Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-k7p5g Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-11T10:44:48.386Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Beyond Inference in Perception

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 February 2022

Stephen P. Stich*
Affiliation:
University of Maryland

Extract

The controversy over inference in perception turns on the nature of the processes that intervene between the stimulus.and the perceptual experience or percept. For those who find it useful to conceptualize such matters in boxological shorthand, the dispute can be viewed as focusing on the contents of the box in Figure 1. (Figures will be found at the end of the paper.)

Gibsonians have stressed the informational richness of the stimulus and have argued that in virtue of that informational richness no complex inferential processes must be postulated in the box. Rather, the perceptual mechanism can be thought of as “picking up” or “resonating to” the information in the stimulus. But, of course, the suggestion that the perceptual system can pick up information “directly” from the stimulus does not really speak to the issue of what is in the box.

Type
Part XI. Inference in Perception
Copyright
Copyright © 1983 Philosophy of Science Association

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Fodor, J. (1983). The Modularity of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marr, D. & Poggio, T. (1979). “A Computational Theory of Human Stereo Vision.” Proceedings of the Royal Society - London B 204: 301-328.Google Scholar
Mynatt, C.; Doherty, M.; and Tweney, R. (1977). “Confirmation Bias in a Simulated Research Environment: An Experimental Study of Scientific Inference.” Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 29: 85-95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mynatt, C.; Doherty, M.; and Tweney, R. (1978). “Consequences of Confirmation and Disconfirmation in a Simulated Research Environment.” Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 30: 395-406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nisbett, R. & Ross, L., (1980). Human Inference: Strategies and Shortcomings of Social Judgement. Englewood-Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Rock, I. (1983). “Inference in Perception.” In PSA 1982. Volume 2. Edited by Asquith, P.D. and Nickles, T.. East Lansing, MI: Philosophy of Science Association. Pages 525-540.Google Scholar
Ross, L.; Lepper, M.; and Hubbard, M. (1975). “Perseverance in Self Perception and Social Perception: Biased Attributional Processes in the Debriefing Paradigm.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 32: 880-892.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stich, S. (1978). “Beliefs and Sub-Doxastic States.” Philosophy of Science 45: 499-518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ullman, S. (1979). The Interpretation of Visual Motion. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ullman, S. (1980). “Against Direct Perception.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3: 373-381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wason, P. & Johnson-Laird, P. (1972). The Psychology of Reasoning: Structure and Content. London: B.T. Batsford.Google Scholar