Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-2lccl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T21:21:09.533Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Age-related brain deviations and aggression

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 April 2022

Nathalie E. Holz*
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim/Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany Donders Center for Brain, Cognition and Behavior, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands Department for Cognitive Neuroscience, Radboud University Medical Center Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
Dorothea L. Floris
Affiliation:
Donders Center for Brain, Cognition and Behavior, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands Department for Cognitive Neuroscience, Radboud University Medical Center Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands Methods of Plasticity Research, Department of Psychology, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
Alberto Llera
Affiliation:
Donders Center for Brain, Cognition and Behavior, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
Pascal M. Aggensteiner
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim/Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany
Seyed Mostafa Kia
Affiliation:
Donders Center for Brain, Cognition and Behavior, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands Department for Cognitive Neuroscience, Radboud University Medical Center Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
Thomas Wolfers
Affiliation:
Donders Center for Brain, Cognition and Behavior, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands Department for Cognitive Neuroscience, Radboud University Medical Center Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands NORMENT, KG Jebsen Centre for Psychosis Research, Division of Mental Health and Addiction, Oslo University Hospital & Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
Sarah Baumeister
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim/Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany
Boris Böttinger
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim/Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany
Jeffrey C. Glennon
Affiliation:
Department for Cognitive Neuroscience, Radboud University Medical Center Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
Pieter J. Hoekstra
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
Andrea Dietrich
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
Melanie C. Saam
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry/Psychotherapy, University Hospital, University of Ulm, Ulm, Germany
Ulrike M. E. Schulze
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry/Psychotherapy, University Hospital, University of Ulm, Ulm, Germany
David J. Lythgoe
Affiliation:
Department of Neuroimaging, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK
Steve C. R. Williams
Affiliation:
Department of Neuroimaging, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK
Paramala Santosh
Affiliation:
Department of Child Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK Centre for Interventional Paediatric Psychopharmacology and Rare Diseases (CIPPRD), South London and Maudsley NHS Trust, London, UK
Mireia Rosa-Justicia
Affiliation:
Clinic Image Diagnostic Center (CDIC), Hospital Clinic of Barcelona; Magnetic Resonance Image Core Facility, IDIBAPS, Barcelona, Spain Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychology Department, Institute Clinic of Neurosciences, Hospital Clinic of Barcelona, IDIBAPS, Barcelona, Spain
Nuria Bargallo
Affiliation:
Clinic Image Diagnostic Center (CDIC), Hospital Clinic of Barcelona; Magnetic Resonance Image Core Facility, IDIBAPS, Barcelona, Spain
Josefina Castro-Fornieles
Affiliation:
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychology Department, Department of Medicine, 2017SGR881, Institute Clinic of Neurosciences, Hospital Clinic of Barcelona, CIBERSAM, IDIBAPS, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
Celso Arango
Affiliation:
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Department, Institute of Psychiatry and Mental health, Hospital General Universitario Gregorio Marañón School of Medicine, Universidad Complutense, IiSGM, CIBERSAM, Madrid, Spain
Maria J. Penzol
Affiliation:
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Department, Institute of Psychiatry and Mental health, Hospital General Universitario Gregorio Marañón School of Medicine, Universidad Complutense, IiSGM, CIBERSAM, Madrid, Spain
Susanne Walitza
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Psychiatric Hospital, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
Andreas Meyer-Lindenberg
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim/Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany
Marcel Zwiers
Affiliation:
Donders Center for Brain, Cognition and Behavior, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
Barbara Franke
Affiliation:
Department of Human Genetics, Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmegen, The Netherlands Department of Psychiatry, Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
Jan Buitelaar
Affiliation:
Donders Center for Brain, Cognition and Behavior, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands Department for Cognitive Neuroscience, Radboud University Medical Center Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands Karakter Child and Adolescent Psychiatry University Center, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
Jilly Naaijen
Affiliation:
Donders Center for Brain, Cognition and Behavior, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands Department for Cognitive Neuroscience, Radboud University Medical Center Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
Daniel Brandeis
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim/Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychology Department, Institute Clinic of Neurosciences, Hospital Clinic of Barcelona, IDIBAPS, Barcelona, Spain Center for Integrative Human Physiology, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland Neuroscience Center Zurich, University of Zurich and ETH Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
Christian Beckmann
Affiliation:
Donders Center for Brain, Cognition and Behavior, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands Department for Cognitive Neuroscience, Radboud University Medical Center Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands Centre for Functional MRI of the Brain, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
Tobias Banaschewski
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim/Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany
Andre F. Marquand*
Affiliation:
Donders Center for Brain, Cognition and Behavior, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands Department for Cognitive Neuroscience, Radboud University Medical Center Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands Department of Neuroimaging, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK
*
Authors for correspondence: Nathalie E. Holz, E-mail: nathalie.holz@zi-mannheim.de; Andre F. Marquand, E-mail: a.marquand@donders.ru.nl
Authors for correspondence: Nathalie E. Holz, E-mail: nathalie.holz@zi-mannheim.de; Andre F. Marquand, E-mail: a.marquand@donders.ru.nl
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Disruptive behavior disorders (DBD) are heterogeneous at the clinical and the biological level. Therefore, the aims were to dissect the heterogeneous neurodevelopmental deviations of the affective brain circuitry and provide an integration of these differences across modalities.

Methods

We combined two novel approaches. First, normative modeling to map deviations from the typical age-related pattern at the level of the individual of (i) activity during emotion matching and (ii) of anatomical images derived from DBD cases (n = 77) and controls (n = 52) aged 8–18 years from the EU-funded Aggressotype and MATRICS consortia. Second, linked independent component analysis to integrate subject-specific deviations from both modalities.

Results

While cases exhibited on average a higher activity than would be expected for their age during face processing in regions such as the amygdala when compared to controls these positive deviations were widespread at the individual level. A multimodal integration of all functional and anatomical deviations explained 23% of the variance in the clinical DBD phenotype. Most notably, the top marker, encompassing the default mode network (DMN) and subcortical regions such as the amygdala and the striatum, was related to aggression across the whole sample.

Conclusions

Overall increased age-related deviations in the amygdala in DBD suggest a maturational delay, which has to be further validated in future studies. Further, the integration of individual deviation patterns from multiple imaging modalities allowed to dissect some of the heterogeneity of DBD and identified the DMN, the striatum and the amygdala as neural signatures that were associated with aggression.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Central Institute of Mental Health, 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Aggressive behavior is commonly seen in disruptive behavior disorders (DBD), including oppositional defiant disorder (ODD) and conduct disorder (CD), prevalent in 5.7% of the youth population (Polanczyk, Salum, Sugaya, Caye, & Rohde, Reference Polanczyk, Salum, Sugaya, Caye and Rohde2015). Given frequent comorbidity, both disorders have been conceptualized on a continuum with CD being more severe regarding the presentation of aggressive symptoms (Pardini, Frick, & Moffitt, Reference Pardini, Frick and Moffitt2010). Research investigating the neurobiology of DBD has highlighted their complex and multifaceted nature, reflected both in altered brain structure and function. In particular, regions that support affective processing (Kohls et al., Reference Kohls, Baumann, Gundlach, Scharke, Bernhard, Martinelli and Konrad2020) and executive functions have been shown to be disrupted in DBD (Blair, Veroude, & Buitelaar, Reference Blair, Veroude and Buitelaar2018; Fairchild et al., Reference Fairchild, Hawes, Frick, Copeland, Odgers, Franke and De Brito2019). As such, recent meta-analyses provided evidence for functional differences in the insula, the fusiform gyrus and top-down control regions such as the medial frontal gyrus and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during emotion processing in DBD when compared to healthy controls (Alegria, Radua, & Rubia, Reference Alegria, Radua and Rubia2016; Noordermeer, Luman, & Oosterlaan, Reference Noordermeer, Luman and Oosterlaan2016). Notably, highly heterogeneous results exist for the amygdala with hyperactivity and hypoactivity observed depending on the aggression subtype (Aggensteiner et al., Reference Aggensteiner, Holz, Bottinger, Baumeister, Hohmann, Werhahn and Brandeis2020; Lozier, Cardinale, VanMeter, & Marsh, Reference Lozier, Cardinale, VanMeter and Marsh2014). In line with this, inconsistencies also exist between results of meta-analyses, where differences in the amygdala have been detected in one meta-analysis (Noordermeer et al., Reference Noordermeer, Luman and Oosterlaan2016), but not in another (Alegria et al., Reference Alegria, Radua and Rubia2016). In addition to these functional alterations, structural differences have been detected in a meta-analysis indicating reduced volumes in similar regions in youths with conduct problems, i.e. the insula, the medial frontal gyrus, the fusiform gyrus, and the amygdala (Rogers & De Brito, Reference Rogers and De Brito2016). By analogy to functional findings, some abnormalities in volume also seem to be influenced by the presence of callous-unemotional (CU) traits (Rogers & De Brito, Reference Rogers and De Brito2016). In general, a good understanding of brain development in DBD is largely lacking. This is despite evidence for a strong neurodevelopmental component, especially for early-onset DBD, implicating an origin in infancy or childhood, atypical brain development, neurocognitive deficits and a life-course-persistent manifestation (Raine, Reference Raine2018).

Overall, recent attempts to uncover the neurobiology of DBD have largely focused on investigating subgroups of patients (Blair, Leibenluft, & Pine, Reference Blair, Leibenluft and Pine2015) based on biology and/or on symptoms. However, especially regarding CD, highly heterogeneous symptom profiles qualify for a diagnosis (Fairchild et al., Reference Fairchild, Hawes, Frick, Copeland, Odgers, Franke and De Brito2019; Viding & McCrory, Reference Viding and McCrory2020). The dominant case–control analysis approach provides inferences about the ‘average patient’ of a particular group and by definition does not provide information on inter-individual differences within each grouping but rather considers these as noise. Finally, as mentioned above, DBD has been shown to affect multiple quantitative phenotypes rather than single features, suggesting that integration of different imaging modalities would allow for a more refined, comprehensive neural characterization of DBD.

Therefore, the aim of this study was to understand individual neurobiological variations beyond group-level differences by investigating functional and structural imaging data via a normative modeling approach that models individual differences while not requiring dysfunctions or alterations to overlap (Marquand et al., Reference Marquand, Kia, Zabihi, Wolfers, Buitelaar and Beckmann2019). Notably, this method has already been successfully applied to model development in schizophrenia, bipolar disorder (Wolfers et al., Reference Wolfers, Doan, Kaufmann, Alnaes, Moberget, Agartz and Marquand2018) and autism (Bethlehem et al., Reference Bethlehem, Seidlitz, Romero-Garcia, Trakoshis, Dumas and Lombardo2020; Floris et al., Reference Floris, Wolfers, Zabihi, Holz, Zwiers, Charman and Group2021; Zabihi et al., Reference Zabihi, Oldehinkel, Wolfers, Frouin, Goyard, Loth and Marquand2019). Given previous evidence for abnormalities in emotion processing and its underlying structures, we first applied normative modeling (Marquand et al., Reference Marquand, Kia, Zabihi, Wolfers, Buitelaar and Beckmann2019) to a cross-sectional dataset, where variation in each time point across individuals is sampled, to investigate how DBD cases deviate from an expected pattern of brain development. Second, since DBD is likely to be explained by multimodal neurobiological variation, we integrated structural and functional deviations from two different imaging modalities by linked independent component analysis [LICA (Groves, Beckmann, Smith, & Woolrich, Reference Groves, Beckmann, Smith and Woolrich2011; Llera, Wolfers, Mulders, & Beckmann, Reference Llera, Wolfers, Mulders and Beckmann2019)] to provide a comprehensive model to explain aggressive behavior at a symptom level.

Method

Study sample

Participants in the current study were part of the EU-funded Aggressotype and MATRICS projects. The assessment followed a strict standard operating protocol, which was consistent across all sites. The first day consisted of a standardized interview, questionnaires to assess psychopathology, behavior, and sociodemographic information and a neuropsychological assessment (approximately 3 h). On the second day, the participants took part in an MRI session (approximately 1.5 h). In total 208 participants (129 cases, 79 controls) aged 8–18 years were assessed using functional MRI (fMRI) and anatomical scans were acquired in n = 248 (154 cases, 94 controls) across nine sites in Europe. Exclusion criteria for all participants were any contraindications for MRI, an IQ<80 [as measured by four subtests (vocabulary, similarities, block design, and picture completion/matrix reasoning) of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-IV (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2011)], and a primary DSM-5 diagnosis of psychosis, bipolar disorder, major depression, and/or an anxiety disorder. Participants who were included as cases with DBD were diagnosed with ODD and/or CD based on the structured diagnostic interviews with the child and parents according to DSM-5, and/or scored above the clinical cut-off for aggressive behavior and/or rule-breaking behavior as measured with the Child Behavior Checklist completed by parents, teachers and/or youths themselves [CBCL/TRF/YSR; (Achenbach, Howell, Quay, & Conners, Reference Achenbach, Howell, Quay and Conners1991)]. In the control group, no DSM axis I disorder, assessed via the K-SADS (Kaufman et al., Reference Kaufman, Birmaher, Brent, Rao, Flynn, Moreci and Ryan1997), and no clinical score in the CBCL, TRF or YSR was allowed, according to standard cutoffs for these instruments. For cases with DBD, medication had to be stable for at least two weeks prior to inclusion. The parent-rated Inventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits (ICU) (Essau, Sasagawa, & Frick, Reference Essau, Sasagawa and Frick2006) and the self-reported Reactive Proactive Aggression Questionnaire (RPQ) (Raine et al., Reference Raine, Dodge, Loeber, Gatzke-Kopp, Lynam, Reynolds and Liu2006) were used to assess features of aggression. ADHD symptoms were measured with the parent-rated SNAP-IV questionnaire (Bussing et al., Reference Bussing, Fernandez, Harwood, Wei, Garvan, Eyberg and Swanson2008). Pubertal status was assessed with the Pubertal Development Scale (Petersen, Crockett, Richards, & Boxer, Reference Petersen, Crockett, Richards and Boxer1988). The internal consistency of all measures ranged from good to excellent (for details see Rosa-Justicia et al., Reference Rosa-Justicia, Saam, Flamarique, Borras, Naaijen, Dietrich and Castro-Fornieles2022). Correlations between all aggression measures are provided in the online Supplementary Fig. S1. The participants received 85 € for their participation. Ethical approval for the study was obtained for all sites separately by local ethics committees. The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. Written informed consent was given by the participants and their parents or legal representatives.

fMRI task

Participants performed an emotional face-matching task (Hariri, Bookheimer, & Mazziotta, Reference Hariri, Bookheimer and Mazziotta2000). In total two emotion matching blocks and two blocks of a sensorimotor control task were presented. During the first, one block consisted of six stimuli depicting a trio of faces with either negative (anger and fear) or positive/neutral faces (happy and neutral) in which the participants had to select one of two emotions (displayed on the bottom) identical to the target stimulus (displayed on the top). Interleaved between these blocks, participants completed two blocks of a sensorimotor control task with one block consisting of six geometric shapes (horizontal ellipses or vertical ellipses) that had to be matched accordingly (for details see online Supplementary Fig. S2 in the Supplementary material). Subjects responded by pressing one of two buttons with their right hand according to the match in emotion or shape. The task lasted 3:57 min. This task robustly elicited the face-processing network in this sample, including the amygdala (Aggensteiner et al., Reference Aggensteiner, Holz, Bottinger, Baumeister, Hohmann, Werhahn and Brandeis2020).

Functional images

MRI scans were performed in nine different sites across Europe. Whole-brain functional emotion matching data were acquired with echo-planar T2*-weighted imaging (EPI), sensitive to the Blood Oxygenation Level Dependent (BOLD) signal contrast (36 axial slices, 3 × 3 × 3 mm, slice gap 0.4 mm, TR = 2100 ms, TE = 35 ms, see Supplementary material text and online Supplementary Table S1). Data were analyzed using SPM12 (https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/) with standard preprocessing steps. Participants with head movement >3 mm or 3° were excluded. Data from five sites were included in the final analysis to reach a minimum of five cases and controls per site, resulting in a sample of N = 140 (85 cases). A further 11 participants were excluded due to residual movement artefacts in the GLM regression coefficients and the Z statistic images [N = 129 (77 cases), see supplement]. There was no systematic bias with regard to demographic and clinical data between included and excluded data sets (see Supplementary material and online Supplementary Table S2). To harmonize the data regarding site effects, scanner difference was modeled and removed from the scans using ComBat (Johnson, Li, & Rabinovic, Reference Johnson, Li and Rabinovic2007).

Anatomical images

In addition to the functional data, an additional high-resolution structural magnetization-prepared rapid gradient echo (MP-RAGE) scan was also acquired at a resolution of 1 × 1 × 1.2 mm. Scan parameters derived from the ADNI-2/ADNI-Go project (Jack et al., Reference Jack, Bernstein, Borowski, Gunter, Fox, Thompson and Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging2010) were consistent across the four sites using a Siemens scanner (TR = 2300, TE = 2.96/2.98) and comparable at the GE site, see online Supplementary Table S3. The complete anatomical data set comprised 248 participants. Out of this sample, n = 129 had both functional and structural data available. Two scans had to be excluded due to bad image quality, leaving a final sample of 127 participants. Pre-processing was done using the anatomical processing tools implemented in FSL (details described in the Supplementary material). For further analyses, Jacobian determinants (JDs) of the deformation fields were used as anatomical features (see Supplementary material). In a further step, scanner variance in these images was removed using ComBat (Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Li and Rabinovic2007).

Constructing normative models of brain structure and function

A model of brain development as a function of age and sex was created by training a Gaussian process regression (GPR) model, which is a nonparametric nonlinear regression technique with additive Gaussian noise, on a sample of n = 129 and n = 127 (77 cases, Table 1, online Supplementary Fig. S3 for an age distribution) to predict brain function and structure, respectively (Fig. 1a). The model can be set up in two ways, using either the whole cohort (both healthy participants and cases) or only the healthy participants as the reference cohort. Applying either has been shown to make little difference (Marquand et al., Reference Marquand, Kia, Zabihi, Wolfers, Buitelaar and Beckmann2019). Here, we used the whole cohort (both TD and DBD) as a reference due to the small sample size of the intersection between anatomical and functional data implying that the frequency of DBD in the normative model (n = 77) is higher than in the control population, which means that results ought to be interpreted with respect to this specific cohort. Note that in our analysis GPR predictions are derived in an unbiased manner under 10-fold cross-validation. Briefly, this Bayesian approach calculates the probability distribution over all functions that fit the data while specifying prior over all possible values and relocating probabilities based on evidence (i.e. observed data). As such, it yields unbiased estimates of generalizability and inferences with increasing uncertainty with fewer data. This, in turn, increases the conservativeness of this approach and renders deviations harder to detect.

Fig. 1. Methodological overview. (a). A normative model was estimated from brain structure and function across typically developing controls (TDs) and DBD cases, which allowed us to estimate normative probability maps, showing the regional deviations from the expected pattern in each subject. (b) These deviations were integrated using linked ICA, which yielded 20 components that were related to behavioral aggression measures.

Table 1. Sample characteristics

As a sensitivity analysis, we leveraged our larger anatomical dataset (n = 248) before the intersection with the smaller fMRI data set to compare results using different reference cohorts, which yielded similar results (see online Supplementary Table S4).

To estimate a pattern of regional deviations from typical brain structure and function for each participant, we derived a normative probability map (NPM) that quantifies the voxel-wise deviation from the normative model. This was done by using the normative model to predict estimates of brain structure and function for each individual participant, then calculate a subject-specific Z score (Marquand, Rezek, Buitelaar, & Beckmann, Reference Marquand, Rezek, Buitelaar and Beckmann2016) indicating the difference between the predicted and true brain activity (or structural measure) scaled by the prediction variance. The NPMs were thresholded at Z = ± 2.6 (i.e. p < 0.005) as in Floris et al. (Reference Floris, Wolfers, Zabihi, Holz, Zwiers, Charman and Group2021), Wolfers et al. (Reference Wolfers, Doan, Kaufmann, Alnaes, Moberget, Agartz and Marquand2018) to facilitate the comparison across participants and to have a more sensitive marker for small individual deviations when compared to false discovery rate correction.

The total number of (absolute) mean deviations from DBD cases and controls were extracted and compared using the non-parametric Mann–Whitney U Test. Multiple comparison were controlled for using FDR correction with the Benjamini–Hochberg method (Benjamini & Hochberg, Reference Benjamini and Hochberg1995).

Mean threshold-free, cluster-enhanced group differences between DBD cases and controls were estimated using Permutation Analysis of Linear Models (PALM) (Winkler, Webster, Vidaurre, Nichols, & Smith, Reference Winkler, Webster, Vidaurre, Nichols and Smith2015) on the normative deviation maps and family-wise error corrected for multiple comparisons.

Linked ICA

We used linked ICA (Groves et al., Reference Groves, Beckmann, Smith and Woolrich2011; Llera et al., Reference Llera, Wolfers, Mulders and Beckmann2019) to integrate the unthresholded normative (Z-transformed) deviation maps across four data modalities: negative faces, positive/neutral faces, and shapes as well as anatomy (Fig. 1b) to tackle the potential overlap of effects across brain regions. Each measure is considered as a different ‘modality’ in this context in the sense that the algorithm will learn the different noise characteristics for each of these data types. Linked ICA is a Bayesian multimodal extension of the ICA model (Groves et al., Reference Groves, Beckmann, Smith and Woolrich2011; Llera et al., Reference Llera, Wolfers, Mulders and Beckmann2019) that provides simultaneous factorizations across multiple data modalities, while linking them at the subject level through a shared mixing matrix reflecting subject-wise contributions to each independent component (1 scalar value per participant). Further, each independent component provides also a map of spatial variation per modality and a vector reflecting the relative contribution of each modality to the component (Beckmann & Smith, Reference Beckmann and Smith2005). Given our sample size and following recommendations described in earlier papers (Wolfers et al., Reference Wolfers, Arenas, Onnink, Dammers, Hoogman, Zwiers and Beckmann2017), linked ICA was used to estimate 20 independent components. Although the results were not critically dependent on this parameter value, such model order provided also a superior performance in predicting DBD cases from controls when compared to 30 independent components in terms of Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) and the Bayesian Information Criterion (see online Supplementary Fig. S4). For visualization, the spatial maps were converted to pseudo-Z-statistics and thresholded at |Z| > 2.6.

Behavioral correlatess

Given the high correlations between the aggression measures (online Supplementary Fig. S1), a principal component analysis with VARIMAX rotation was performed that included the DSM-oriented subscales conduct problems and oppositional defiant problems from the CBCL as well as the total ICU score and reactive and proactive aggression from the RPQ. This yielded one factor that explained 67% of the variance in the aggression measures. Spearman correlations were performed to relate the deviations from each modality separately to the aggression factor. Further, to assess how combined deviations across all modalities act in concert to explain psychopathology, all 20 components were entered into a logistic regression to predict DBD diagnosis. Further, each of the 20 linked ICA components was associated with the aggression factor using FDR correction and was tested as being different between cases and controls using Mann–Whitney U test.

Sensitivity analyses

Significant correlations between relevant independent components and the aggression factor were further controlled for IQ, binary medication use, internalizing symptoms, ADHD symptoms and pubertal status using nonparametric partial correlations.

In addition, we fed the unmodeled contrast maps and JD's (i.e. without fitting any normative model) in the LICA, which indicated a better model fit of the linked ICA based on the deviation scores to predict DBD (details depicted in the supplement). While this was not an explicit aim of this study, our findings illustrate a benefit for normative modeling relative to applying stratification tools to the data directly. This can be attributed to the accurate, non-linear modeling of demographic characteristics using GPR and the placement of each subject with respect to centiles of variance in a common reference model. Importantly, this yields z-statistics that are consistently calibrated across modalities which enables the subsequent LICA to explain slightly more variance and – more importantly – detect stronger associations with categorical and dimensional clinical measures. This is in line with recent evidence of the increased predictive performance of cortical volume deviation scores over raw scores regarding psychopathology (Parkes et al., Reference Parkes, Moore, Calkins, Cook, Cieslak, Roalf and Bassett2021).

Results

Sample

Out of the 77 included cases, 33 were diagnosed with ODD, four with CD, and 15 with ODD/CD. The other 25 participants were included as ‘clinical case’ based on a CBCL/TRF/YSR aggression and/or rule-breaking behavior subscale T ⩾ 70. As expected, the case group consisted of more males, had a lower IQ, and scored higher on all measures of aggression (see online Supplementary Fig. S5 for the distribution) and ADHD as compared to controls (Table 1). No differences across site or site by group effects were seen regarding demographics and clinical variables (online Supplementary Table S5). Cases and controls did not differ regarding the movement in the scanner (translation: U = 1755, Z = −1.186, p = .24; rotation: U = 1663, Z = −1.63, p = 0.11) or task performance.

Normative models

The accuracy of the normative models for predicting brain structure and function was evaluated using the correlation between the true and the predicted voxel values ρ for cases and controls (online Supplementary Fig. S6).

As shown in Fig. 2a, decreases as well as increases of brain activity are observed in the functional images, while mostly relative contractions are seen in the anatomical images (see Supplementary material for details).

Fig. 2. (a) Spatial representation of the voxel-wise normative model for all modalities in males. The panels show the beta values depicting the change across 8 to 18 years of age for negative face processing, positive/neutral face processing, shape processing and anatomical images. Red and blue indicate an increase or decline, respectively, in activity or relative expansion or contraction to match the template for the Jacobian determinants. (b) Mean positive deviations from the model for controls and cases. Cases showed significantly more positive deviations, i.e. a higher age-related increase in prefrontal and limbic (i.e. cluster comprising the amygdala) activity presumably during negative and positive/neutral face matching. For visualization purposes only the left side is shown.

Normative modeling-mean age-related deviations

PALM analyses yielded significant group differences in regional deviations including the parahippocampal gyrus/amygdala, the fusiform gyrus, the middle temporal gyrus and the cuneus for negative faces only (Fig. 3). Thus, this indicates that – on average – cases exhibit more age-related activation deviations in these regions, which can be considered as an age-by-diagnosis interaction.

Fig. 3. Cases showed increased deviations, i.e. higher age-related activity, from the normative model during negative face processing in the amygdala, the parahippocampal gyrus, the (inferior) temporal gyrus, the fusiform gyrus and the cuneus when compared to controls. This is suggestive of delayed maturational trajectories of brain activation in DBD cases.

Normative modeling-DBD cases show more positive deviations

Participants with DBD showed more individual positive deviations when compared to controls with regard to all functional measures (Fig. 2b; online Supplementary Table S6 in the Supplementary material) indicating relative age-related increases in activity from childhood to late adolescence at the individual level (that are not necessarily consistent across the cohort). No significant differences emerged with regard to negative deviations and with regard to the JDs (all comparisons p > 0.05).

Behavioral associations

While the correlation between positive deviations from the normative model of negative face processing and the aggression factor fell short of significance after FDR correction (ρ = .19, p FDR = 0.06), deviations from the normative model of positive/neutral face processing were significantly associated with aggression (ρ = 0.24, p FDR = 0.036). No significant correlations were found regarding deviation from shape processing.

Linked ICA

Each of the 20 linked ICA components represents the spatial pattern of the deviations. The components are depicted in Fig. 4a, i.e. the loadings of each modality for each component. Participants' loadings of each of the 20 imaging markers based on the deviation maps of the functional and anatomical data together predicted 23% (Mc Faddens R 2) of the variance of DBD (χ2 = 39.2, df = 20, p = 0.006; Fig. 4a). Two components significantly differed between cases and controls at the nominal level (component 5: U = 1515, Z = −2.08, p uncorr = 0.04, p FDR = 0.28, component 8: U = 1417, Z = −2.56, p uncorr = 0.01, p FDR = 0.20) but did not survive FDR correction. The latter multimodal component (number 8) showed additional associations with the aggression factor across the whole sample that survived multiple comparison correction (p FDR = 0.02). This component loaded most strongly on deviations from positive/neutral faces and shape processing with a negligible loading on anatomic deviations (Fig. 4b) and explained 9% of the variance in the aggression factor. The corresponding spatial maps (Fig. 4a right side) show that component 8 is strongly reflective of the default mode network (DMN), with high weightings in the precuneus and the medial prefrontal cortex. Moreover, both positive and neutral face and shape processing show additional weightings in the (ventral) striatum (nucleus accumbens, caudate), the amygdala/hippocampus, and the temporal gyrus, while the spatial map of negative face processing loaded also to the thalamus, caudate, post- and precentral gyrus, and superior frontal gyrus. The results remained unchanged or similar after the sensitivity analyses (presented in the Supplementary material).

Fig. 4. Linked ICA. (a). Loading matrix showing the contribution of each modality to each of the 20 components (blue: negative faces, orange: positive/neutral faces, green: shapes, red: anatomy). The underlying brain pattern of component 8 (in the matrix number 8) per functional modality is depicted on the right side (thresholded at Z > 2). (b) Component 8 was negatively associated with the aggression factor, which was still significant when outliers were excluded (p = 0.003).

Discussion

The present investigation provides several novel findings in relation to DBD: First, we provide evidence for aberrant age-related brain structure and function in cases with DBD. As such, on average, DBD cases showed increased deviations from the normative pattern in the face-processing network, including the fusiform gyrus, the amygdala, and the temporal gyrus during negative affective processing, which is indicative of higher age-related activity when compared to controls. Second, by moving beyond case–control differences, our normative modeling approach showed striking, widespread patterns of more positive age-related deviations in and beyond the face processing circuit in the DBD cases that are indicative of an increased age-related activity at the level of the individual. Third, assuming multi-faceted manifestations across multiple quantitative phenotypes in aggression, the integration of modalities explained almost one fourth of the variance in DBD symptomatology, thus revealing that the inter-individual variation across these phenotypes is matched. The most predictive marker for aggression on a continuous level was multimodal, predominantly affected by positive/neutral face processing and shape processing. Taken together, our results (i) provide the first evidence for neurodevelopmental atypicalities reflected in higher age-related brain activity in DBD, (ii) underscore the importance of moving beyond case–control comparisons and provide inferences at the level of the individual and (iii) show the advantage to integrate across multiple data modalities.

Our normative modeling approach suggests that cases with DBD have a higher age-related activity during face processing that was region-specific at the group-level and widespread at the level of the individual. The latter is reflected in widespread non-focal patterns of atypical activation which might have been overshadowed by classical case–control differences that per nature require a consistent pattern of neurobiological variation within groups. Interestingly, only positive deviations were observed and classical case–control analyses emphasized age-related deviations in the amygdala during negative face processing. Specifically, in the right amygdala the normative model predicted a gradual decrease in activity as a function of age during negative face processing. Thus, one may posit that positive deviations in this region as seen in DBD, which imply a higher activity with respect to the trajectory as expected in the current sample, could point to a maturational delay. Note, however, that this interpretation might not be valid for other brain regions which followed different age trajectories as shown in Fig. 2a. Further, due to the cross-sectional nature of our study this interpretation ought to be further validated in large longitudinal studies enabling to make inferences about neurodevelopmental trajectories of the face-processing network across several modalities. Overall, this can complement the picture of a less synchronized pattern of brain development in adolescent-onset CD (Fairchild et al., Reference Fairchild, Toschi, Sully, Sonuga-Barke, Hagan, Diciotti and Passamonti2016) and altered developmental trajectories of emotion-relevant brain structures in DBD (Hummer, Wang, Kronenberger, Dunn, & Mathews, Reference Hummer, Wang, Kronenberger, Dunn and Mathews2015). In addition to higher activity during face processing, age-related positive deviations in shape processing were observed. This may point to abnormal cognitive perceptual processing, which might be attributable to increased ADHD symptoms in cases (Fuermaier et al., Reference Fuermaier, Hupen, De Vries, Muller, Kok, Koerts and Tucha2018).

The combination of several markers of deviation showed robust associations with DBD caseness. The top component related to aggression on a symptom level showed focal effects, presumably in the precuneus and the medial prefrontal cortex, which strongly resemble the DMN. Notably, this is in line with reports of reduced functional connectivity of the DMN in DBD (Broulidakis et al., Reference Broulidakis, Fairchild, Sully, Blumensath, Darekar and Sonuga-Barke2016) and positive associations between abnormalities of the DMN and impulsivity scores (Lu, Zhou, Zhang, Wang, & Yuan, Reference Lu, Zhou, Zhang, Wang and Yuan2017). Given the role of the DMN in self-referential cognitive processing (Andrews-Hanna, Reidler, Sepulcre, Poulin, & Buckner, Reference Andrews-Hanna, Reidler, Sepulcre, Poulin and Buckner2010), age-related deviations in the DMN might further contribute to deficient emotion and empathy processing that is often observed in DBD (Blair et al., Reference Blair, Leibenluft and Pine2015; Fairchild et al., Reference Fairchild, Hawes, Frick, Copeland, Odgers, Franke and De Brito2019).

Interestingly, the ventral striatum and the amygdala also loaded onto the top ICA component, which echoes previous reports emphasizing the importance of these structures in DBD (Fairchild et al., Reference Fairchild, Hawes, Frick, Copeland, Odgers, Franke and De Brito2019). The amygdala has been suggested to be one of the core regions disrupted in structure and function in DBD, thereby, likely contributing to the observed deficits in emotion recognition and affective empathy (Blair et al., Reference Blair, Veroude and Buitelaar2018; Fairchild et al., Reference Fairchild, Hawes, Frick, Copeland, Odgers, Franke and De Brito2019). Notably, the striatum, including the caudate, has been highlighted as being dysfunctional during reward processing with blunted activity during anticipation (Holz et al., Reference Holz, Boecker-Schlier, Buchmann, Blomeyer, Jennen-Steinmetz, Baumeister and Laucht2017), impaired representation of expected value information and increased punishment-related prediction error representation (White et al., Reference White, Pope, Sinclair, Fowler, Brislin, Williams and Blair2013). In sum, our findings are suggestive of a dysfunctional affective circuit, and provide the first evidence for multimodal age-dependent variation that are related to aggression, which, in turn, might result in impulsive decisions and frustration (Blair et al., Reference Blair, Veroude and Buitelaar2018).

While this study employed a well-phenotyped sample of cases with DBD and healthy controls from a harmonized multicenter cohort, several caveats should be taken into account. First, even larger sample sizes would allow for improved normative modeling, e.g. permitting to derive normative models for TDs only. However, given the similar results in the larger anatomical data set with TDs only as reference cohort, it can be assumed that the normative model across the whole sample is not biased (Marquand et al., Reference Marquand, Kia, Zabihi, Wolfers, Buitelaar and Beckmann2019). Second, while our data allowed investigating aggression dimensions, the sample size did not allow for a distinction between early onset and adolescent-onset DBD, the latter being linked to a less severe developmental trajectory (Fairchild et al., Reference Fairchild, Hawes, Frick, Copeland, Odgers, Franke and De Brito2019). However, evidence suggests that both subgroups exhibit deficits in core regions of emotion processing, such as the amygdala (Passamonti et al., Reference Passamonti, Fairchild, Goodyer, Hurford, Hagan, Rowe and Calder2010). Third, our normative model was constructed based on age and sex. While the findings are suggestive of sex differences, our sample size does not allow to statistically test this. Future studies with more power should investigate this interesting topic. Fourth, given that our results are based on a cross-sectional data set, future longitudinal studies are warranted to further examine individualized brain development in DBD. Fifth, the nature of the task which included simultaneous matching of neutral and positive faces might have diluted subtle effects. Based on our results of increased age-related differences at the individual level during this condition, future studies should disentangle whether individual neurodevelopmental deviations in DBD exist specifically for positive face processing. Sixth, the inclusion of participants with clinical aggression scores in the CBCL renders the DBD group more heterogeneous, which is a limitation in the case–control comparisons at the behavioral level but supports the use of the individual-based normative modeling approach.

In conclusion, our findings add substantially to our understanding of DBD. On the one hand, we used normative modeling to map the heterogeneous biological variation in brain function and structure underlying DBD as a function of age. We provide clear evidence for age-related differences in the affective circuit with involvement of the amygdala for these disorders and make progress toward developing individualized inferences in the spirit of precision medicine. On the other hand, our integrated analysis using linked ICA identifies the DMN along with the amygdala and the striatum, as common disrupted neurobiological signatures that may contribute to the emergence of aggression.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329172200068X

Financial support

This project has received funding from the European Union's Seventh Framework Programme for research, technological development and demonstration under grant agreement 602805 (AGGRESSOTYPE) and no 603016 (MATRICS). This manuscript reflects only the author's view and the European Union is not liable for any use that may be made of the information contained herein. NEH and TB gratefully acknowledge grant support from the German Research Foundation (grant numbers DFG HO 5674/2-1, GRK2350/1). NEH further acknowledges funding from the Olympia Morata Program of the University of Heidelberg, the Ministry of Science, Research and the Arts of the State of Baden-Württemberg, Germany (Special support program SARS CoV-2 pandemic), and the Radboud Excellence Fellowship. DLF is supported by funding from the European Union's Horizon 2020 research and innovation program under the Marie Skłodowska-Curie grant agreement No 101025785. BF received additional funding from a personal Vici grant of the Dutch Organisation for Scientific Research (grant 016-130-669) and from a grant for the Dutch National Science Agenda for the NWA NeurolabNL project (grant 400 17 602). AML acknowledges grant support by the German Research Foundation (DFG, Research Training Group GRK2350/1 project B02, Collaborative Research Center SFB 1158 project B09, Collaborative Research Center TRR 265 project S02, grant ME 1591/4-1), German Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF, grants 01EF1803A, 01ZX1314G, 01GQ1003B), European Union's Seventh Framework Programme (FP7, grants 602450, 602805, 115300, HEALTH-F2-2010-241909) (Horizon2020 CANDY grant 847818 and Eat2beNICE grant 728018), Innovative Medicines Initiative Joint Undertaking (IMI, grant 115008, PRISM grant 115916, EU-AIMS grant 115300, and AIMS-2-TRIALS grant 777394) and Ministry of Science, Research and the Arts of the State of Baden-Wuerttemberg, Germany (MWK, grant 42-04HV.MED(16)/16/1). CA and MJP have been supported by the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation. Instituto de Salud Carlos III (SAM16PE07CP1, PI16/02012, PI19/024), co-financed by ERDF Funds from the European Commission, ‘A way of making Europe’, CIBERSAM. Madrid Regional Government (B2017/BMD-3740 AGES-CM-2), Fundación Familia Alonso and Fundación Alicia Koplowitz. JN received additional funding from a personal Veni grant of the Dutch Organisation for Scientific Research (NWO grant V1.Veni.194.032). CFB gratefully acknowledges support from the Dutch Organisation for Scientific Research (NWO VICI grant 17854) and funding from the Wellcome Trust (Collaborative Award in Science 215573/Z/19/Z). AFM gratefully acknowledges support from the Dutch Organisation for Scientific Research (VIDI grant 016.156.415).

Conflict of interest

TB served in an advisory or consultancy role for Actelion, Hexal Pharma, Lilly, Lundbeck, Medice, Novartis and Shire. He received conference support or speaker's fees from Lilly, Medice, Novartis and Shire. He has been involved in clinical trials conducted by Shire and Viforpharma. He received royalties from Hogrefe, Kohlhammer, CIP Medien and Oxford University Press. PS's department has received his speaker's fees from Medice. DB serves as an unpaid scientific consultant for an EU-funded neurofeedback trial unrelated to the present work. AML has received consultant fees from American Association for the Advancement of Science, Atheneum Partners, Blueprint Partnership, Boehringer Ingelheim, Daimler und Benz Stiftung, Elsevier, F. Hoffmann-La Roche, ICARE Schizophrenia, K. G. Jebsen Foundation, L.E.K Consulting, Lundbeck International Foundation (LINF), R. Adamczak, Roche Pharma, Science Foundation, Sumitomo Dainippon Pharma, Synapsis Foundation – Alzheimer Research Switzerland, System Analytics, and has received lectures fees including travel fees from Boehringer Ingelheim, Fama Public Relations, Institut d'investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer (IDIBAPS), Janssen-Cilag, Klinikum Christophsbad, Göppingen, Lilly Deutschland, Luzerner Psychiatrie, LVR Klinikum Düsseldorf, LWL Psychiatrie Verbund Westfalen-Lippe, Otsuka Pharmaceuticals, Reunions i Ciencia S. L., Spanish Society of Psychiatry, Südwestrundfunk Fernsehen, Stern TV, and Vitos Klinikum Kurhessen. JKB has been a consultant to, advisory board member of, and a speaker for Takeda/Shire, Medice, Roche, Angelini, and Servier. He is not an employee of any of these companies and not a stock shareholder of any of these companies. He has no other financial or material support, including expert testimony, patents, or royalties. CFB is director and shareholder in SBGneuro Ltd. TC has received consultancy from Roche and received book royalties from Guildford Press and Sage. DM has been a consultant to, and advisory board member, for Roche and Servier. He is not an employee of any of these companies, and not a stock shareholder of any of these companies. CA has been a consultant to or has received honoraria or grants from Acadia, Angelini, Gedeon Richter, Janssen Cilag, Lundbeck, Minerva, Otsuka, Roche, Sage, Servier, Shire, Schering Plough, Sumitomo Dainippon Pharma, Sunovion and Takeda. The present work is unrelated to the above grants and relationships. The other authors report no potential conflicts of interest.

Footnotes

*

Shared first authorship.

Shared last authorship.

References

Achenbach, T. M., Howell, C. T., Quay, H. C., & Conners, C. K. (1991). National survey of problems and competencies among four- to sixteen-year-olds: Parents’ reports for normative and clinical samples. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 56(3), 1131. Retrieved from https://doi.org/110.1111/j.1540–5834.1991.tb01174.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aggensteiner, P. M., Holz, N. E., Bottinger, B. W., Baumeister, S., Hohmann, S., Werhahn, J. E., … Brandeis, D. (2020). The effects of callous-unemotional traits and aggression subtypes on amygdala activity in response to negative faces. Psychological Medicine, 52(3), 476484. doi:10.1017/S0033291720002111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alegria, A. A., Radua, J., & Rubia, K. (2016). Meta-analysis of fMRI studies of disruptive behavior disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 173(11), 11191130. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.15081089.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Reidler, J. S., Sepulcre, J., Poulin, R., & Buckner, R. L. (2010). Functional-anatomic fractionation of the brain's default network. Neuron, 65(4), 550562. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.02.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beckmann, C. F., & Smith, S. M. (2005). Tensorial extensions of independent component analysis for multisubject FMRI analysis. NeuroImage, 25(1), 294311. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.10.043.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple hypothesis testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B, 57, 289300.Google Scholar
Bethlehem, R. A. I., Seidlitz, J., Romero-Garcia, R., Trakoshis, S., Dumas, G., & Lombardo, M. V. (2020). A normative modelling approach reveals age-atypical cortical thickness in a subgroup of males with autism spectrum disorder. Communications Biology, 3(1), 486. doi: 10.1038/s42003-020-01212-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blair, R. J., Leibenluft, E., & Pine, D. S. (2015). Conduct disorder and callous-unemotional traits in youth. The New England Journal of Medicine, 372(8), 784. doi: 10.1056/NEJMc1415936.Google ScholarPubMed
Blair, R. J. R., Veroude, K., & Buitelaar, J. K. (2018). Neuro-cognitive system dysfunction and symptom sets: A review of fMRI studies in youth with conduct problems. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 91, 6990. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.10.022.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Broulidakis, M. J., Fairchild, G., Sully, K., Blumensath, T., Darekar, A., & Sonuga-Barke, E. J. (2016). Reduced default mode connectivity in adolescents with conduct disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 55(9), 800808 e801. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2016.05.021.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bussing, R., Fernandez, M., Harwood, M., Wei, H., Garvan, C. W., Eyberg, S. M., & Swanson, J. M. (2008). Parent and teacher SNAP-IV ratings of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms: Psychometric properties and normative ratings from a school district sample. Assessment, 15(3), 317328. doi: 10.1177/1073191107313888.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Essau, C. A., Sasagawa, S., & Frick, P. J. (2006). Callous-unemotional traits in a community sample of adolescents. Assessment, 13(4), 454469. doi: 10.1177/1073191106287354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fairchild, G., Hawes, D. J., Frick, P. J., Copeland, W. E., Odgers, C. L., Franke, B., … De Brito, S. A. (2019). Conduct disorder. Nature Reviews Disease Primers, 5(1), 43. doi: 10.1038/s41572-019-0095-y.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fairchild, G., Toschi, N., Sully, K., Sonuga-Barke, E. J., Hagan, C. C., Diciotti, S., … Passamonti, L. (2016). Mapping the structural organization of the brain in conduct disorder: Replication of findings in two independent samples. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57(9), 10181026. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12581.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Floris, D. L., Wolfers, T., Zabihi, M., Holz, N. E., Zwiers, M. P., Charman, T., … Group, E.-A. L. E. A. P. (2021). Atypical brain asymmetry in autism-A candidate for clinically meaningful stratification. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 6(8), 802812. doi:10.1016/j.bpsc.2020.08.008.Google ScholarPubMed
Fuermaier, A. B. M., Hupen, P., De Vries, S. M., Muller, M., Kok, F. M., Koerts, J., … Tucha, O. (2018). Perception in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. ADHD Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders, 10(1), 2147. doi: 10.1007/s12402-017-0230-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Groves, A. R., Beckmann, C. F., Smith, S. M., & Woolrich, M. W. (2011). Linked independent component analysis for multimodal data fusion. NeuroImage, 54(3), 21982217. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.073.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hariri, A. R., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Mazziotta, J. C. (2000). Modulating emotional responses: Effects of a neocortical network on the limbic system. Neuroreport, 11(1), 4348. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200001170-00009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holz, N. E., Boecker-Schlier, R., Buchmann, A. F., Blomeyer, D., Jennen-Steinmetz, C., Baumeister, S., … Laucht, M. (2017). Ventral striatum and amygdala activity as convergence sites for early adversity and conduct disorder. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(2), 261272. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hummer, T. A., Wang, Y., Kronenberger, W. G., Dunn, D. W., & Mathews, V. P. (2015). The relationship of brain structure to age and executive functioning in adolescent disruptive behavior disorder. Psychiatry Research, 231(3), 210217. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2014.11.009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jack, C. R. Jr., Bernstein, M. A., Borowski, B. J., Gunter, J. L., Fox, N. C., Thompson, P. M, … Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging, I. (2010). Update on the magnetic resonance imaging core of the Alzheimer's disease neuroimaging initiative. Alzheimer's & Dementia: The Journal of the Alzheimer's Association, 6(3), 212220. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2010.03.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, W. E., Li, C., & Rabinovic, A. (2007). Adjusting batch effects in microarray expression data using empirical Bayes methods. Biostatistics (Oxford, England), 8(1), 118127. doi: 10.1093/biostatistics/kxj037.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufman, J., Birmaher, B., Brent, D., Rao, U., Flynn, C., Moreci, P., … Ryan, N. (1997). Schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children-present and lifetime version (K-SADS-PL): Initial reliability and validity data. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(7), 980988. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199707000-00021.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kohls, G., Baumann, S., Gundlach, M., Scharke, W., Bernhard, A., Martinelli, A., … Konrad, K. (2020). Investigating sex differences in emotion recognition, learning, and regulation among youths with conduct disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 59(2), 263273. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2019.04.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Llera, A., Wolfers, T., Mulders, P., & Beckmann, C. F. (2019). Inter-individual differences in human brain structure and morphology link to variation in demographics and behavior. Elife, 8, e44443. doi:10.7554/eLife.44443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lozier, L. M., Cardinale, E. M., VanMeter, J. W., & Marsh, A. A. (2014). Mediation of the relationship between callous-unemotional traits and proactive aggression by amygdala response to fear among children with conduct problems. JAMA Psychiatry, 71(6), 627636. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.4540.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lu, F. M., Zhou, J. S., Zhang, J., Wang, X. P., & Yuan, Z. (2017). Disrupted small-world brain network topology in pure conduct disorder. Oncotarget, 8(39), 6550665524. doi: 10.18632/oncotarget.19098.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marquand, A. F., Kia, S. M., Zabihi, M., Wolfers, T., Buitelaar, J. K., & Beckmann, C. F. (2019). Conceptualizing mental disorders as deviations from normative functioning. Molecular Psychiatry, 24(10), 14151424. doi: 10.1038/s41380-019-0441-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marquand, A. F., Rezek, I., Buitelaar, J., & Beckmann, C. F. (2016). Understanding heterogeneity in clinical cohorts using normative models: Beyond case–control studies. Biological Psychiatry, 80(7), 552561. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.12.023.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noordermeer, S. D., Luman, M., & Oosterlaan, J. (2016). A systematic review and meta-analysis of neuroimaging in oppositional defiant disorder (ODD) and conduct disorder (CD) taking Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) into account. Neuropsychology Review, 26(1), 4472. doi: 10.1007/s11065-015-9315-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pardini, D. A., Frick, P. J., & Moffitt, T. E. (2010). Building an evidence base for DSM-5 conceptualizations of oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder: Introduction to the special section. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119(4), 683688. doi: 10.1037/a0021441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parkes, L., Moore, T. M., Calkins, M. E., Cook, P. A., Cieslak, M., Roalf, D. R., … Bassett, D. S. (2021). Transdiagnostic dimensions of psychopathology explain individuals’ unique deviations from normative neurodevelopment in brain structure. Translational Psychiatry, 11(1), 232. doi: 10.1038/s41398-021-01342-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Passamonti, L., Fairchild, G., Goodyer, I. M., Hurford, G., Hagan, C. C., Rowe, J. B., & Calder, A. J. (2010). Neural abnormalities in early-onset and adolescence-onset conduct disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67(7), 729738. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, A. C., Crockett, L., Richards, M., & Boxer, A. (1988). A self-report measure of pubertal status: Reliability, validity, and initial norms. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 17(2), 117133. doi: 10.1007/BF01537962.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Polanczyk, G. V., Salum, G. A., Sugaya, L. S., Caye, A., & Rohde, L. A. (2015). Annual research review: A meta-analysis of the worldwide prevalence of mental disorders in children and adolescents. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56(3), 345365. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12381.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raine, A. (2018). Antisocial personality as a neurodevelopmental disorder. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 14, 259289. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050817-084819.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raine, A., Dodge, K., Loeber, R., Gatzke-Kopp, L., Lynam, D., Reynolds, C., … Liu, J. (2006). The reactive-proactive aggression questionnaire: Differential correlates of reactive and proactive aggression in adolescent boys. Aggressive Behavior, 32(2), 159171. doi: 10.1002/ab.20115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, J. C., & De Brito, S. A. (2016). Cortical and subcortical gray matter volume in youths with conduct problems: A meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry, 73(1), 6472. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.2423.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosa-Justicia, M., Saam, M. C., Flamarique, I., Borras, R., Naaijen, J., Dietrich, A., … Castro-Fornieles, J. (2022). Subgrouping children and adolescents with disruptive behaviors: Symptom profiles and the role of callous-unemotional traits. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 31(1), 5166. doi: 10.1007/s00787-020-01662-w.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Viding, E., & McCrory, E. (2020). Disruptive behavior disorders: The challenge of delineating mechanisms in the face of heterogeneity. American Journal of Psychiatry, 177(9), 811817. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2020.20070998.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (2011). Wechsler intelligence scale for children (4th ed.). Frankfurt: Pearson Assessment.Google Scholar
White, S. F., Pope, K., Sinclair, S., Fowler, K. A., Brislin, S. J., Williams, W. C., … Blair, R. J. (2013). Disrupted expected value and prediction error signaling in youths with disruptive behavior disorders during a passive avoidance task. American Journal of Psychiatry, 170(3), 315323. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12060840.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winkler, A. M., Webster, M. A., Vidaurre, D., Nichols, T. E., & Smith, S. M. (2015). Multi-level block permutation. NeuroImage, 123, 253268. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.05.092.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolfers, T., Arenas, A. L., Onnink, A. M. H., Dammers, J., Hoogman, M., Zwiers, M. P., … Beckmann, C. F. (2017). Refinement by integration: Aggregated effects of multimodal imaging markers on adult ADHD. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience, 42(6), 386394. doi: 10.1503/jpn.160240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolfers, T., Doan, N. T., Kaufmann, T., Alnaes, D., Moberget, T., Agartz, I., … Marquand, A. F. (2018). Mapping the heterogeneous phenotype of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder using normative models. JAMA Psychiatry, 75(11), 11461155. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.2467.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zabihi, M., Oldehinkel, M., Wolfers, T., Frouin, V., Goyard, D., Loth, E., … Marquand, A. F. (2019). Dissecting the heterogeneous cortical anatomy of autism spectrum disorder using normative models. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 4(6), 567578. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2018.11.013.Google ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Methodological overview. (a). A normative model was estimated from brain structure and function across typically developing controls (TDs) and DBD cases, which allowed us to estimate normative probability maps, showing the regional deviations from the expected pattern in each subject. (b) These deviations were integrated using linked ICA, which yielded 20 components that were related to behavioral aggression measures.

Figure 1

Table 1. Sample characteristics

Figure 2

Fig. 2. (a) Spatial representation of the voxel-wise normative model for all modalities in males. The panels show the beta values depicting the change across 8 to 18 years of age for negative face processing, positive/neutral face processing, shape processing and anatomical images. Red and blue indicate an increase or decline, respectively, in activity or relative expansion or contraction to match the template for the Jacobian determinants. (b) Mean positive deviations from the model for controls and cases. Cases showed significantly more positive deviations, i.e. a higher age-related increase in prefrontal and limbic (i.e. cluster comprising the amygdala) activity presumably during negative and positive/neutral face matching. For visualization purposes only the left side is shown.

Figure 3

Fig. 3. Cases showed increased deviations, i.e. higher age-related activity, from the normative model during negative face processing in the amygdala, the parahippocampal gyrus, the (inferior) temporal gyrus, the fusiform gyrus and the cuneus when compared to controls. This is suggestive of delayed maturational trajectories of brain activation in DBD cases.

Figure 4

Fig. 4. Linked ICA. (a). Loading matrix showing the contribution of each modality to each of the 20 components (blue: negative faces, orange: positive/neutral faces, green: shapes, red: anatomy). The underlying brain pattern of component 8 (in the matrix number 8) per functional modality is depicted on the right side (thresholded at Z > 2). (b) Component 8 was negatively associated with the aggression factor, which was still significant when outliers were excluded (p = 0.003).

Supplementary material: File

Holz et al. supplementary material

Holz et al. supplementary material

Download Holz et al. supplementary material(File)
File 1.1 MB