Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-r5zm4 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-27T08:32:03.823Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Born to Rebel and Its Critics

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 May 2016

Frank J. Sulloway*
Affiliation:
University of California, Berkeley, USA
Get access

Abstract

Frederic Townsend's critique of Born to Rebel (Sulloway, 1996) is based on an unfortunate pattern of misrepresentation and faulty empirical methods. First, the historical data in Born to Rebel are neither unrepresentative nor undisclosed. They were compiled with the help of 110 expert raters, who validated their representative nature and operationalized the principal outcome measures. Second, birth order is moderately associated with political radicalism, especially in real-life and within-family studies. Third, the meta-analytic data are statistically compelling for various behavioral attributes, although effect sizes are generally modest. Fourth, recent studies using large samples and anchored scales demonstrate the influence of birth order on a wide variety of personality traits, especially in within-family comparisons. In addition, the relationship between birth order and openness to radical innovation—highlighted in Born to Rebel—has been replicated by other researchers. Because Townsend fails to employ formal methods of hypothesis testing—relying instead on selected anecdotal examples, adversarial tactics, and an inadequate grasp of statistical principles—he has drawn numerous false conclusions about the influence of birth order.

Type
Roundtable Response
Copyright
Copyright © Association for Politics and the Life Sciences 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Altus, W.D. (1963). “The First-Born as a Conservative: Adler Revisited.” American Psychologist 18:356.Google Scholar
Beer, J.M. and Horn, J.M. (2000). “The Influence of Rearing Order on Personality Development within Two Adoption Cohorts.” Journal of Personality 68:789819.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brush, S.G. (1999). “Why Was Relativity Accepted?” Physics in Perspective 1:184214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cervone, D. and Shoda, Y. (1999). “Beyond Traits in the Study of Personality Coherence.” Current Directions in Psychological Science 8:2732.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chao, M. (2001). “The Birth-Order Controversy: Within-Family Effects and Their Generalizability.” Honors Thesis, University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Costa, P.T. Jr. and McCrae, R.R. (1992). NEO PI-R Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Costa, P.T. Jr., Terracciano, A., and McCrae, R.R. (2001). “Gender Differences in Personality Traits across Cultures: Robust and Surprising Findings.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81:322–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daly, M. and Salmon, C.A. (1999). “Birth Order Has Real Effects that Indignant Denials Cannot Banish.” Unpublished commentary on an earlier version of Frederic Townsend's (1998) target article.Google Scholar
Desmond, A. (1989). The Politics of Evolution: Morphology, Medicine, and Reform in Radical London. Chicago and London: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Ernst, C. and Angst, J. (1983). Birth Order: Its Influence on Personality. Berlin and New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farley, F. and Farley, S.V. (1974). “Birth Order and Political Orientation in College Women.” Psychological Reports 34:1045–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feingold, A. (1994). “Gender Differences in Personality: A Meta-Analysis.” Psychological Bulletin 116:429–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freese, J., Powell, B., and Steelman, L.C. (1999). “Rebel without a Cause or Effect: Birth Order and Social Attitudes.” American Sociological Review 64:207–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gillespie, N.C. (1979). Charles Darwin and the Problem of Creation. Chicago and London: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Glick, T., ed. (1972). The Comparative Reception of Darwinism. Austin and London: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
Hall, E.E. Jr. and Barger, B. (1964). “Attitudinal Structures in Older and Younger Siblings.” Journal of Individual Psychology 20:5968.Google Scholar
Harris, J.R. (1998). The Nurture Assumption: Why Children Turn Out the Way They Do. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Harris, J.R. (2000). “Context-Specific Learning, Personality, and Birth Order.” Current Directions in Psychological Research 9:174–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, J.R. (2001). “The 179 ‘Studies’ (Actually, Findings) in Ernst & Angst's (1983) Text and Tables.” Unpublished manuscript (http://home.att.net/~xchar/tna/birth-order/index.htm#findings).Google Scholar
Hertwig, R., Davis, J.N., and Sulloway, F.J. (2002). “Parental Investment: How an Equity Motive Can Produce Inequality.” Psychological Bulletin 128:728–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Higonnet, P. (1985). “The Social and Cultural Antecedents of Revolutionary Discontinuity: Montagnards and Girondins.” English Historical Review 100:513–44.Google Scholar
Hull, D.L., Tessner, P.D., and Diamond, A.M. (1978). “Planck's Principle.” Science 202:717–23.Google Scholar
Hunter, J.E. and Schmidt, F.L. (1990). Methods of Meta-Analysis: Correcting Error and Bias in Research Findings. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Jefferson, T., Herbst, J.H., and McCrae, R.R. (1998). “Associations between Birth Order and Personality Traits: Evidence from Self-Reports and Observer Ratings.” Journal of Research in Personality 32:498509.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leventhal, G.S. (1970). “Influence of Brothers and Sisters on Sex-Role Behavior.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 16:452–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loehlin, J. (1992). Genes and Environment in Personality Development. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Loomis, S. (1964). Paris in the Terror: June 1793-July 1794. Philadelphia and New York: J. B. Lippincott.Google Scholar
Macbeth, B.L. (1975). “Birth Order, Personality, and Scholastic Aptitude.” Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Department of Psychology, University of Oregon. Dissertation Abstracts International 36-B (1976):4757.Google Scholar
MacDonald, A. (1969). “Birth Order and Religious Affiliation.” Developmental Psychology 1:628.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marjoribanks, K. (1979). “Social Attitudes: Social Status and Sibling Constellation Correlates.” Psychological Reports 45:9951000.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McAdams, D.P. (1992). “The Five-Factor Model in Personality: A Critical Approach.” Journal of Personality 60:329–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCrae, R.R. (1994). “Openness to Experience: Expanding the Boundaries of Factor V.” European Journal of Personality 8:251–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCrae, R.R. and John, O.P. (1992). “An Introduction to the Five-Factor Model and Its Applications.” Journal of Personality 60:175215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Michalski, R.L. and Shackelford, T.K. (2002). “An Attempted Replication of the Relationships between Birth Order and Personality.” Journal of Research in Personality 36:182–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mischel, W. and Shoda, Y. (1995). “A Cognitive-Affective System Theory of Personality: Reconceptualizing Situations, Dispositions, Dynamics, and Invariance in Personality Structure.” Psychological Review 102:246–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Modell, J. (1997). “Family Niche and Intellectual Bent.” Review of Born to Rebel, by F.J. Sulloway. Science 275:624.Google Scholar
Mysterud, I., Drevon, T., and Slagsvold, T. (2001). “An Evolutionary Interpretation of Gift-Giving Behavior in Modern Norwegian Society.” Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Numbers, R.L. (1998). Darwinism Comes to America. Cambridge and London: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Ojha, H. and Sah, B. (1990). “Personality and Socio-Familial Correlates of Conservatism in Indian Youth.” International Journal of Psychology 25:295304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parker, W.D. (1998). “Birth Order Effects in the Academically Talented.” Gifted Child Quarterly 42:2938.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paulhus, D.L., Trapnell, P.D., and Chen, D. (1999). “Birth Order Effects on Personality and Achievement within Families.” Psychological Science 10:482–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Price, J. (1969). “Personality Differences within Families: Comparisons of Adult Brothers and Sisters.” Journal of Biosocial Science 1:117205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rejai, M. and Phillips, K. (1979). Leaders of Revolution. Beverly Hills and London: Sage.Google Scholar
Rejai, M. and Phillips, K. (1983). World Revolutionary Leaders. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.Google Scholar
Rejai, M. and Phillips, K. (1988). Loyalists and Revolutionaries: Political Leaders Compared. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.Google Scholar
