button (W judgement). In movement trials, they were required to attend to when they actually pressed the button (M judgement). After the button press, the ball continued moving for 1000–2500 ms (0.4–1 revolution). Then, it appeared on a random angle on the clock face, and participants were required to return it to the position it was in when they first felt the urge to press the button, or actually pressed it, depending on the type of trial. The final position of the red ball was recorded if still for 2 s or after the 7 s period for responding elapsed. The inter-trial interval lasted 1–6 s. Average latencies of the W and M judgements were calculated as the differences between the final position of the ball and its position at the time of the recorded button press.

Prior to fMRI data acquisition, participants took part in a brief practice session of 10 movement followed by 10 intention trials. The fMRI task consisted of four blocks of 20 trials in the order of movement–intention–movement–intention. The fMRI experiment lasted around 18–21 min.

MRI data acquisition

MRI data were acquired with a 3 T Tim Trio scanner (Siemens, Germany) with a 32-channel head coil at the Wolfson Brain Imaging Centre, University of Cambridge. Foam pads were used to restrict head movements. Echo planar imaging (EPI) images were acquired in an interleaved fashion [repetition time (TR) = 2.32 s, echo time (TE) = 30 ms, 3 × 3 × 3 mm voxel size, 0.75 mm gap, flip angle (FA) = 78°, 64 × 64 matrix size, 39 slices]. Images were angled 30° to the anterior commissure–posterior commissure line in order to avoid susceptibility signal loss in the orbitofrontal regions. The T1-weighted structural images were acquired using a magnetization-prepared rapid gradient-echo (MPRAGE) sequence [TR = 2.30 s, TE = 2.98 ms, field of view (FOV) = 176 × 240 mm2, 1 × 1 × 1 mm3 voxel size, inversion time = 1100 ms].

Resting-state fMRI (rsfMRI) data were acquired after the fMRI task for 10 min with open eyes. A multi-echo EPI sequence was used with online reconstruction [TR = 2.47 s, FA = 78°, matrix size 64 × 64, 3.75 mm in-plane resolution, FOV = 240 mm, 32 oblique slices, alternating slice acquisition, slice thickness 3.75 mm with 10% gap, integrated parallel imaging technique (iPAT) factor 3, bandwidth = 1698 Hz/pixel, TE = 12, 28, 44 and 60 ms].

fMRI data analysis

fMRI data were analysed using SPM8 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm8/; Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, UK). After slice-timing correction, a mean image for all functional scans was generated for each subject, to which individual volumes were spatially realigned by rigid body transformation. Unwarping was performed during realignment to correct for dynamic motion–distortion interaction artefacts. Structural images were co-registered with the mean EPI image, segmented into grey and white matter, and normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template. Normalization parameters were then applied to the EPI images for an anatomically informed normalization. Images were then subsampled to 2 × 2 × 2 mm3 and spatially smoothed with a 10 mm full width at half maximum (FWHM) Gaussian filter. Scan-by-scan head motions exceeding 0.5 mm in the functional volumes were repaired using Art Repair (Mazaika et al. Reference Mazaika, Whitfield-Gabrieli, Reiss and Glover2007).

The subject-level statistical analyses were performed using the general linear model (GLM). The main events of interest were the 1 s periods prior to the recorded button press in intention and movement trials. Missed trials and the 7 s period for responding were modelled as events of no interest. Vectors containing the event onsets and durations were convolved with the canonical haemodynamic response function, and its temporal and dispersion derivatives. The statistical parameter estimates were computed separately for each voxel for all 12 columns in the design matrix. Movement parameters were also included as regressors of no interest in the GLM in addition to deweighting on the repaired volumes.

Intention v. movement trials were contrasted at the individual level and subsequently inputted into an independent-samples t test to compare FND patients and HV. Whole-brain voxel-wise group comparisons were performed with a cluster extent threshold of 15 voxels at p < 0.001 (uncorrected), correcting for multiple comparisons at p < 0.05 assuming an individual-voxel type I error of p = 0.01 (Slotnick et al. Reference Slotnick, Moo, Segal and Hart2003). We also confirmed fMRI activations in a priori regions of interest (ROIs) implicated in Lau et al. (Reference Lau, Rogers, Haggard and Passingham2004), the pre-SMA, the inferior parietal lobule (IPL) and dlPFC, using small-volume correction as well.

The intention v. movement blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) contrast was also correlated with the behavioural parameter W–M across all individuals.

rsfMRI data analysis

rsfMRI data were analysed with multi-echo independent component analysis (ME-ICAv2.5 beta6; http://afni.nimh.nih.gov). First, FastICA was used to decompose multi-echo fMRI data. The BOLD signal is characterized by TE-dependence, which was measured using the pseudo-F statistic κ; whereas TE-independence was measured by the pseudo-F statistic ρ. Components were then categorized as BOLD or non-BOLD based on their κ- and ρ-value weightings, respectively (Kundu et al. Reference Kundu, Inati, Evans, Luh and Bandettini2012), and non-BOLD components were removed by projection. Denoised EPI images were co-registered to MPRAGE, normalized to the MNI template, spatially smoothed with a 6 mm FWHM Gaussian filter, and temporally band-pass filtered between 0.008 and 0.09 Hz. Anatomical scans were segmented into grey matter, white matter and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF), and significant principal components in white matter and CSF regions were identified and removed following the CompCor strategy (Behzadi et al. Reference Behzadi, Restom, Liau and Liu2007).

ROI-driven functional connectivity was computed with the CONN-fMRI Functional Connectivity toolbox (Whitfield-Gabrieli & Nieto-Castañón, Reference Whitfield-Gabrieli and Nieto-Castañón2012) for SPM8. Based on the task-based results, the rsfMRI analysis was seeded within the right IPC, as defined in the Automated Anatomical Labelling atlas.

ROI-to-voxel whole-brain connectivity maps were computed and entered into full factorial GLMs to compare whole-brain connectivity patterns between FND patients and HV. Whole-brain voxel-wise group comparisons were performed with a cluster extent threshold of 15 voxels at p < 0.001 (uncorrected), correcting for multiple comparisons at p < 0.05 assuming an individual-voxel type I error of p = 0.01 (Slotnick et al. Reference Slotnick, Moo, Segal and Hart2003).

Ethical standards

All procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Results

Participants

In all, 25 FND patients and 24 age- and gender-matched HV completed the study. Participant characteristics are described in Table 1, with FND subjects displaying significantly higher depression and anxiety scores than controls.

Table 1. Patient characteristics

FND, Functional neurological disorder; HV, healthy volunteers; s.d., standard deviation; df, degrees of freedom; BDI-II, Beck Depression Inventory-II; STAI, Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory.

a FND symptom severity scores were available in 24 patients and were rated from 1 (mild) to 4 (very severe).

Symptom patterns and severity scores were available for 24 out of 25 FND patients (Table 1). Of the patients, 17 reported pain symptoms (headache n = 13, legs n = 3, body n = 5) with a mean duration of 54.5 (s.d. = 64.2) months. Of the patients, 13 had positive motor symptoms (myoclonic jerks n = 2, tremor n = 7, dystonia n = 3, gait abnormality n = 3), and 20 patients reported negative motor symptoms of weakness (lower extremities n = 7 or upper and lower extremities, n = 13). Average duration of motor symptoms was 53.5 (s.d. = 44.2) months (omission in one subject). Of the patients, 19 stated sensory symptoms (loss of sensation, numbness, pins and needles n = 14, tinnitus n = 3, hearing loss n = 1, double vision n = 3, blurred vision n = 8, loss of vision n = 2) with a mean duration of 46.8 (s.d. = 42.6) months (omission in five subjects). Other symptoms such as stutter (n = 4), dysarthria (n = 8), dysphonia (n = 4), swallowing (n = 8), memory (n = 10), gastrointestinal (n = 6), genitourinary (n = 10) and cardiovascular symptoms (n = 10) were also reported. Most participants had overlapping symptom profiles. For instance, when considering only positive motor, non-epileptic or weakness symptoms, 16 had mixed symptoms, four had only weakness, two had only non-epileptic seizures and two had only positive symptoms. Of the patients, 12 had a symptom affecting the left upper limb they were using to button press. Eight patients had a symptom affecting the right upper limb that they were using to indicate W or M with a mouse. All subjects were watched carefully to ensure that the symptoms did not interfere with their capacity to perform the experiment. One might also anticipate that the symptom might similarly affect both W and M which were contrasted.

Two participants had current depression of mild severity, 10 additional subjects had a history of depression, two had panic disorder and two had a history of obsessive–compulsive disorder. Medication use included antidepressants (n = 17), pregabalin (n = 5), gabapentin (n = 1), lamotrigine (n = 2), topiramate (n = 1) and a synthetic opioid (n = 1).

Behavioural results

Average latencies of the W and M judgements relative to the recorded button press were calculated for each individual after excluding outlier trials (> 2 s.d. from the individual mean). There was no significant difference in the number of excluded trials in HV and FND patients (mean 3.7, s.d. = 1.2 v. 4.1, s.d. = 1.1, p = 0.21). Two outlier FND patients (>3 s.d. from mean across both groups) were excluded from the behavioural data analysis, thus 22 FND patients and 24 HVs were included in behavioural data analysis.

A mixed-design analysis of variance (ANOVA) with the factors judgement (W, M) and group (FND, HV) showed significant main effects of group (F 1,45 = 16.54, p < 0.001) and judgement (F 1,45 = 33.79, p < 0.001), and a significant interaction (F 1,45 = 4.38, p = 0.042). FND patients showed delayed W [FND −99 (s.d. = 125) ms, 95% confidence interval (CI) −163 to −35 ms; HV −252 (s.d. = 175) ms, 95% CI −315 to −190 ms] and M [FND −15 (s.d. = 79) ms, 95% CI −43 to +14 ms; HV −72 (s.d. = 56) ms, 95% CI −100 to −44 ms] judgements compared with HV. The interval between W and M judgements, W–M, was used as an implicit measure of conscious awareness of volitional intention. An independent-samples t test demonstrated that FND patients exhibited a significantly reduced W–M interval compared with HV [FND −76 (s.d. = 80) ms; HV −180 (s.d. = 180) ms, t 32.3 = 2.57, p = 0.015].

Individual standard deviations across trials were compared which assess the reporting precision of the W judgement. FND subjects reported the W judgement (average s.d. = 156 ms) more precisely than HV (average s.d. = 305 ms, t 42.3 = −2.61, p = 0.013), but not the M judgement (FND average s.d. = 135 ms; HV average s.d. = 80 ms, t 27.9 = 1.95, p = 0.06).

Exploratory one-way ANOVAs were used to compare the W–M measure of FND patients divided as a function of neurological symptom presentation with HV, showing significant differences for all symptom categories (Table 2). Post-hoc analyses showed that FND patients with any kind of motor symptoms had more reduced W–M intervals than HV. To assess the specificity of these findings we compared those with only negative motor symptoms, positive symptoms (with or without negative) and HV. The one-way ANOVA showed a trend (p = 0.052); the post-hoc analysis showed that patients with positive motor symptoms differed from HV (p = 0.019), but not those with only negative symptoms (p > 0.2) (Bonferroni correction p < 0.025). Note that all subjects with positive symptoms also had negative symptoms. We attempted to examine laterality for positive motor symptoms but only three subjects had positive symptoms that did not affect the hand used in the task. We compared subjects with bilateral/left-sided weakness [n = 8, −49 (s.d. = 52) ms] and right-sided weakness [n = 8, −82 (s.d. = 75) ms] with HV [n = 20, −178 (s.d. = 174) ms], which showed a trend using a one-way ANOVA (p=0.054).

Table 2. W and M judgements as a function of functional symptom typea

Data are given as mean difference between W and M judgements (in ms) (s.d.).

HV, Healthy volunteers; s.d., standard deviation; ANOVA, analysis of variance; df, degrees of freedom.

a F values of the ANOVAs are given, as well as p values of the ANOVAs and Fisher's least significant difference post-hoc tests. All ANOVAs had dffactor = 2 and dferror = 43, except the ANOVA comparing functional neurological disorder patients with positive motor symptoms, patients with only negative symptoms and HV, which had dffactor = 2 and dferror = 41.

fMRI results

We excluded two FND patients and two HV from the analysis due to excessive head movement (>0.5 mm), where over one-third of volumes needed repairing with ArtRepair (Mazaika et al. Reference Mazaika, Whitfield-Gabrieli, Reiss and Glover2007). Data from 23 FND patients and 22 HV were included in the fMRI analysis; in the comparison between groups, data were also analysed after excluding the two outlier patients identified in the behavioural analysis, and all fMRI findings were consistently observed.

Across all participants, attention to intention relative to attention to movement was associated with increased activity in the bilateral IPL, dlPFC/inferior frontal gyrus and pre-SMA/dlPFC, as well as some minor clusters within temporal and occipital lobes (Fig. 2 a, online Supplementary Table S1).

Fig. 2. Attention to intention v. movement. (a) Significant activations associated with attention to intention compared with attention to movement (intention v. movement contrast) across all participants (n = 45). (b) Regions of significantly decreased activity for functional neurological disorder patients (n = 23) compared with healthy volunteers (n = 22) when attending to intention compared with attending to movement. Image displayed at p < 0.005 (uncorrected) for illustration. (c) Results of the correlation between the intention v. movement contrast and the behavioural measure W-M across all participants (n = 45). Image displayed at p < 0.001 (uncorrected) for illustration. pre-SMA, Pre-supplementary motor area; dmPFC, dorsomedial prefrontal cortex; IPL, inferior parietal lobule; dlPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; IFG, inferior frontal gyrus; M1, primary motor cortex.

When comparing intention v. movement trials between groups, FND patients exhibited significantly reduced activity within the right IPL [supramarginal gyrus, Brodmann area (BA) 40; peak voxel reported in MNI coordinates x y z in mm: 54, −32, 42; cluster size = 36; Z = 3.58] compared with HV (Fig. 2 b). The above-mentioned results were replicated after excluding the two outlier patients: FND patients showed significantly reduced IPL activity with the same peak coordinates (cluster size = 43; Z = 3.70) compared with HV. An additional activation in the left premotor cortex was also revealed (superior frontal gyrus; MNI coordinates: −18, 6, 50; cluster size = 49; Z = 3.67).

The W–M interval was positively correlated with BOLD activity in intention v. movement trials in the bilateral IPL and primary motor/premotor areas, and left dlPFC and precuneus (Fig. 2 c, online Supplementary Table S2).

rsfMRI results

Data from 25 FND patients and 70 HV [40 women, 40.19 (s.d. = 12.70) years old], including those HV that took part in the fMRI study, were included in the rsfMRI analysis. Compared with HV, FND patients demonstrated reduced connectivity between the right IPC and frontal control regions [dlPFC, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), BA 10; Fig. 3 b], but increased functional connectivity with the premotor cortex and SMA (Fig. 3 a, online Supplementary Table S3).

Fig. 3. Resting-state functional connectivity from right inferior parietal cortex (IPC) seed. (a) Increased (functional neurological disorder patients > healthy volunteers; FND>HV) and (b) decreased (HV > FND) functional connectivity from IPC to whole brain for FND patients (n = 25) compared with HV (n = 70) during rest. Image displayed at p < 0.005 (uncorrected) for illustration. SMA, Supplementary motor area; IPL, inferior parietal lobule; dlPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; MTG, middle temporal gyrus; ITG, inferior temporal gyrus; BA, Brodmann area; PCC, posterior cingulate cortex.

Discussion

We show using Libet's clock task (Libet et al. Reference Libet, Gleason, Wright and Pearl1983) that FND is characterized by impaired awareness of voluntary motor intention with delayed W relative to M judgements. The judgement of W was both abnormally delayed and more precise relative to HV. On an exploratory basis, this delay in W was particularly evident in patients with involuntary functional positive motor symptoms and less so in those with only negative motor symptoms. Our results concur with a previous report of abnormalities in the conscious experience of action in a small sample of 11 patients with psychogenic tremor (Edwards et al. Reference Edwards, Moretto, Schwingenschuh, Katschnig, Bhatia and Haggard2011), replicating these findings in a larger sample. These findings might also explain observations of impaired effortful voluntary movement in FND (Criswell et al. Reference Criswell, Sterling, Swisher, Evanoff and Racette2010) and common observations of enhanced attention towards both voluntary and involuntary movement. As expected, across all subjects, the attention to intention v. movement was associated with enhanced activity in the bilateral IPL, dlPFC and pre-SMA. Crucially, FND subjects had lower activity in the right IPL (BA 40, supramarginal gyrus) compared with HV in the contrast of W–M. This region was also positively correlated with the behavioural measure W–M across all subjects, thus emphasizing its role in the conscious awareness of volitional intention. The network implicated in voluntary motor intention also showed aberrant functional connectivity at rest in FND subjects. FND showed reduced resting-state functional connectivity between the right IPC and prefrontal structures (dlPFC, ACC, BA 10), regions upstream in the processing of intention and action selection, but increased connectivity with the premotor cortex and SMA, regions downstream in the processing of intention and motor preparation. Previous studies in FND have implicated lower TPJ/IPC activity to involuntary functional symptoms (Voon et al. Reference Voon, Gallea, Hattori, Bruno, Ekanayake and Hallett2010) and lower SMA activity during voluntary motor preparation (Voon et al. Reference Voon, Brezing, Gallea and Hallett2011). Here we specifically isolate the awareness of voluntary intention and build on data implicating an abnormal fronto-parietal network and suggest that these findings might play a role in the subjective sense of involuntariness.

Our findings may implicate a general impairment in inferior parietal function. Previous studies have shown hypoactivity of the right TPJ in the comparison of involuntary and mimicked tremor (Voon et al. Reference Voon, Gallea, Hattori, Bruno, Ekanayake and Hallett2010). As neural regions associated with the sensory outcome appeared to be intact, we had speculated that the primary deficit might be related to an impairment at the level of the sensory prediction (Voon et al. Reference Voon, Gallea, Hattori, Bruno, Ekanayake and Hallett2010). A follow-up study further showed decreased functional connectivity of the right TPJ seed using average coordinates from a meta-analysis of self-agency studies (peak x y z = 51, −46, 31 mm) within BA 39 (Maurer et al. Reference Maurer, LaFaver, Ameli, Epstein, Hallett and Horovitz2016). The TPJ is a large region encompassing the IPL. The right IPL activation in the attention to intention v. movement condition across all subjects was very large (cluster size k = 10 439) and included the right TPJ. However, the right IPL activation (peak x y z = 54, –32, 42 mm) in the group comparison (FND < HV; Figs 2 and 3) is localized in the angular/supramarginal gyrus in BA 40 and distant from the superior temporal lobule. Both BA 39 and BA 40 are implicated in impairments in intention using Libet's clock (Desmurget & Sirigu, Reference Desmurget and Sirigu2012). A generalized impairment in inferior parietal cortical function may be an issue which has been observed in some but not all FND studies (for a review, see Voon et al. Reference Voon, Cavanna, Coburn, Sampson, Reeve and LaFrance2016). Differences in imaging task design and hence function might thus highlight different parietal regions. Thus, although both studies implicate impairments in the IPC, the focus on intention in voluntary movements implicates BA 40 whereas differences between the comparison of involuntary and voluntary movement might highlight more comparator systems of sensory prediction and outcome implicating BA 39 or the TPJ.

Mechanistic considerations

There are several possible explanations for our findings. Here we discuss plausible mechanisms including the role of attention, impairments in explicit intentional processes, decision thresholds and movement veto.

Attending to and reporting the intention to move might be more difficult and demanding more attention than reporting the movement itself, so that subjects with attentional impairments may have more difficulty in making the W judgement. Non-specific impairments in attention have been documented across some but not all studies in FND (for a review, see Voon et al. Reference Voon, Cavanna, Coburn, Sampson, Reeve and LaFrance2016). However, in our study, FND patients reported the W judgement more precisely than HV, thus suggesting that the reduced latency is unlikely to be due to a non-specific attentional impairment. We also controlled for non-specific attention effects by comparing two conditions that require the allocation of attention to either intention or movement. An intriguing follow-up of this study might be to consider how W–M might present in subjects with malingering in which the symptoms are deliberately produced. One might speculate that the variability associated with reporting of W might be much greater than observed in FND.

Theoretical models and empirical evidence suggest enhanced directed attention towards self-related representations and pathological symptoms (Brown, Reference Brown2004; Cojan et al. Reference Cojan, Waber, Carruzzo and Vuilleumier2009; Edwards et al. Reference Edwards, Adams, Brown, Pareés and Friston2012). These findings of an enhanced precision (inverse variance) of abnormally delayed voluntary intention converge with theories of enhanced attention towards and thus precision of abnormal pathological priors or expectations in FND (for a review, see Edwards et al. Reference Edwards, Adams, Brown, Pareés and Friston2012). Although enhanced attention to the movement symptom is observed in FND, what is not clear is whether this is a primary pathology or possibly a compensatory mechanism underlying primary impairments in volitional processes. Enhanced attention to both voluntary and functional movements is observed clinically in FND (van Poppelen et al. Reference van Poppelen, Saifee, Schwingenschuh, Katschnig, Bhatia, Tijssen and Edwards2011) and attention appears to play a role in the expression of the symptom as demonstrated by clinical signs of improvement or cessation of functional symptoms with distractability or shifts of attention away from the functional movement (Schwingenschuh & Deuschl, Reference Schwingenschuh, Deuschl, Hallett, Stone and Carson2016). Attention focused on a novel voluntary movement is required for optimal motor function but can impair automatic overlearned habitual movements. That attention is enhanced to self in FND might be compensatory and secondarily related to primary impairments in voluntary motor intention. The enhanced attention might then also interfere with skilled automatic movements. That focused attention increases the functional movement suggests that the aberrant movement is novel and goal-oriented in nature rather than automatic and habitual as otherwise, attention should impair habits and decrease the functional movement. Our findings would be consistent with clinical observations of increased attention towards both the involuntary and voluntary movements in FND.

Several lines of evidence suggest impairments in explicit but not implicit motor processes in FND, suggesting possible impairments in motor conceptualization or volition, first theorized by Spence et al. (Reference Spence, Crimlisk, Cope, Ron and Grasby2000) (for a review, see Voon et al. Reference Voon, Cavanna, Coburn, Sampson, Reeve and LaFrance2016). FND subjects are impaired at explicit instructed v. implicit automatic mental rotation of hands or feet (Roelofs et al. Reference Roelofs, Näring, Keijsers, Hoogduin, Van Galen and Maris2001). In this study, functional paralysis subjects subjectively reported that they could not explicitly mentally rotate their hands or feet to match the image in 51% of foot trials, compared with 0% in controls. Similarly, FND subjects were impaired in tasks dissociating explicit pre-planned movements under full control or under greater certainty but intact in implicit movements occurring more automatically under contexts in which subjects may be less aware (e.g. one-back reaching, visuomotor transformation or variable predictive value of precued reaction time) (Parees et al. Reference Parees, Kassavetis, Saifee, Sadnicka, Davare, Bhatia, Rothwell, Bestmann and Edwards2013). Focusing on the motor intention phase of movement, FND subjects also show decreased activity in the SMA and increased activity in limbic regions (amygdala, anterior insula) relative to healthy controls (Voon et al. Reference Voon, Brezing, Gallea and Hallett2011). FND was also associated with decreased functional connectivity between the dlPFC and SMA in freely chosen v. directed cued movement, suggesting a possible impairment in higher-order action selection during freely chosen actions. Together these findings suggest primary impairments in explicit motor intentional processes with intact implicit automatic processes. These findings help explain observations of impaired voluntary movement in FND as tested on finger tapping (Criswell et al. Reference Criswell, Sterling, Swisher, Evanoff and Racette2010) and common clinical observations of enhanced attention and effort directed towards voluntary movement which may be secondary to the primary impairment. Indeed, a supportive clinical criterion of functional movement is slowed effortful voluntary movement (Schwingenschuh & Deuschl, Reference Schwingenschuh, Deuschl, Hallett, Stone and Carson2016). The mechanisms underlying functional movement may also represent an extension of this impairment in explicit volitional motor process engaging enhanced attention. The enhanced attention may also then secondarily interfere with skilled automatic movements.

These abnormalities in intention awareness appear to be greater in patients with positive motor symptoms, although we note that the relationship with specific neurological symptoms should be interpreted cautiously given the limited sample size. Nevertheless, several intriguing mechanisms may be specifically relevant to positive motor symptoms. It has been suggested that movements are produced when a certain decision threshold is crossed and that the precise moment at which this threshold is crossed is largely determined by spontaneous subthreshold fluctuations of neuronal activity (Schurger et al. Reference Schurger, Sitt and Dehaene2012). In the context of FND, it could be speculated that patients might have a lower decision threshold and/or increased neuronal subthreshold fluctuations, resulting in an increased likelihood of abnormally crossing said threshold and thus producing aberrant movements. Indeed, the Bereitschaftspotential, which has been posited to reflect the increase in spontaneous neuronal subthreshold fluctuations preceding voluntary movements (Schurger et al. Reference Schurger, Sitt and Dehaene2012), has been found to precede psychogenic myoclonus (Terada et al. Reference Terada, Ikeda, Van Ness, Nagamine, Kaji, Kimura and Shibasaki1995).

Another possible underlying mechanism could be related to the interval between awareness of intention and movement, which has been hypothesized to temporally allow for a conscious inhibition or veto of the movement (Libet, Reference Libet1999; Haggard & Libet, Reference Haggard and Libet2001), as well as for an evaluation of whether the selected action might be optimal to obtain the desired effect (Haggard & Libet, Reference Haggard and Libet2001). A recent study (Schultze-Kraft et al. Reference Schultze-Kraft, Birman, Rusconi, Allefeld, Görgen, Dähne, Blankertz and Haynes2016) reported that initiated movements could be inhibited if a stop signal occurred up to 200 ms prior to movement onset, but not if it occurred under that time. Since the temporal shift of intention awareness towards the movement in FND patients is well under 200 ms, it could suggest that this results in a decrease in the time available and likelihood of action inhibition or movement veto. Still, this remains speculative based solely on the data at hand. However, as in the current study, shortened W–M intervals have been mainly observed in neurological and neuropsychiatric disorders characterized by positive symptomatology (Edwards et al. Reference Edwards, Moretto, Schwingenschuh, Katschnig, Bhatia and Haggard2011; Moretto et al. Reference Moretto, Schwingenschuh, Katschnig, Bhatia and Haggard2011; Tabu et al. Reference Tabu, Aso, Matsuhashi, Ueki, Takahashi, Fukuyama, Shibasaki and Mima2015). These findings converge with a study of FND focusing on positive motor symptoms in which the only cognitive deficit observed across a range of tasks was impaired response inhibition on a Go/NoGo task (Voon et al. Reference Voon, Ekanayake, Wiggs, Kranick, Ameli, Harrison and Hallett2013). Alternatively, the findings may also be related to prior experience with functional movements, which may secondarily result in impaired reliance on the subjective awareness of the intention to move.

IPC

Our findings implicate the IPC, but, contrary to our hypothesis, not the SMC. Cortical stimulation of the IPC (BA 39/40) has been associated with subjective feelings of ‘wanting to move’ a body part, whereas stimulation of the SMC results in an uncontrollable ‘urge’ to produce a specific movement (Desmurget et al. Reference Desmurget, Reilly, Richard, Szathmari, Mottolese and Sirigu2009; Desmurget & Sirigu, Reference Desmurget and Sirigu2012). This phenomenological difference suggests the intentional feelings evoked in the IPC may lie upstream of the SMC, and has been hypothesized to reflect the role of the IPC in pure intention (Desmurget et al. Reference Desmurget, Reilly, Richard, Szathmari, Mottolese and Sirigu2009). However, which of these structures is key to voluntary action remains controversial. Some authors attribute this role to the pre-SMA, highlighting its involvement in transforming thoughts into actions (Haggard, Reference Haggard2008), whereas others emphasize evidence suggesting that the locus of consciousness might be within posterior brain regions (Koch et al. Reference Koch, Massimini, Boly and Tononi2016). Damage to both fronto-mesial and IPC regions has been associated with the alien hand syndrome, in which movements are expressed outside of volitional control (Scepkowski & Cronin-Golomb, Reference Scepkowski and Cronin-Golomb2003). In an fMRI study of a patient with a posterior parietal lesion, alien hand movement was associated with circumscribed activity of the contralateral primary motor cortex, which was suggested to arise in the absence of volitional awareness from the IPC (Assal et al. Reference Assal, Schwartz and Vuilleumier2007). Indeed, in patients with IPC stroke lesions, the latency of the W judgement is shifted towards movement onset as compared with HV and cerebellar patients (Sirigu et al. Reference Sirigu, Daprati, Ciancia, Giraux, Nighoghossian, Posada and Haggard2004). Together these findings suggest a role for the IPC at an early stage of motor intention.

Another possible parallel lies in studies of motor imagery or internal rehearsal of motor representations. Recently, a tetraplegic patient was shown to be able to control posterior parietal activity though motor imagery (Aflalo et al. Reference Aflalo, Kellis, Klaes, Lee, Shi, Pejsa, Shanfield, Hayes-Jackson, Aisen, Heck, Liu and Andersen2015). Recordings showed that neurons in this area code both goal and imagined trajectory of the movements, indicating that this region encodes motor intention and that its signals could be used for neuroprosthetic applications (Aflalo et al. Reference Aflalo, Kellis, Klaes, Lee, Shi, Pejsa, Shanfield, Hayes-Jackson, Aisen, Heck, Liu and Andersen2015). Motor imagery implicates similar neural regions as action execution (Decety et al. Reference Decety, Perani, Jeannerod, Bettinardi, Tadary, Woods, Mazziotta and Fazio1994; Sirigu et al. Reference Sirigu, Duhamel, Cohen, Pillon, Dubois and Agid1996) and has been studied in FND as discussed above. In a limb rotation task (Roelofs et al. Reference Roelofs, Näring, Keijsers, Hoogduin, Van Galen and Maris2001), participants with functional paralysis showed no difference to HV when judging whether images of rotated hands and feet corresponded to the right or left limb. However, they were slower than HV when explicitly asked to mentally rotate their own limbs to match the position of the image, with patients subjectively reporting impaired motor imagery in that they had difficulty mentally rotating their limbs to match the image whereas no difficulties were reported in controls. These impairments in explicit motor imagery prior to and without actual movement led the authors to emphasize an impairment in intentional processes in FND (Roelofs et al. Reference Roelofs, Näring, Keijsers, Hoogduin, Van Galen and Maris2001).

Limitations and conclusion

The study is not without limitations. Although we specifically recruited subjects that were able to remain still in the MRI environment, it cannot be completely ruled out that the subjects’ neurological symptoms interfered with testing and that weakness or functional movements occurred during scanning. However, as these symptoms may have occurred randomly across the blocks, the contrast used should effectively cancel any neural activity from adventitious symptoms. Larger sample sizes with greater symptom specificity would also be helpful in understanding the relationship of this measure with specific symptoms. Furthermore, how these findings might be influenced by other symptoms commonly observed in FND including panic symptoms or other somatoform disorders remains to be established. The majority of our sample was also on medications, which is commonly observed in FND cohorts. The use of medications might interfere with imaging findings; further studies in other samples on such chronic medications might be indicated. Co-morbid symptoms of depression or anxiety are also very common in FND, and the effect of depression or anxiety symptoms cannot be ruled out. When BDI and STAI are added as covariates in ANOVA in behaviour analysis, significant interactions between conditions (intention or movement) v. BDI or STAI were observed and the main effect of groups or conditions was no longer observed. FND is also commonly associated with generalized anxiety which was not assessed in this study.

Despite the main findings of Libet et al. (Reference Libet, Gleason, Wright and Pearl1983) having been widely replicated (Lau et al. Reference Lau, Rogers, Haggard and Passingham2004; Edwards et al. Reference Edwards, Moretto, Schwingenschuh, Katschnig, Bhatia and Haggard2011; Fried et al. Reference Fried, Mukamel and Kreiman2011), including in the current study, Libet's clock task has not been exempt of criticism. The precision and nature of the W judgement has been called into question, with authors suggesting that the high attentive demands of the task might result in an imprecise measurement of the internal process (Stamm, Reference Stamm1985), or that the W judgement might represent a peak of intention (Ringo, Reference Ringo1985), meaning that the subject's intention would in reality precede the reported urge, in contrast to the simultaneity posited by Libet et al. (Reference Libet, Gleason, Wright and Pearl1983). Recent studies suggest that the decision for a movement does not abruptly appear in a binary manner but is gradually constructed; thus Libet's task may involve a cut-off for binary translation of the continuous preparation of action decision (Guggisberg & Mottaz, Reference Guggisberg and Mottaz2013). In addition, that subjective time of intention onset is influenced by perceptual information such as sensory feedback (Guggisberg & Mottaz, Reference Guggisberg and Mottaz2013; Wolpe & Rowe, Reference Wolpe and Rowe2014). Introspection to one's own intention might be also intermittent or rather retrospectively inferred. Critics thus implied that the unconscious nature of the movement intention, as reflected by the Bereitschaftspotential preceding intention awareness, would not be such (Stamm, Reference Stamm1985). Haggard & Libet (Reference Haggard and Libet2001), however, countered that even the largest estimates of the prior entry effect, by which attention affects the judgement of synchrony of events occurring in different streams, are an order of magnitude smaller than the 200 ms interval between Bereitschaftspotential and W judgement posited in the original work (Libet et al. Reference Libet, Gleason, Wright and Pearl1983). Moreover, even those who questioned the precision or validity of the absolute W and/or M judgements conceded that their criticism might not affect the relative distance between them (Rollman, Reference Rollman1985). The latter is of particular importance to the current work, as it is this interval that is postulated to be used by the subject to monitor the desirability and effect of the action (Libet's veto) and whether it is an optimal action plan (Haggard's specificity) (Haggard & Libet, Reference Haggard and Libet2001). Therefore, irrespective of the conscious nature of the movement, a reduced veto period would have a higher likelihood of resulting in maladaptive actions, such as the aberrant behaviours observed in our FND patients and reported by previous researchers (Edwards et al. Reference Edwards, Moretto, Schwingenschuh, Katschnig, Bhatia and Haggard2011; Moretto et al. Reference Moretto, Schwingenschuh, Katschnig, Bhatia and Haggard2011; Tabu et al. Reference Tabu, Aso, Matsuhashi, Ueki, Takahashi, Fukuyama, Shibasaki and Mima2015).

In conclusion, our findings build on reports suggesting impairments in motor intention awareness in FND and specifically highlight abnormalities of the early motor processing network focusing on the right IPL. These findings might explain common clinical observations of enhanced attention towards movements in FND and impaired effortful voluntary movements and provide possible novel targets for therapeutic intervention that might include psychological or physiological interventions or neuromodulation.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717000071

Acknowledgements

N.D. is a research fellow of the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DO1915/1-1). V.V. is a Wellcome Trust Intermediate Fellow (983705/Z/10/Z). The study was funded by the Biomedical Research Council. Funders had no role in the study design, in the collection, analysis and interpretation of the data, in the writing of the report, or in the decision to submit the paper for publication.

Declaration of Interest

None.

Footnotes

These authors contributed equally to the paper.

References

Aflalo, T, Kellis, S, Klaes, C, Lee, B, Shi, Y, Pejsa, K, Shanfield, K, Hayes-Jackson, S, Aisen, M, Heck, C, Liu, C, Andersen, RA (2015). Decoding motor imagery from the posterior parietal cortex of a tetraplegic human. Science 348, 906910.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Anderson, KE, Gruber-Baldini, AL, Vaughan, CG, Reich, SG, Fishman, PS, Weiner, WJ, Shulman, LM (2007). Impact of psychogenic movement disorders versus Parkinson's on disability, quality of life, and psychopathology. Movement Disorders 22, 22042209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Assal, F, Schwartz, S, Vuilleumier, P (2007). Moving with or without will: functional neural correlates of alien hand syndrome. Annals of Neurology 62, 301306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Behzadi, Y, Restom, K, Liau, J, Liu, TT (2007). A component based noise correction method (CompCor) for BOLD and perfusion based fMRI. NeuroImage 37, 90101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, AT, Ward, CH, Mendelson, MM, Mock, JJ, Erbaugh, JJ (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry 4, 561571.Google Scholar
Brass, M, Haggard, P (2007). To do or not to do: the neural signature of self-control. Journal of Neuroscience 27, 91419145.Google Scholar
Brown, RJ (2004). Psychological mechanisms of medically unexplained symptoms: an integrative conceptual model. Psychological Bulletin 130, 793812.Google Scholar
Carson, AJ, Ringbauer, B, Stone, J, McKenzie, L, Warlow, C, Sharpe, M (2000). Do medically unexplained symptoms matter? A prospective cohort study of 300 new referrals to neurology outpatient clinics. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 68, 207210.Google Scholar
Cojan, Y, Waber, L, Carruzzo, A, Vuilleumier, P (2009). Motor inhibition in hysterical conversion paralysis. NeuroImage 47, 10261037.Google Scholar
Criswell, S, Sterling, C, Swisher, L, Evanoff, B, Racette, BA (2010). Sensitivity and specificity of the finger tapping task for the detection of psychogenic movement disorders. Parkinsonism Related Disorders 16, 197201.Google Scholar
Decety, J, Perani, D, Jeannerod, M, Bettinardi, V, Tadary, B, Woods, R, Mazziotta, JC, Fazio, F (1994). Mapping motor representations with positron emission tomography. Nature 371, 600602.Google Scholar
Desmurget, M, Reilly, KT, Richard, N, Szathmari, A, Mottolese, C, Sirigu, A (2009). Movement intention after parietal cortex stimulation in humans. Science 324, 811813.Google Scholar
Desmurget, M, Sirigu, A (2012). Conscious motor intention emerges in the inferior parietal lobule. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 22, 10041011.Google Scholar
Edwards, MJ, Adams, RA, Brown, H, Pareés, I, Friston, KJ (2012). A Bayesian account of ‘hysteria’. Brain 135, 34953512.Google Scholar
Edwards, MJ, Moretto, G, Schwingenschuh, P, Katschnig, P, Bhatia, KP, Haggard, P (2011). Abnormal sense of intention preceding voluntary movement in patients with psychogenic tremor. Neuropsychologia 49, 27912793.Google Scholar
Fried, I, Katz, A, McCarthy, G, Sass, KJ, Williamson, P, Spencer, SS, Spencer, DD (1991). Functional organization of human supplementary motor cortex studied by electrical stimulation. Journal of Neuroscience 11, 36563666.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fried, I, Mukamel, R, Kreiman, G (2011). Internally generated preactivation of single neurons in human medial frontal cortex predicts volition. Neuron 69, 548562.Google Scholar
Guggisberg, AG, Mottaz, A (2013). Timing and awareness of movement decisions: does consciousness really come too late? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7, 385.Google Scholar
Haggard, P (2008). Human volition: towards a neuroscience of will. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience 9, 934946.Google Scholar
Haggard, P, Libet, B (2001). Conscious intention and brain activity. Journal of Consciousness Studies 8, 4764.Google Scholar
Hallett, M (2010). Physiology of psychogenic movement disorders. Journal of Clinical Neurosci ence 17, 959965.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koch, C, Massimini, M, Boly, M, Tononi, G (2016). Neural correlates of consciousness: progress and problems. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience 17, 307321.Google Scholar
Kundu, P, Inati, SJ, Evans, JW, Luh, W-M, Bandettini, PA (2012). Differentiating BOLD and non-BOLD signals in fMRI time series using multi-echo EPI. NeuroImage 60, 17591770.Google Scholar
Lau, HC, Rogers, RD, Haggard, P, Passingham, RE (2004). Attention to intention. Science 303, 12081210.Google Scholar
Libet, B (1999). Do we have free will? Journal of Consciousness Studies 6, 4757.Google Scholar
Libet, B, Gleason, CA, Wright, EW, Pearl, DK (1983). Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential). Brain 106, 623642.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maurer, CW, LaFaver, K, Ameli, R, Epstein, SA, Hallett, M, Horovitz, SG (2016). Impaired self-agency in functional movement disorders: a resting-state fMRI study. Neurology 87, 564570.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mazaika, PK, Whitfield-Gabrieli, S, Reiss, AL, Glover, GH (2007). Artifact repair for fMRI data from high motion clinical subjects. In Annual Meeting of the Organization of Human Brain Mapping: Chicago, USA.Google Scholar
Moretto, G, Schwingenschuh, P, Katschnig, P, Bhatia, KP, Haggard, P (2011). Delayed experience of volition in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 82, 13241327.Google Scholar
Parees, I, Kassavetis, P, Saifee, TA, Sadnicka, A, Davare, M, Bhatia, KP, Rothwell, JC, Bestmann, S, Edwards, MJ (2013). Failure of explicit movement control in patients with functional motor symptoms. Movement Disorders 28, 517523.Google Scholar
Ringo, JL (1985). Timing volition: questions of what and when about W. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8, 550551.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roelofs, K, Näring, GWB, Keijsers, GPJ, Hoogduin, CAL, Van Galen, GP, Maris, E (2001). Motor imagery in conversion paralysis. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 6, 2140.Google Scholar
Rollman, GB (1985). Sensory events with variable central latencies provide inaccurate clocks. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8, 551552.Google Scholar
Scepkowski, LA, Cronin-Golomb, A (2003). The alien hand: cases, categorizations, and anatomical correlates. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews 2, 261277.Google Scholar
Schultze-Kraft, M, Birman, D, Rusconi, M, Allefeld, C, Görgen, K, Dähne, S, Blankertz, B, Haynes, J-D (2016). The point of no return in vetoing self-initiated movements. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 113, 10801085.Google Scholar
Schurger, A, Sitt, JD, Dehaene, S (2012). An accumulator model for spontaneous neural activity prior to self-initiated movement. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 109, E2904E2913.Google Scholar
Schwingenschuh, P, Deuschl, G (2016). Functional tremor. In Functional Neurological Disorders (ed. Hallett, M, Stone, J and Carson, A), p. 19. Elsevier: Amsterdam.Google Scholar
Sirigu, A, Daprati, E, Ciancia, S, Giraux, P, Nighoghossian, N, Posada, A, Haggard, P (2004). Altered awareness of voluntary action after damage to the parietal cortex. Nature Neuroscience 7, 8084.Google Scholar
Sirigu, A, Duhamel, J-R, Cohen, L, Pillon, B, Dubois, B, Agid, Y (1996). The mental representation of hand movements after parietal cortex damage. Science 273, 15641568.Google Scholar
Slotnick, SD, Moo, LR, Segal, JB, Hart, J Jr. (2003). Distinct prefrontal cortex activity associated with item memory and source memory for visual shapes. Cognitive Brain Research 17, 7582.Google Scholar
Spence, SA, Crimlisk, HL, Cope, H, Ron, MA, Grasby, PM (2000). Discrete neurophysiological correlates in prefrontal cortex during hysterical and feigned disorder of movement. Lancet 355, 12431244.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spielberger, CD, Gorsuch, RL, Lushene, R, Vagg, PR, Jacobs, GA (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Consulting Psychologists Press: Palo Alto, CA.Google Scholar
Stamm, JS (1985). The uncertainty principle in psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8, 553554.Google Scholar
Tabu, H, Aso, T, Matsuhashi, M, Ueki, Y, Takahashi, R, Fukuyama, H, Shibasaki, H, Mima, T (2015). Parkinson's disease patients showed delayed awareness of motor intention. Neuroscience Research 95, 7477.Google Scholar
Terada, K, Ikeda, A, Van Ness, PC, Nagamine, T, Kaji, R, Kimura, J, Shibasaki, H (1995). Presence of Bereitschaftspotential preceding psychogenic myoclonus: clinical application of jerk-locked back averaging. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 58, 745747.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Poppelen, D, Saifee, TA, Schwingenschuh, P, Katschnig, P, Bhatia, KP, Tijssen, MA, Edwards, MJ (2011). Attention to self in psychogenic tremor. Movement Disorders 26, 25752576.Google Scholar
Voon, V, Brezing, C, Gallea, C, Hallett, M (2011). Aberrant supplementary motor complex and limbic activity during motor preparation in motor conversion disorder. Movement Disorders 26, 23962403.Google Scholar
Voon, V, Cavanna, AE, Coburn, K, Sampson, S, Reeve, A, LaFrance, WC Jr.; on behalf of the American Neuropsychiatric Association Committee for Research (2016). Functional neuroanatomy and neurophysiology of functional neurological disorders (conversion disorder). Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences 28, 168190.Google Scholar
Voon, V, Ekanayake, V, Wiggs, E, Kranick, S, Ameli, R, Harrison, NA, Hallett, M (2013). Response inhibition in motor conversion disorder. Movement Disorders 28, 612618.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Voon, V, Gallea, C, Hattori, N, Bruno, M, Ekanayake, V, Hallett, M (2010). The involuntary nature of conversion disorder. Neurology 74, 223228.Google Scholar
Whitfield-Gabrieli, S, Nieto-Castañón, A (2012). Conn: a functional connectivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated brain networks. Brain Connectivity 2, 125141.Google Scholar
Wolpe, N, Rowe, JB (2014). Beyond the “urge to move”: objective measures for the study of agency in the post-Libet era. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8, 450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Libet's clock task. (a) Schematic representation of the task. The red ball revolved around the unnumbered clock face for a maximum of three cycles; participants had to make a button press after waiting one cycle. The ball continued moving for a random interval, after which participants returned the ball to its position when they had felt the urge (W judgement) or actually pressed the button (M judgement). (b) Estimated times of intention (W judgement), movement (M judgement) and difference between intention and movement (W-M) relative to the recorded button press for functional neurological disorder (FND) patients and healthy controls (HV). Values are means, with standard errors represented by the horizontal bars. ++p = 0.017, * p = 0.009, ** p = 0.001. For a colour figure, see the online version of the paper.

Figure 1

Table 1. Patient characteristics

Figure 2

Table 2. W and M judgements as a function of functional symptom typea

Figure 3

Fig. 2. Attention to intention v. movement. (a) Significant activations associated with attention to intention compared with attention to movement (intention v. movement contrast) across all participants (n = 45). (b) Regions of significantly decreased activity for functional neurological disorder patients (n = 23) compared with healthy volunteers (n = 22) when attending to intention compared with attending to movement. Image displayed at p < 0.005 (uncorrected) for illustration. (c) Results of the correlation between the intention v. movement contrast and the behavioural measure W-M across all participants (n = 45). Image displayed at p < 0.001 (uncorrected) for illustration. pre-SMA, Pre-supplementary motor area; dmPFC, dorsomedial prefrontal cortex; IPL, inferior parietal lobule; dlPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; IFG, inferior frontal gyrus; M1, primary motor cortex.

Figure 4

Fig. 3. Resting-state functional connectivity from right inferior parietal cortex (IPC) seed. (a) Increased (functional neurological disorder patients > healthy volunteers; FND>HV) and (b) decreased (HV > FND) functional connectivity from IPC to whole brain for FND patients (n = 25) compared with HV (n = 70) during rest. Image displayed at p < 0.005 (uncorrected) for illustration. SMA, Supplementary motor area; IPL, inferior parietal lobule; dlPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; MTG, middle temporal gyrus; ITG, inferior temporal gyrus; BA, Brodmann area; PCC, posterior cingulate cortex.

Supplementary material: File

Baek supplementary material

Tables S1-S3

Download Baek supplementary material(File)
File 90.6 KB