Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-rkxrd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-22T19:57:54.808Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Exploring the Effect of Combinational Pictorial Stimuli on Creative Design Performance

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 July 2019

Min Hua*
Affiliation:
Imperial College London;
Ji Han
Affiliation:
University of Liverpool;
Xuezi Ma
Affiliation:
University of Cambridge
Peter Childs
Affiliation:
Imperial College London;
*
Contact: HUA, MIN, Imperial College London, Dyson school of design engineering, United Kingdom, m.hua15@imperial.ac.uk

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

Visual stimuli can be useful in supporting design ideation process. However, researchers still know very little about how stimuli should be delivered to designers during the early design stage. This question is crucial to the effective use of stimuli because previous researches have proved that ill-presented stimuli can have a negative impact on design creativity. Therefore, an empirical study was conducted with the aim of exploring if and how combinational pictorial stimuli can affect designers' creative performance. Results from a total of 36 participants show that the design outcomes presented by the group exposed to combinational pictorial stimuli were more creative than those given by the group exposed to no stimuli or randomly presented pictorial stimuli. These results imply that the form of stimuli delivery can affect creative design outcomes and combinational pictorial stimuli best support design creativity among these three conditions. These findings give us a better understanding of the roles that visual stimuli play in design, which is expected to bring us important implications for both design education and design support tool development

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is unaltered and is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained for commercial re-use or in order to create a derivative work.
Copyright
© The Author(s) 2019

References

Agogué, M., Kazakçi, A., Weil, B. and Cassotti, M. (2011), “The impact of examples on creative design: explaining fixation and stimulation effects”, in DS 68-2: Proceedings of the 18th International Conference on Engineering Design (ICED 11), Impacting Society through Engineering Design, Vol. 2: Design Theory and Research Methodology, Lyngby/Copenhagen, Denmark, 15.-19.08. 2011.Google Scholar
Bacciotti, D., Borgianni, Y. and Rotini, F. (2016), “An original design approach for stimulating the ideation of new product features”, Computers in industry. Elsevier, Vol. 75, pp. 80100.Google Scholar
Borgianni, Y., Rotini, F. and Tomassini, M. (2017), “Fostering ideation in the very early design phases: How textual, pictorial and combined stimuli affect creativity”, in DS 87-8 Proceedings of the 21st International Conference on Engineering Design (ICED 17) Vol 8: Human Behaviour in Design, Vancouver, Canada, 21-25.08. 2017.Google Scholar
Cheng, P.-J. (2016), “Development of a mobile app for generating creative ideas based on exploring designers’ on-line resource searching and retrieval behavior”, Design Studies. Elsevier, Vol. 44, pp. 7499.Google Scholar
Childs, P. R. N., Hamilton, T., Morris, R. D. and Johnston, G. (2006), “Centre for technology enabled creativity”, in DS 38: Proceedings of E&DPE 2006, the 8th International Conference on Engineering and Product Design Education, Salzburg, Austria, 07.-08.09. 2006, pp. 367372.Google Scholar
Dahl, D.W. and Moreau, P. (2002), “The influence and value of analogical thinking during new product ideation”, Journal of Marketing Research. American Marketing Association, Vol. 39 No. 1, pp. 4760.Google Scholar
Diedrich, J., Benedek, M., Jauk, E. and Neubauer, A.C. (2015), “‘Are creative ideas novel and useful?”, Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. Educational Publishing Foundation, Vol. 9 No. 1, p. 35.Google Scholar
Douglas, L.D., Jillian, M.H., Thomas, L.R. and Eric, L.S. (2006), “Identifying quality, novel, and creative ideas: constructs and scales for idea evaluation1”, Journal of the Association for Information Systems, Vol. 7 No. 10, p. 646.Google Scholar
Finke, R.A. (1990), Creative imagery: Discoveries and inventions in visualization, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
Finke, R.A. and Slayton, K. (1988), “Explorations of creative visual synthesis in mental imagery”, Memory & Cognition. Springer, Vol. 16 No. 3, pp. 252257.Google Scholar
Gassmann, O. and Zeschky, M. (2008), “Opening up the solution space: the role of analogical thinking for breakthrough product innovation”, Creativity and Innovation Management. Wiley Online Library, Vol. 17 No. 2, pp. 97106.Google Scholar
Goldschmidt, G. and Sever, A.L. (2011), “Inspiring design ideas with texts”, Design Studies. Elsevier, Vol. 32 No. 2, pp. 139155.Google Scholar
Goldschmidt, G. and Smolkov, M. (2006), “Variances in the impact of visual stimuli on design problem solving performance”, Design Studies. Elsevier, Vol. 27 No. 5, pp. 549569.Google Scholar
Gonçalves, M., Cardoso, C. and Badke-Schaub, P. (2014), “What inspires designers? Preferences on inspirational approaches during idea generation”, Design studies. Elsevier, Vol. 35 No. 1, pp. 2953.Google Scholar
Guo, J. and McLeod, P.L. (2014), “The Impact of Semantic Relevance and Heterogeneity of Pictorial Stimuli on Individual Brainstorming: An Extension of the SIAM Model”, Creativity Research Journal. Taylor & Francis, Vol. 26 No. 3, pp. 361367.Google Scholar
Han, J., Shi, F., Chen, L. and Childs, P.R.N. (2018), “The Combinator–a computer-based tool for creative idea generation based on a simulation approach”, Design Science. Cambridge University Press, Vol. 4, https://doi.org/10.1017/dsj.2018.7.Google Scholar
Han, J., Shi, F. and Childs, P. R. N. (2016), “The Combinator: A computer-based tool for idea generation”, in Proceedings of the Design 2016 14th International Design Conference, pp. 639648.Google Scholar
Hanington, B. (2003), “Methods in the making: A perspective on the state of human research in design”, Design issues. MIT Press, Vol. 19 No. 4, pp. 918.Google Scholar
Henderson, K. (1998), On line and on paper: Visual representations, visual culture, and computer graphics in design engineering, MIT press.Google Scholar
Lutz, K.A. and Lutz, R.J. (1977), “Effects of interactive imagery on learning: Application to advertising”, Journal of Applied Psychology. American Psychological Association, Vol. 62 No. 4, p. 493.Google Scholar
Malaga, R.A. (2000), “The effect of stimulus modes and associative distance in individual creativity support systems”, Decision Support Systems. Elsevier, Vol. 29 No. 2, pp. 125141.Google Scholar
Mednick, S. (1962), “The associative basis of the creative process”, Psychological review. American Psychological Association, Vol. 69 No. 3, p. 220.Google Scholar
Mougenot, C., Aucouturier, J.-J., Yamanaka, T. and Watanabe, K. (2010), “Comparing the effects of auditory stimuli and visual stimuli in design creativity”, in Proceedings of The Third International Workshop on Kansei.Google Scholar
Mougenot, C., Bouchard, C., Aoussat, A. and Westerman, S. (2008), “Inspiration, images and design: an investigation of designers’ information gathering strategies”, Inderscience Publishers.Google Scholar
Muller, W. (1989), “Design discipline and the significance of visuo-spatial thinking”, Design Studies. Citeseer, Vol. 10 No. 1, pp. 1223.Google Scholar
Paivio, A., Rogers, T.B. and Smythe, P.C. (1968), “Why are pictures easier to recall than words?”, Psychonomic Science. Springer, Vol. 11 No. 4, pp. 137138.Google Scholar
Perttula, M. and Sipilä, P. (2007), “The idea exposure paradigm in design idea generation”, Journal of Engineering Design. Taylor & Francis, Vol. 18 No. 1, pp. 93102.Google Scholar
Plucker, J.A. and Makel, M.C. (2010), “Assessment of creativity”, The Cambridge handbook of creativity, pp. 4873.Google Scholar
Sarkar, P. and Chakrabarti, A. (2008), “The effect of representation of triggers on design outcomes”, Ai Edam. Cambridge University Press, Vol. 22 No. 2, pp. 101116.Google Scholar
Shah, J.J., Smith, S.M. and Vargas-Hernandez, N. (2003), “Metrics for measuring ideation effectiveness”, Design studies. Elsevier, Vol. 24 No. 2, pp. 111134.Google Scholar
Siangliulue, P., Chan, J., Gajos, K. Z. and Dow, S. P. (2015), “Providing timely examples improves the quantity and quality of generated ideas”, in Proceedings of the 2015 ACM SIGCHI Conference on Creativity and Cognition. ACM, pp. 8392.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R.J. and Sternberg, R.J. (1999), Handbook of creativity, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ward, T. B. and Kolomyts, Y. (2010) “Cognition and creativity”, The Cambridge handbook of creativity. Cambridge University Press, New York, NY, pp. 93112.Google Scholar
Ware, C. (2010), Visual thinking: For design, Elsevier.Google Scholar
Yang, M.C., Wood, W.H. and Cutkosky, M.R. (2005), “Design information retrieval: a thesauri-based approach for reuse of informal design information”, Engineering with computers. Springer, Vol. 21 No. 2, pp. 177192.Google Scholar