Rodgers, J.L., Cleveland, H.H., van den Oord, E., and Rowe, D.C. (2000). “Resolving the Debate over Birth Order, Family Size, and Intelligence.” American Psychologist 55:599612.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rohde, P.A., Andres, A.S., Atzwanger, K., Butovskaya, M., Lampert, A., Mysterud, I., and Sulloway, F.J.(in press). “Perceived Parental Favoritism, Closeness to Kin, and the Rebel of the Family: The Effects of Birth Order and Sex.” Evolution and Human Behavior.Google Scholar
Rosenthal, R. (1987). Judgment Studies: Design, Analysis, and Meta-Analysls. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rudwick, M.J.S. (1985). The Meaning of Fossils: Episodes in the History of Paleontology. 2nd ed.Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruse, M. (1979). The Darwinian Revolution: Science Red in Tooth and Claw. Chicago and London: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Ruse, M. (1997). Review of Born to Rebel, by Sulloway, F. J.Evolution and Human Behavior 18:369–73.Google Scholar
Ruse, M. (1999). Ratings on Scientific Stance for Prominent Participants in the Darwinian Revolution, Drawn from Ruse (1979). Personal communication.Google Scholar
Salmon, C.A. (1998). “The Evocative Nature of Kin Terminology in Political Rhetoric.” Politics and the Life Sciences 17:5157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salmon, C.A. and Daly, M. (1998). “Birth Order and Familial Sentiment: Middleborns Are Different.” Evolution and Human Behavior 19:299312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shermer, M. (1999). How We Believe: The Search for God in an Age of Science. New York: W.H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Simonton, D.K. (1997). “Evolution, Personality, and History.” Review of Born to Rebel, by F.J. Sulloway. American Journal of Psychology 110:457–60.Google Scholar
Somit, A., Arwin, A., and Petersen, S. (1996). Birth Order and Political Behavior. Lanham, MD: University Press of America.Google Scholar
Sulloway, F.J. (1991). “Modeling Responses to Scientific Innovation.” National Science Foundation Grant Proposal.Google Scholar
Sulloway, F.J. (1995). “Birth Order and Evolutionary Psychology: A Meta-Analytic Overview.” Psychological Inquiry 6:7580.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sulloway, F.J. (1996). Born to Rebel: Birth Order, Family Dynamics, and Creative Lives. New York: Pantheon.Google Scholar
Sulloway, F.J. (1997). Born to Rebel: Birth Order, Family Dynamics, and Creative Lives. Corrected edition. New York: Vintage.Google Scholar
Sulloway, F.J. (1999). “Birth Order.” In Runco, M.A. and Pritzker, S. (eds.), Encyclopedia of Creativity 1:189202. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Sulloway, F.J. (2001a). “Birth Order, Sibling Competition, and Human Behavior.” In Holcomb, H.R. III (ed.), Conceptual Challenges in Evolutionary Psychology: Innovative Research Strategies. Dordrecht and Boston: Kluwer Academic Publishers.Google Scholar
Sulloway, F.J. (2001b). “Sibling-Order Effects.” International Encyclopedia of the Social and Behavioral Sciences 21:14058–63. Cambridge: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sulloway, F.J. (2002a). “Biographical Data on Political Activists.” Manuscript (http://www.sulloway.org).Google Scholar
Sulloway, F.J. (2002b). “Technical Report on a Vote-Counting Meta-Analysis of the Birth-Order Literature (1940–1999).” Manuscript (http://www.sulloway.org).Google Scholar
Sutton-Smith, B. and Rosenberg, B.G. (1970). The Sibling. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Townsend, F. (1997). “Rebelling against Born to Rebel.” Journal of Social and Evolutionary Systems 20:191204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Townsend, F. (1998). “Birth Order and Rebellion: Reconstructing the Research in Born to Rebel.” Unpublished manuscript (the original version of Townsend's [2000] target article, withdrawn from publication).Google Scholar
Townsend, F. (2000). “Birth Order and Rebelliousness: Reconstructing the Research in Born to Rebel.” Politics and the Life Sciences 19: [this issue].CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turkheimer, E. and Waldron, M. (2000). “Nonshared Environment: A Theoretical, Methodological, and Quantitative Review.” Psychological Bulletin 126:78108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watts, W.A. and Whittaker, D. (1966). “Free Speech Advocates at Berkeley.” Journal of Applied Behavioral Science 2:4162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weber, P. (1984). “The Birth Order Oddity in Supreme Court Appointments.” Presidential Studies Quarterly 14:561–68.Google Scholar
Zajonc, R.B. (2001). “The Family Dynamics of Intellectual Development.” American Psychologist 56:490–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zweigenhaft, R.L. and Von Ammon, J. (2000). “Birth Order and Civil Disobedience: A Test of Sulloway's ‘Born to Rebel’ Hypothesis.” Journal of Social Psychology 140:624–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar