Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-75dct Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-01T15:26:34.982Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Prosthetic Models

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2022

Abstract

What are the relative epistemic merits of building prosthetic models versus building nonprosthetic models and simulations? I argue that prosthetic models provide a sufficient test of affordance validity, that is, of whether the target system affords mechanisms that can be commandeered by a prosthesis. In other respects, prosthetic models are epistemically on par with nonprosthetic models. I focus on prosthetics in neuroscience, but the results are general. The goal of understanding how brain mechanisms work under ecologically and physiologically relevant conditions is narrow compared to the search for maker's knowledge about how the brain can be made to work for us.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Philosophy of Science Association

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

Thanks to Anna Alexandrova, Masviita Chirimuuta, Dennis Des Chene, David Kaplan, Dan Moran, Wendy Parker, Susan Sterrett, and the members of the Washington University Philosophy of Science Co-op for comments and discussion.

References

Bacon, F. 1620. Novum Organum. London.Google Scholar
Benzania, F. 1997–2003. Nerve (version 2), http://nerve.bsd.uchicago.edu/nerve1.html.Google Scholar
Berger, T. W., et al. 2005. “Restoring Lost Cognitive Function.” IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Magazine 24 (September/October): 3044.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bogen, J. 2005. “Regularities and Causality: Generalizations and Causal Explanations.” Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences 36:397420.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craver, C. F. 2001. “Role Functions, Mechanisms and Hierarchy.” Philosophy of Science 68:3155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craver, C. F.. 2006. “When Mechanistic Models Explain.” Synthese 153:355–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craver, C. F.. 2007. Explaining the Brain: Mechanisms and the Mosaic Unity of Neuroscience. Oxford: Clarendon.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craver, C. F.. 2008. “Physical Law and Mechanistic Explanation in the Hodgkin and Huxley Model of the Action Potential.” Philosophy of Science 75:1022–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cummins, R. 1975. “Functional Analysis.” Journal of Philosophy 72:741–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dayan, P., and Abbott, L. F.. 2001. Theoretical Neuroscience: Computational and Mathematical Modeling of Neural Systems. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Destexhe, A., and Bal, T., eds. 2009. Dynamic-Clamp: From Principles to Applications. Series in Computational Neuroscience. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Felton, E. A., Wilson, A., Williams, J. C., and Charles, G. P.. 2007. “Electrocorticographically Controlled Brain-Computer Interfaces Using Motor and Sensory Imagery in Patients with Temporary Subdural Electrode Implants: Report of Four Cases.” Journal of Neurosurgery 106:495500.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guala, F. 2002. “Models, Simulations, and Experiments.” In Model-Based Reasoning: Science, Technology, Values, ed. Magnani, L. and Nersessian, N. J., 5974. New York: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartman, S. 1996. “The World as a Process: Simulations in the Natural and Social Sciences.” In Modeling and Simulation in the Social Sciences from the Philosophy of Science Point of View, ed. Hegselmann, R., Müller, U., and Troitzsch, K., 77100. Theory and Decision Library. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Hille, B. 1992. Ion Channels of Excitable Membranes. 2nd ed. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.Google Scholar
Hodgkin, A. L., and Huxley, A. F.. 1952. “A Quantitative Description of Membrane Current and Its Application to Conduction and Excitation in Nerve.” Journal of Physiology 117:500544.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Korb, K. B., and Mascaro, S.. 2009. “The Philosophy of Computer Simulation.” In Logic, Methodology, and Philosophy of Science: Proceedings of the Thirteenth International Congress, ed. Glymour, C., Wang, W., and Westerstahl, D.. London: College Publications.Google Scholar
Lebedev, M. A., Carmena, J. M., O'Doherty, J. E., Zacksenhouse, M., Henriquez, C. S., Principe, J. C., and Nicolelis, M. A. L.. 2005. “Cortical Ensemble Adaptation to Represent Actuators Controlled by a Brain Machine Interface.” Journal of Neuroscience 25:4681–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lebedev, M. A., and Nicolelis, M. A. L.. 2006. “Brain Machine Interfaces: Past, Present and Future.” Trends in Neurosciences 29:536–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marr, D. 1981. “Artificial Intelligence: A Personal View.” In Mind Design, ed. Haugeland, J., 129–42. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Mauk, M. 2000. “The Potential Effectiveness of Simulations versus Phenomenological Models.” Nature Neuroscience 3:649–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nicolelis, M. A. L. 2003. “Brain-Machine Interfaces to Restore Motor Function and Probe Neural Circuits.” Nature Reviews Neuroscience 4:417–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parker, W. 2009. “Does Matter Really Matter? Computer Simulations, Experiments, and Materiality.” Synthese 169 (3): 483–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pérez-Ramos, A. 1988. Francis Bacon's Idea of Science and the Maker's Knowledge Tradition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Prinz, A. A., Abbott, L., and Marder, E.. 2004. “The Dynamic Clamp Comes of Age.” Trends in Neurosciences 27:218–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, A. B. 2004. “Cortical Neural Prostheses.” Annual Review of Neuroscience 27:487507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, A. B., Cui, X. T., Weber, D. J., and Moran, D. W.. 2006. “Brain-Controlled Interfaces: Movement Restoration with Neural Prosthetics.” Neuron 52:205–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Touretzky, D. S., Albert, M. V., Daw, N. D., Ladsariya, A., and Bonakdarpour, M.. 2008. HHsim: Graphical Hodgkin-Huxley Simulator, http://www.cs.cmu.edu/~dst/HHsim/.Google Scholar
Velliste, M., Perel, S., Spalding, M. C., Whitford, A. S., and Schwartz, A. B.. 2008. “Cortical Control of a Prosthetic Arm for Self-Feeding.” Nature 453 (June 19): 10981101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weisberg, M. 2007. “Who Is a Modeler?British Journal for Philosophy of Science 58:207–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar