Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-k7p5g Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-10T22:15:23.799Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Daily routines of pre-school children: effects of age, birth order, sex and social class, and developmental correlates

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 July 2009

Annette Lawson
Affiliation:
Medical Research Council Unit on Environmental Factors in Mental and Physical Illness, Londoncor1corresp
1
J. D. Ingleby
Affiliation:
Medical Research Council Unit on Environmental Factors in Mental and Physical Illness, Londoncor1corresp
1

Sysnopsis

Children's daily activities and interactions were quantified from mothers' accounts of the preceding 24-hour period. These ‘diaries’ were obtained on three occasions from 54 families with two pre-school children. Effects of age, birth order, sex, and social class were found which support the findings of other studies: an inverted U-shaped relationship existed between the intensity of attention received and the child's intelligence and developmental quotients. A relatively high correlation between IQ and an affective measure of child care furnished further evidence that the differences in caretaking relevant to intellectual development are qualitative rather than quantitative ones.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1974

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Clark, A. H., Wyon, S. M., and Richards, M. P. M. (1969). Free-play in nursery school children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 10, 205216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coleman, J. S. (1966). Equality of Educational Opportunity. U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare: Washington.Google Scholar
Coleman, R. W., and Provence, S. (1957). Environmental retardation (hospitalism) in infants living in families. Pediatrics, 19, 285292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooper, J. (1970). The Leyton Obsessional Inventory. Psychological Medicine, 1, 4864.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooper, J., and McNeil, J. (1968). A study of houseproud housewives and their interaction with their children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 9, 173188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denenberg, V. H. (1962). The effects of early experience. In The Behaviour of Domestic Animals, pp. 109138. Edited by Hafez, E. S. E.. Baillière: London.Google Scholar
Douglas, J. W. B. (1964). The Home and the School. MacGibbon and Kee: London.Google Scholar
Douglas, J. W. B., Ross, J. M., and Simpson, H. R. (1968). All Our Future: a longitudinal study of secondary education. Davies: London.Google Scholar
Douglas, J. W. B., Lawson, A., Cooper, J. E., and Cooper, E. (1968). Family interaction and the activities of young children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 9, 157171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Douglas, J. W. B., Cooper, J. E., Lawson, A., Cooper, E., McNeil, J., Alderman, B., and Ingleby, J. D. (1971). Family Interaction and the Activities of Children. Unpublished internal report (available from authors).Google Scholar
Fraser, E. (1959). Home Environment and the School. University of London Press: London.Google Scholar
Goldfarb, W. (1943). Infant rearing and problem behavior. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 13, 249625.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldfarb, W. (1945). Psychological privation in infancy and subsequent adjustment. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 15, 247255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodman, L. A., and Kruskal, W. H. (1954, 1963). Measures of association for cross classifications. Parts 1 and 3. Journal of American Statistical Association, 49, 732764; 58, 310364.Google Scholar
Koch, H. L. (1954). Child psychology. Annual Review of Psychology, 5, 126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levine, S. (1956). A further study of infantile handling and adult avoidance learning. Journal of Personality, 25, 7080.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, M., and Wilson, C. D. (1972). Infant development in lower-class American families. Human Development, 15, 112127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moss, H. A. (1967). Sex, age, and state as determinants of mother-infant interaction. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 13, 1936.Google Scholar
Newson, J., and Newson, E. (1963). Infant Care in an Urban Community. Allen and Unwin: London.Google Scholar
Newson, J., and Newson, E. (1968). Four Years Old in an Urban Community. Allen and Unwin: London.Google Scholar
Provence, S. A., and Lipton, R. C. (1962). Infants in Institutions. International Universities Press: New York.Google Scholar
Rubenstein, J. (1967). Maternal attentiveness and subsequent exploratory behavior in the infant. Child Development, 38, 10891100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skeels, H. M., and Dye, H. B. (1939). A study of the effects of differential stimulation on mentally retarded children. Proceedings and Addresses of the American Association on Mental Deficiency, 63, Part 1, 114136.Google Scholar
Skeels, H. M. (1942). A study of the effects of differential stimulation on mentally retarded children: A follow-up report. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 46, 340350.Google Scholar
Skeels, H. M. (1966). Adult status of children with contrasting early life experiences. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 31, No. 3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, P. K., and Connolly, K. (1972). Patterns of play and social interaction in pre-school children. In Ethological Studies of Child Behaviour, pp. 6595. Edited by Blurton Jones, N.. Cambridge University Press: London.Google Scholar
Spitz, R. A. (1945, 1946). Hospitalism. An inquiry into the genesis of psychiatric conditions in early childhood. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 1, 5374. (A follow-up report, 2, 113117.)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thoman, E. B., Turner, A. M., Leiderman, P. H., and Barnett, C. R. (1970). Neonate-mother interaction: effects of parity on feeding behavior. Child Development, 41, 11031111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tulkin, S. R., and Kagan, J. (1972). Mother-child interaction in the first year of life. Child Development, 43, 3141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woods, P. J., Ruckelshaus, S. I., and Bowling, D. M. (1960) Some effects of “free” and “restricted” environmenta rearing conditions upon adult behavior in the rat. Psychological Reports, 6, 191200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yarrow, L. J., Rubenstein, J. L., Pedersen, F. A., and Jankowski, J. J. (1972). Dimensions of early stimulation and their differential effects on infant development, Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 18, 205218.Google Scholar