Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-vsgnj Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-20T00:45:20.895Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Contribution of the Roman rat lines/strains to personality neuroscience: neurobehavioral modeling of internalizing/externalizing psychopathologies

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 October 2023

Alberto Fernández-Teruel*
Affiliation:
Medical Psychology Unit, Department of Psychiatry & Forensic Medicine, Faculty of Medicine & Institute of Neurosciences, Autonomous University of Barcelona, 08193 Bellaterra, Barcelona, Spain
Toni Cañete
Affiliation:
Medical Psychology Unit, Department of Psychiatry & Forensic Medicine, Faculty of Medicine & Institute of Neurosciences, Autonomous University of Barcelona, 08193 Bellaterra, Barcelona, Spain
Daniel Sampedro-Viana
Affiliation:
Medical Psychology Unit, Department of Psychiatry & Forensic Medicine, Faculty of Medicine & Institute of Neurosciences, Autonomous University of Barcelona, 08193 Bellaterra, Barcelona, Spain
Ignasi Oliveras
Affiliation:
Medical Psychology Unit, Department of Psychiatry & Forensic Medicine, Faculty of Medicine & Institute of Neurosciences, Autonomous University of Barcelona, 08193 Bellaterra, Barcelona, Spain
Rafael Torrubia
Affiliation:
Medical Psychology Unit, Department of Psychiatry & Forensic Medicine, Faculty of Medicine & Institute of Neurosciences, Autonomous University of Barcelona, 08193 Bellaterra, Barcelona, Spain
Adolf Tobeña
Affiliation:
Medical Psychology Unit, Department of Psychiatry & Forensic Medicine, Faculty of Medicine & Institute of Neurosciences, Autonomous University of Barcelona, 08193 Bellaterra, Barcelona, Spain
*
Corresponding author: Alberto Fernández-Teruel; Email: albert.fernandez.teruel@uab.cat
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The Roman high-avoidance (RHA) and low-avoidance (RLA) rat lines/strains were established in Rome through bidirectional selection of Wistar rats for rapid (RHA) or extremely poor (RLA) acquisition of a two-way active avoidance task. Relative to RHAs, RLA rats exhibit enhanced threat sensitivity, anxiety, fear and vulnerability to stress, a passive coping style and increased sensitivity to frustration. Thus, RLA rats’ phenotypic profile falls well within the “internalizing” behavior spectrum. Compared with RLAs and other rat strains/stocks, RHAs present increased impulsivity and reward sensitivity, deficits in social behavior and attentional/cognitive processes, novelty-induced hyper-locomotion and vulnerability to psychostimulant sensitization and drug addiction. Thus, RHA rats’ phenotypes are consistent with a “disinhibiting externalizing” profile. Many neurobiological/molecular traits differentiate both rat lines/strains. For example, relative to RLA rats, RHAs exhibit decreased function of the prefrontal cortex (PFC), hippocampus and amygdala, increased functional tone of the mesolimbic dopamine system, a deficit of central metabotropic glutamate-2 (mGlu2) receptors, increased density of serotonin 5-HT2A receptors in the PFC, impairment of GABAergic transmission in the PFC, alterations of several synaptic markers and increased density of pyramidal immature dendrític spines in the PFC. These characteristics suggest an immature brain of RHA rats and are reminiscent of schizophrenia features like hypofrontality and disruption of the excitation/inhibition cortical balance. We review evidence supporting RLA rats as a valid model of anxiety/fear, stress and frustration vulnerability, whereas RHA rats represent a promising translational model of neurodevelopmental alterations related to impulsivity, schizophrenia-relevant features and comorbidity with drug addiction vulnerability.

Type
Review Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© Autonoumous University of Barcelona, 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Once upon a time, some rats left Rome – the many roads leading to it took them everywhere

The Roman high-avoidance (RHA) and low-avoidance (RLA) rat lines were established in Rome through bidirectional selection and outbreeding of Wistar rats for rapid (RHA) or extremely poor (RLA) acquisition of a two-way active avoidance task (TWAA; Figure 1) (Bignami, Reference Bignami1965). The two lines were then moved to Birmingham, UK (e.g., Broadhurst & Bignami, Reference Broadhurst and Bignami1965). Two inbred strains were established in London (in 1974) and, derived from them, two inbred Roman sub-strains were established in Nijmegen (Durcan, Wraight & Fulker, Reference Durcan, Wraight and Fulker1984). Outbred sublines, from the Birmingham stock, were established in Canada (e.g., Satinder, Reference Satinder1971), France (e.g., Delacour, Houcine & Monmaur, Reference Delacour, Houcine and Monmaur1978) and in Zurich, Switzerland, in 1972 (the RHA/Verh and RLA/Verh sublines; e.g., Driscoll & Bättig, Reference Driscoll, Bättig and Lieblich1982). Outbred sublines from the Zürich stock were established in Cagliari, Italy (e.g., Giorgi et al., Reference Giorgi, Orlandi, Escorihuela, Driscoll, Lecca and Corda1994), Bordeaux, France (e.g., Castanon & Mormède, Reference Castanon and Mormède1994), Geneva, Switzerland (Steimer & Driscoll, Reference Steimer and Driscoll2003) and in Germany (e.g., Schwegler et al., Reference Schwegler, Pilz, Koch, Fendt, Linke and Driscoll1997). Inbred RHA and RLA strains, generated through brother/sister mating of the respective Swiss/Verh sublines, were established in Barcelona in 1995 (e.g., Driscoll et al., Reference Driscoll, Escorihuela, Fernández-Teruel, Giorgi, Schwegler, Steimer and Tobeña1998). Over 270 international articles have been published on the Roman rats (or using them as part of broader studies) during the past fifty-five years. They have been used for psychological, neurobiological and genetic research purposes in over 20 different laboratories around the world (see Supplementary Table S1, in Fernández-Teruel et al., 2021), and continue to be very productive, as shown by the over 35 papers published in the past five years.

Figure 1. Two-way active avoidance acquisition in the shuttle box (Medical Psychology Unit, Dept. Psychiatry & Forensic Medicine, Autonomous University of Barcelona). The rat is crossing from the right compartment to the opposite one to escape/avoid the shock but is also faced with the memory of having to escape from the left compartment on the previous trial. For further explanation see text.

In the TWAA task rodents can learn to escape/avoid an aversive stimulus (unconditioned stimulus, US; usually a foot shock) that is preceded by a warning signal (conditioned stimulus, CS; usually a tone, a light, or both) by shuttling between opposite compartments (Figure 1). After a few initial CS–US pairings, the TWAA generates approach-avoidance conflict, with the appearance of context- and CS-conditioned freezing (e.g., Fernández-Teruel & Tobeña, Reference Fernández-Teruel and Tobeña2020). This tendency to freeze (i.e., passive avoidance: Tendency to stay freezing in the present compartment, where they receive the CS) predominates in the initial trials, as the subject learns that the opposite compartment is also threatening (i.e., approach-avoidance conflict), because in that compartment he has also received a foot shock. As training and CS–US associations progress, the subject learns that the US can be overcome by shuttling to the opposite compartment, first by escaping (when the shock US is present) and later on by avoiding it (i.e., by crossing to the opposite compartment when the CS is still on and the US has still not been presented).

The RLA rat line/strain presents high levels of freezing to the CS and the context (during the inter-trial intervals), and very low rate of active avoidance responses and inter-trial crossings. The RHA line/strain displays very low levels of freezing, very rapid active avoidance acquisition and high levels of inter-trial activity (Broadhurst & Bignami, Reference Broadhurst and Bignami1965; Satinder, Reference Satinder1971; Driscoll & Bättig, Reference Driscoll, Bättig and Lieblich1982; Fernández-Teruel et al., Reference Fernández-Teruel, Escorihuela, Tobeña and Driscoll1991; Escorihuela et al., Reference Escorihuela, Tobeña, Driscoll and Fernández-Teruel1995; reviewed by Fernández-Teruel et al., 2021; Giorgi et al., Reference Giorgi, Corda and Fernández-Teruel2019).

Selection for poor two-way active avoidance task acquisition led to behaviorally inhibited, high “neuroticism” and “internalizing” Roman low-avoidance rats

Thus, when facing different types of threatening situations, RLA rats are predominantly high freezers, displaying behavioral inhibition and a passive coping style (opposite to the trait profiles of RHA rats, as we will see below; see also reviews by Driscoll et al., Reference Driscoll, Escorihuela, Fernández-Teruel, Giorgi, Schwegler, Steimer and Tobeña1998; Fernández-Teruel et al., 2021; Steimer & Driscoll, Reference Steimer and Driscoll2003; Giorgi et al., Reference Giorgi, Corda and Fernández-Teruel2019). These trait differences generalize to many between-strain phenotype differences in conditioned and unconditioned tests/tasks reflecting coping style of fear-, anxiety- and depression-related responses. For example, relative to RHA rats, RLAs present increased signs of anxiety in typical anxiety tests such as the Vogel’s punished-drinking, the elevated zero- and plus-maze and light-dark box tests among others; they also show enhanced frustration-related (Amsel, Reference Amsel1992) responding under reward devaluation, and heightened stress-linked endocrine and depressive-like responses (reviewed by Fernández-Teruel et al., 2021; Giorgi et al., Reference Giorgi, Corda and Fernández-Teruel2019). Remarkably, RLAs also exhibit increased context- and cue-conditioned freezing and, like high trait-anxiety humans (Andión et al., Reference Andión, Caseras, Fullana, Fernández-Teruel, Ferrer, Casas and Torrubia2013), RLAs display enhanced stress- and fear(cue)-potentiated startle (Aguilar, Gil, Tobeña, Escorihuela & Fernández-Teruel, Reference Aguilar, Gil, Tobeña, Escorihuela and Fernández-Teruel2000; López-Aumatell et al., Reference López-Aumatell, Blázquez, Gil, Aguilar, Cañete, Giménez-Llort and Fernández-Teruel2009). From the perspective of the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) framework (for Review, see Michelini, Palumbo, DeYoung, Latzman & Kotov, Reference Michelini, Palumbo, DeYoung, Latzman and Kotov2021) such a profile suggests that RLA rats are mainly driven by “negative valence” (e.g., Michelini et al., Reference Michelini, Palumbo, DeYoung, Latzman and Kotov2021) (see section A in Table 1). In fact, they have enhanced sensitivity to threat/distress and behavioral inhibition, which is compatible with a high “neuroticism” and low “extraversion” personality profile (see section C in Table 1) (e.g., Gray & McNaughton, Reference Gray and McNaughton2000). In sum, RLAs’ behavioral profile seems to be dominated (and driven) by negative affectivity, which is a core characteristic of human internalizing disorders (e.g., depression, anxiety- and stress-related disorders; see e.g., Michelini et al., Reference Michelini, Palumbo, DeYoung, Latzman and Kotov2021). Thus, RLA rats phenotypes seem to fall within the domain of “Internalizing” spectra and “Fear” and “Distress” sub-factors in terms of the “Hierarchical Taxonomy of Psychopathology” (HiTOP; Michelini et al., Reference Michelini, Palumbo, DeYoung, Latzman and Kotov2021) system (see section B in Table 1).

Table 1. Overview of neurobehavioral traits of RLA and RHA rats, correspondence with RDoC domains, the HiTOP psychopathology spectra (Michelini et al., Reference Michelini, Palumbo, DeYoung, Latzman and Kotov2021) and the “Big Five” and “Eysenck-Gray” personality factors

(+), (-); increased/improved, decreased/impaired, respectively. For reasons of space, the references are placed in columns C and D, but they also apply to columns A and B. See text for abbreviations.

Neuroanatomical and neuro-functional profiles of Roman low-avoidance rats

At neurochemical and molecular levels, selection of outbred rats for extremely divergent rates of acquisition of TWAA, or comparison of RLA vs RHA rats, has been shown to be associated with differential expression of many genes related to neural and synaptic function at the hippocampal and amygdala levels (Díaz-Morán et al., 2013a, Reference Díaz-Morán, Palència, Mont-Cardona, Cañete, Blázquez, Martínez- Membrives and Fernández-Teruel2013b; Sabariego et al., Reference Sabariego, Morón, Gómez, Donaire, Tobeña, Fernández-Teruel and Torres2013). In this regard, and consistent with descriptions of the neuropsychology of anxiety (Gray & McNaughton, Reference Gray and McNaughton2000; McNaughton & Corr, Reference McNaughton and Corr2004), relative to RHA rats the RLAs display a more pronounced neural activation of the hippocampus (HPC) and amygdala (AMY), as reflected by increased c-Fos expression when exposed to different novelty-based approach-avoidance conflict situations (Meyza, Boguszewski, Nikolaev & Zagrodzka, Reference Meyza, Boguszewski, Nikolaev and Zagrodzka2009). In addition, the activity of the phospholipase C signaling cascade is higher in the HPC of RLA vs. RHA rats (Sallés et al., Reference Sallés, López de Jesús, Goñi, Fernández-Teruel, Driscoll, Tobeña and Escorihuela2001). Accordingly, the volume of the HPC and AMY is larger (Río-Álamos et al., Reference Río-Álamos, Piludu, Gerbolés, Barroso, Oliveras, Sánchez-González and Fernández-Teruel2019), and the neuronal density is higher in both brain areas of RLA vs. RHA rats (Gómez et al., Reference Gómez, Morón, Torres, Esteban, de la Torre, Cándido and Escarabajal2009; Garcia-Falgueras, Castillo-Ruiz, Put, Tobeña & Fernández-Teruel, Reference Garcia-Falgueras, Castillo-Ruiz, Put, Tobeña and Fernández-Teruel2012).

Authors from our group have studied the relationship between functioning of Gray and McNaughton’s “Behavioural Inhibition System” (BIS; as measured by the “Sensitivity to Punishment” scale; Torrubia, Ávila, Moltó & Caseras, Reference Torrubia, Ávila, Moltó and Caseras2001) and HPC and AMY volumes in adult healthy human subjects. Consistent with the theory (Gray & McNaughton, Reference Gray and McNaughton2000), and with the above findings from RLA (vs. RHA) rats, the authors found that BIS scores were highly correlated with HPC and AMY gray matter volume, thus suggesting that behavioral inhibition (resulting from increased activity of the BIS) is associated with enhanced HPC and AMY function (Barrós-Loscertales et al., Reference Barrós-Loscertales, Meseguer, Sanjuán, Belloch, Parcet, Torrubia and Avila2006, and references therein).

Importantly also, the activation of the medial PFC (mPFC; as measured by c-Fos expression) following exposure to various novelty tests or to a prepulse inhibition (PPI) session, as well as mPFC volume, is also enhanced in RLA rats relative to their RHA counterparts (Meyza et al., Reference Meyza, Boguszewski, Nikolaev and Zagrodzka2009; Río-Álamos et al., Reference Río-Álamos, Piludu, Gerbolés, Barroso, Oliveras, Sánchez-González and Fernández-Teruel2019; Tapias-Espinosa et al., Reference Tapias-Espinosa, Río-Álamos, Sánchez-González, Oliveras, Sampedro-Viana, Castillo-Ruiz and Fernández-Teruel2019, Reference Tapias-Espinosa, Sánchez-González, Cañete, Sampedro-Viana, Castillo-Ruiz, Oliveras and Fernández-Teruel2023) (see section D in Table 1). On the other hand, alterations of the GABA-A/benzodiazepine receptor and levels of endogenous benzodiazepine-like molecules have also been found in cortex and HPC of RLA (vs. RHA) rats (Giorgi et al., Reference Giorgi, Orlandi, Escorihuela, Driscoll, Lecca and Corda1994; Driscoll et al., Reference Driscoll, Ferré, Fernández-Teruel, Levi de Stein, Wolfman, Medina and Escorihuela1995; Bentareha et al., Reference Bentareha, Araujo, Ruano, Driscoll, Escorihuela, Tobeña and Vitorica1998) (see section D in Table 1).

In sum, the enhanced activity of HPC, mPFC and AMY, together with more stress-responsive hypothalamus (HPA-axis) and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (Steimer & Driscoll, Reference Steimer and Driscoll2003; Carrasco et al., Reference Carrasco, Márquez, Nadal, Tobeña, Fernández-Teruel and Armario2008; Río-Álamos et al., Reference Río-Álamos, Oliveras, Piludu, Gerbolés, Cañete, Blázquez and Fernández-Teruel2017; see section D in Table 1), seem to be consistent with theory (Gray & McNaughton, Reference Gray and McNaughton2000) and with the behaviorally-inhibited and high threat-sensitivity profile of RLA rats (Giorgi et al., Reference Giorgi, Corda and Fernández-Teruel2019) (sections A-B in Table 1).

Selection for rapid two-way active avoidance task acquisition led to behaviorally disinhibited, reward sensitive and “externalizing” Roman high-avoidance rats

Compared with RLA and other laboratory strains/lines, the RHAs are behaviorally disinhibited, exhibiting a consistent low-anxious profile in unconditioned and conditioned tests of anxiety or fear (Fernández-Teruel et al., 2021; Steimer & Driscoll Reference Steimer and Driscoll2003; Giorgi et al., Reference Giorgi, Corda and Fernández-Teruel2019). They are hyperactive and highly exploratory, novelty/sensation seeking, impulsive and present impairments of attention, sensorimotor gating (PPI), latent inhibition, startle habituation, working memory and cognitive flexibility (see sections A-B in Table 1). Remarkably, RHA rats exhibit increased locomotor and nucleus accumbens (NAcc) dopaminergic (DA) sensitization to chronic psychostimulants and other abused drugs (morphine, ethanol), enhanced cocaine self-administration and reinstatement after extinction, enhanced preference for ethanol and natural rewards and increased functional tone of the mesolimbic DA system (see sections B and D in Table 1). Vulnerability to drug addiction and schizophrenia are thought to share some core features and/or mechanisms. In particular, for instance, the increased functional tone of the mesolimbic DA system is considered to be at the core of both schizophrenia and drug addiction and is present in RHA rats relative to RLAs (Giorgi et al., Reference Giorgi, Corda and Fernández-Teruel2019; Fernández-Teruel et al., Reference Fernández-Teruel, Oliveras, Cañete, Rio-Álamos, Tapias-Espinosa, Sampedro-Viana and Giorgi2021).

Collectively, the above profiles are consistent with RHA rats being driven by “positive valence” processes (see section A in Table 1) and characterized by high reward sensitivity (and impulsivity) and impairments in the cognitive domain, which are compatible with a personality dominated by “Impulsivity/Sensation seeking” traits (Zuckerman, Murtaugh & Siegel, Reference Zuckerman, Murtaugh and Siegel1974; Siegel, Sisson & Driscoll, Reference Siegel, Sisson and Driscoll1993; Gray & McNaughton, Reference Gray and McNaughton2000; Giorgi et al., Reference Giorgi, Corda and Fernández-Teruel2019) (see section C in Table 1). Impulsivity, or impairment of behavioral control (i.e., behavioral inhibition deficit), is often a cause of both maladaptive behaviors directed toward an individual’s environment and interference in life functioning. These traits are typically associated with attention/cognition impairments, impulsive choices/actions (i.e., premature responses or actions without foresight, often leading to adverse or maladaptive consequences) and hyperactivity. If combined with high reward/sensation seeking, they relate to risky behavior and vulnerability to substance use and abuse (e.g., review by Giorgi et al., Reference Giorgi, Corda and Fernández-Teruel2019, and references therein). All these aspects converge in RHA’s behavioral profile (Fernández-Teruel et al., Reference Fernández-Teruel, Oliveras, Cañete, Rio-Álamos, Tapias-Espinosa, Sampedro-Viana and Giorgi2021; Giorgi et al., Reference Giorgi, Corda and Fernández-Teruel2019). Hence, the RHA rats seem to fall within the “Disinhibited Externalizing” spectra and “Substance Abuse” sub-factor, and likely also within the “Thought disorder” spectra (Michelini et al., Reference Michelini, Palumbo, DeYoung, Latzman and Kotov2021; see section B in Table 1), as they present various schizophrenia-linked traits, such as, e.g., hyperactivity, relative asociality, and impairments of inhibitory control (impulsivity) and attentional/cognitive processes (Fernández-Teruel et al., Reference Fernández-Teruel, Oliveras, Cañete, Rio-Álamos, Tapias-Espinosa, Sampedro-Viana and Giorgi2021; Giorgi et al., Reference Giorgi, Corda and Fernández-Teruel2019) (see Table 1).

Neurobiological and molecular profiles of Roman high-avoidance rats

Recent studies have shown that RHA rats present alterations of pre-/postsynaptic markers and trophic factors in the PFC and/or HPC (e.g., neuregulin1, homer1, synaptophysin and brain-derived neurotrophic factor/BDNF) that have been linked with glutamatergic dysfunction, PFC maturation and schizophrenia (Elfving et al., Reference Elfving, Müller, Oliveras, Østerbøg, Rio-Alamos, Sánchez-González and Aznar2019; reviewed by Fernandez-Teruel et al., Reference Fernández-Teruel, Oliveras, Cañete, Rio-Álamos, Tapias-Espinosa, Sampedro-Viana and Giorgi2021). In addition, RHA rats exhibit increased DA D1 receptors in the PFC and the NAcc, augmented NMDA2B receptors in the PFC, enhanced 5-HT2A receptors in PFC and HPC, a dramatic deficit of mGlu2 receptors in PFC, HPC and striatum (Elfving et al., Reference Elfving, Müller, Oliveras, Østerbøg, Rio-Alamos, Sánchez-González and Aznar2019), and increased density of immature dendritic spines in the PFC (Sánchez-González et al., Reference Sánchez-González, Thougaard, Tapias-Espinosa, Cañete, Sampedro-Viana, Saunders and Fernández-Teruel2021). This is overall consistent with the evidence of lowered function and volume of the PFC and HPC in RHA rats (e.g., Meyza et al., Reference Meyza, Boguszewski, Nikolaev and Zagrodzka2009; Río-Álamos et al., Reference Río-Álamos, Piludu, Gerbolés, Barroso, Oliveras, Sánchez-González and Fernández-Teruel2019; Tapias-Espinosa et al., Reference Tapias-Espinosa, Río-Álamos, Sánchez-González, Oliveras, Sampedro-Viana, Castillo-Ruiz and Fernández-Teruel2019, Reference Tapias-Espinosa, Sánchez-González, Cañete, Sampedro-Viana, Castillo-Ruiz, Oliveras and Fernández-Teruel2023). These molecular/synaptic markers and neuro-functional profiles strongly suggest that RHA rats have immature (i.e., adolescent-like) PFC and (likely) HPC, which lead to impaired top-down inhibitory control (e.g., of the mesolimbic DA system) (Sønderstrup et al., Reference Sønderstrup, Batiuk, Mantas, Tapias-Espinosa, Oliveras, Cañete and Aznar2023). Collectively, these profiles cohere with their behavioral/cognitive alterations, and with their drug-seeking and schizophrenia-linked trait profiles (Fernandez-Teruel et al., Reference Fernández-Teruel, Oliveras, Cañete, Rio-Álamos, Tapias-Espinosa, Sampedro-Viana and Giorgi2021; Giorgi et al., Reference Giorgi, Corda and Fernández-Teruel2019).

It has been known since the late 1990s that the reward-sensitive and impulsive RHA rats are characterized by an enhanced functional tone of the mesolimbic DA system, as shown, for instance, by in vivo DA microdialisis in the NAcc following sensitization to psychostimulants (e.g., Giorgi et al., Reference Giorgi, Piras and Corda2007; Tournier et al., Reference Tournier, Steimer, Millet, Moulin-Sallanon, Vallet, Ibañez and Ginovart2013; Bellés et al., Reference Bellés, Dimiziani, Tsartsalis, Millet, Herrmann and Ginovart2021a; reviewed by Giorgi et al., Reference Giorgi, Corda and Fernández-Teruel2019; and Fernández-Teruel et al., Reference Fernández-Teruel, Oliveras, Cañete, Rio-Álamos, Tapias-Espinosa, Sampedro-Viana and Giorgi2021). Remarkably, these early findings are consistent with more recent findings obtained in healthy humans. Thus, reward sensitivity, as measured by the “Behavioural Activation System” (BAS) trait scale (Carver & White, Reference Carver and White1994; Gray & McNaughton, Reference Gray and McNaughton2000), is positively correlated with activity (measured through fMRI) of the dorsomedial and ventral striatum (equivalent to the NAcc in rats) during a gambling task involving reward-punishment conflict (Costumero et al., Reference Costumero, Barrós-Loscertales, Fuentes, Rosell-Negre, Bustamante and Ávila2016). Hence, it is noteworthy that the findings on DA function in RHA (vs. RLA) rats translate to humans.

Epigenetic and “genotype x environment” interaction effects on personality/psychopathology

Certain traumas, or stressful or positive events, particularly those experienced at young ages, can lead to markers (e.g., DNA methylation) being placed on genes, changing their functional expression. These are the subject of study of epigenetics (e.g., Francis, Diorio, Liu & Meaney, Reference Francis, Diorio, Liu and Meaney1999; Bahari-Javan et al., Reference Bahari-Javan, Varbanov, Halder, Benito, Kaurani, Burkhardt and Fischer2017). Among other things, epigenetics deals with the study of “why”, “when” and “how” the markers that environmental life events place on genes can lead to traceable changes in the development of the nervous system, biological and mental processes and behavior.

Neonatal handling (NH; a neonatal stimulation treatment administered to rats or mice during the first weeks of life) and environmental enrichment (EE; administered for several months from puberty to adulthood) induce stable long-lasting changes in the personality profile and HPA-axis stress responses in RLA as well as many other laboratory rats, so that treated rats become enduringly much less threat- and stress-sensitive (e.g., see Fernández-Teruel, Escorihuela, Castellano, González & Tobeña, Reference Fernández-Teruel, Escorihuela, Castellano, González and Tobeña1997; Fernández-Teruel et al., Reference Fernández-Teruel, Giménez-Llort, Escorihuela, Gil, Aguilar, Steimer and Tobeña2002a, Reference Fernández-Teruel, Giménez-Llort, Escorihuela, Gil, Aguilar, Steimer and Tobeña2002b; Río-Álamos et al., Reference Río-Álamos, Oliveras, Piludu, Gerbolés, Cañete, Blázquez and Fernández-Teruel2017, Reference Río-Álamos, Piludu, Gerbolés, Barroso, Oliveras, Sánchez-González and Fernández-Teruel2019, and references therein) than their non-handled counterparts. NH treatment of offspring induces improved dams’ maternal behavior, which in turn induces epigenetic changes in the HPC involving the nerve growth factor-inducible protein A (NGFI-A) transcription factor and glucocorticoid receptors, and thus HPA-axis function (Francis et al., Reference Francis, Diorio, Liu and Meaney1999; Weaver et al., Reference Weaver, D’Alessio, Brown, Hellstrom, Dymov, Sharma and Meaney2007). Importantly, neonatal handling leads to an enduring reduction of HPC and AMY volume in RLA rats, which correlates with the NH-decreased threat sensitivity and anxious/fear behavior (Río-Álamos et al., Reference Río-Álamos, Oliveras, Piludu, Gerbolés, Cañete, Blázquez and Fernández-Teruel2017, Reference Río-Álamos, Piludu, Gerbolés, Barroso, Oliveras, Sánchez-González and Fernández-Teruel2019).

Many “genotype x environment” interactions have been reported for the effects of NH and/or EE treatments on RHA/RLA rat behavior and neuroendocrine traits (for reviews, see Fernández-Teruel et al., Reference Fernández-Teruel, Giménez-Llort, Escorihuela, Gil, Aguilar, Steimer and Tobeña2002a, Reference Fernández-Teruel, Oliveras, Cañete, Rio-Álamos, Tapias-Espinosa, Sampedro-Viana and Giorgi2021; and Río-Álamos et al., Reference Río-Álamos, Oliveras, Piludu, Gerbolés, Cañete, Blázquez and Fernández-Teruel2017, Reference Río-Álamos, Piludu, Gerbolés, Barroso, Oliveras, Sánchez-González and Fernández-Teruel2019, and references therein). NH and EE treatments also influence many aspects of personality of RHA rats. For example, NH improves attentional (PPI) and cognitive functions in this strain, and NH and/or EE long-lastingly improve spatial cognitive processes of RHA rats and enhance novelty- and/or substance-seeking behavior in both Roman rat strains (e.g., Fernández-Teruel et al., Reference Fernández-Teruel, Escorihuela, Castellano, González and Tobeña1997, Reference Fernández-Teruel, Driscoll, Gil, Aguilar, Tobeña and Escorihuela2002b; Río-Álamos et al., Reference Río-Álamos, Piludu, Gerbolés, Barroso, Oliveras, Sánchez-González and Fernández-Teruel2019; Bellés, Dimiziani, Herrmann & Ginovart, Reference Bellés, Dimiziani, Herrmann and Ginovart2021b; Bellés, Arrondeau, Urueña-Méndez & Ginovart, Reference Bellés, Arrondeau, Urueña-Méndez and Ginovart2023).

Regarding adverse early environmental events, we have found that social isolation rearing (from pre-puberty to adulthood) affects RHA/RLA rat behavior in a manner dependent upon the strain (i.e., “genotype x environment” interactions). Thus, in RHA (but not RLA) rats this isolation enhances anxiety and hyperactivity; and impairs PPI and PFC-/HPC-related reference memory, thus further increasing the schizophrenia-related impairments of RHA rats (Oliveras et al., Reference Oliveras, Sánchez-González, Piludu, Gerbolés, Río-Álamos, Tobeña and Fernández-Teruel2016; Sánchez-González et al., Reference Sánchez-González, Oliveras, Río-Álamos, Piludu, Gerbolés, Tapias-Espinosa and Fernández-Teruel2020).

Still, in the context of adverse early events (i.e., early life stress), caregiving maltreatment produces epigenetic changes in the rat adolescent brain (e.g., HPC, AMY), which could have relevance to adolescent mental health and behavior (Doherty et al., Reference Doherty, Forster and Roth2016). In relation to psychopathology, Bahari-Javan et al., (Reference Bahari-Javan, Varbanov, Halder, Benito, Kaurani, Burkhardt and Fischer2017) showed that (i) early life stress (ELS)-induced schizophrenia-like behavioral and synaptic phenotypes in mice were associated with increased expression of histone-deacetylase-1 (Hdac1; one of the epigenetic processes) in PFC, possibly linked to DNA methylation changes (another epigenetic process) and (ii) administration of an HDAC1 inhibitor reversed these schizophrenia-like phenotypes. Importantly, Hdac1 blood levels were also increased specifically in patients with schizophrenia who had been exposed to ELS as compared with patients with schizophrenia who didn’t suffer ELS (Bahari-Javan et al., Reference Bahari-Javan, Varbanov, Halder, Benito, Kaurani, Burkhardt and Fischer2017). Thus, by using a translatable animal model of induction of schizophrenia-relevant behavioral features, it was suggested that a biological (epigenetic) factor might be a relevant antecedent of at least some types of (environmentally-linked) schizophrenia, to the point that “…analysis of Hdac1 expression in blood could be used for patient stratification and individualized therapy” (Bahari-Javan et al., Reference Bahari-Javan, Varbanov, Halder, Benito, Kaurani, Burkhardt and Fischer2017, p. E4686).

The above examples illustrate that both personality and associated neurobiological and psychopathological processes can be enduringly influenced by environmental factors in a manner dependent upon genetic background (“genotype x environment” interactions). Epigenetics tells us that these environmental influences on proneness to particular types of pathological symptom clusters will operate in many cases through environmentally-induced changes in the epigenome. Thus, manipulating the environment in animal models, at different neurodevelopmental stages and in both (negative and positive) directions (Fernández-Teruel, Reference Fernández-Teruel2021), and exploring neurobiological and epigenetic changes and their relation with variation of personality and psychopathology-linked phenotypes, might be crucial for a coherent/integrative and translatable development of personality neuroscience and neuroscience-based psychopathology.

What more can the Roman rats tell us about personality neuroscience and psychopathology?

Unlike selection of many other rat lines/strains for simpler innate behavioral traits, selection of the Roman rats for their (high/low) ability to acquire the functionally complex two-way avoidance task involves a variety of processes. Two-way avoidance involves (at least) CS and context Pavlovian threat conditioning (related to passive avoidance and freezing – i.e., passive coping- tendencies), instrumental escape conditioning, one-way active avoidance conditioning, locomotor activity in a threat-conditioned context (e.g., inter-trial crossings) and flight responses (undirected active responses searching alternative escape routes).

It might be argued that the markedly enhanced capacity of RHA rats to acquire TWAA learning might be due to the fact that they make better associations between the fear-eliciting CS and the aversive US (Mowrer & Lamoreaux, Reference Mowrer and Lamoreaux1946). Studies of Pavlovian threat conditioning (i.e., classically-conditioned freezing) and fear-potentiated startle seem to argue against that contention, since in these paradigms RHAs exhibit both lowered conditioned freezing and decreased potentiated startle responses (respectively) relative to RLA rats (López-Aumatell et al., Reference López-Aumatell, Blázquez, Gil, Aguilar, Cañete, Giménez-Llort and Fernández-Teruel2009; see further references in Fernández-Teruel et al., 2021). Importantly, also, the ability to acquire one-way active avoidance responding does not differ in RHA vs. RLA rats when safety time is relatively long (30 s), i.e., when approach-avoidance conflict is low. Conversely, when safety time is drastically reduced (1 s), and thus approach-avoidance conflict is high, RHA rats acquire one-way active avoidance faster than RLA rats (Torres et al., Reference Torres, Escarabajal, Cándido, de la Torre, Gómez, Maldonado and Fernández-Teruel2007). These findings collectively suggest that fear-eliciting CS–US associations are stronger in RLA than RHA rats, thus prompting the former strain to develop enhanced Pavlovian reactions (i.e., freezing, passive avoidance), which interfere with active (escape/avoidance) responses, particularly when intensity of approach-avoidance conflict (and thus, threat) is high (Fernández-Teruel & McNaughton, Reference Fernández-Teruel and McNaughton2023).

In sum, the various processes involved in TWAA acquisition, as mentioned above, together (or in interaction) with parental strain (Wistar) and individual genetic characteristics and/or vulnerabilities (e.g., threat sensitivity vs. reward sensitivity), have led to two rat lines/strains that differ in a large number of stable neurobehavioural traits. These are not specific for two-way active avoidance, nor are only related to defensive responses in the face of threat (threat sensitivity) or approach-avoidance conflict, but extend to changes in responses to positive reinforcers (reward sensitivity, drug addiction), impulsivity and attentional/cognitive processes (Fernández-Teruel et al., Reference Fernández-Teruel, Oliveras, Cañete, Rio-Álamos, Tapias-Espinosa, Sampedro-Viana and Giorgi2021; Giorgi et al., Reference Giorgi, Corda and Fernández-Teruel2019).

Hence, the threat-sensitive RLA rats present comorbidity with fear and distress, within the “Internalizing” spectra (see Table 1; Michelini et al., Reference Michelini, Palumbo, DeYoung, Latzman and Kotov2021). The extensive neurobiological and behavioral characterization carried out with RLA rats (e.g., Fernández-Teruel et al., Reference Fernández-Teruel, Oliveras, Cañete, Rio-Álamos, Tapias-Espinosa, Sampedro-Viana and Giorgi2021; Steimer & Driscoll, Reference Steimer and Driscoll2003; Giorgi et al., Reference Giorgi, Corda and Fernández-Teruel2019) makes them a very promising tool for personality neuroscience and psychopathology research. They might be particularly useful, for example, to study the neurobiological underpinnings of behavioral disinhibition (and top-down inhibitory control) mechanisms (e.g., PFC, HPC), relations among top-down inhibitory control and (good) cognitive performance, anxiety, stress-induced depression, anxious depression, reward-devaluation-induced frustration, environmental influences on these symptoms/phenotypes and epigenetic changes underlying these influences.

The high reward-sensitive and behaviorally-disinhibited RHA rats present comorbidity of symptoms associated with “Thought disorder” (i.e., schizophrenia-linked phenotypes) and “Disinhibited externalizing” (substance abuse, impulsivity) spectra of the HiTOP system (Michelini et al., Reference Michelini, Palumbo, DeYoung, Latzman and Kotov2021; Table 1). Their behavioral/attentional/cognitive profile, combined with the extensively characterized neurobehavioural and molecular schizophrenia- and drug-addiction-linked phenotypes make them a unique tool to investigate comorbidity of schizophrenia-relevant features, drug abuse (and addiction) and PFC-, HPC- and NAcc-linked cognitive/behavioral alterations. As suggested by recent gene expression findings (e.g., Elfving et al., Reference Elfving, Müller, Oliveras, Østerbøg, Rio-Alamos, Sánchez-González and Aznar2019; Sánchez-González et al., Reference Sánchez-González, Thougaard, Tapias-Espinosa, Cañete, Sampedro-Viana, Saunders and Fernández-Teruel2021; Sønderstrup et al., Reference Sønderstrup, Batiuk, Mantas, Tapias-Espinosa, Oliveras, Cañete and Aznar2023), the RHA rats can be a useful tool for understanding the neural maturational trajectory leading to the behavioral and cognitive disturbances associated with schizophrenia and perhaps comorbid traits/symptoms, such as drug-seeking/abuse and impulsivity. In addition, this would allow us to investigate whether early “therapeutic” interventions (e.g., infantile and/or juvenile enriched stimulation, such as NH and EE treatments, combined or not with ELS) might modify these neuro-molecular pathways (or factors) and phenotype expression.

In summary, some examples of the heuristic and translational potential of the Roman rat models are the following:

  1. 1. In the RHA and RLA strains, HPC and AMY volume positively correlate with behavioral inhibition in anxiety-related tests (Río-Álamos et al., Reference Río-Álamos, Oliveras, Piludu, Gerbolés, Cañete, Blázquez and Fernández-Teruel2017), consistent with what has been reported between a BIS measure (Torrubia et al., Reference Torrubia, Ávila, Moltó and Caseras2001) and volume of these two regions in humans (Barros-Loscertales et al., 2006).

  2. 2. Also, in line with human studies showing a positive association between a BAS-trait measure and activity of the striatum during a gambling task (Costumero et al., Reference Costumero, Barrós-Loscertales, Fuentes, Rosell-Negre, Bustamante and Ávila2016), behavioral disinhibition (impulsivity) and novelty seeking in Roman rats are predicted by striatal function (measured by amphetamine-induced DA release; Tournier et al., Reference Tournier, Steimer, Millet, Moulin-Sallanon, Vallet, Ibañez and Ginovart2013; Bellés et al., Reference Bellés, Dimiziani, Tsartsalis, Millet, Herrmann and Ginovart2021a; see Giorgi et al., Reference Giorgi, Corda and Fernández-Teruel2019).

  3. 3. The anxious (and alcohol-non-preferring) RLA rats are more sensitive to reward loss-induced frustration than the low-anxious (and alcohol-preferring) RHA rats. Reward loss (appetitive extinction) enhances ethanol preference in RLA (but not RHA) rats, similar to enhanced alcohol consumption (i.e., emotion-induced “self-medication”) in humans experiencing sudden anxiogenic/stressful conditions. Pre-training in a partial reinforcement (PR) instrumental program, a treatment that leads to development of resilience to (reward) loss-induced anxiety, suppresses loss-induced ethanol preference in RLAs in parallel to increasing their frustration tolerance (Cuenya et al., Reference Cuenya, Sabariego, Donaire, Fernández-Teruel, Tobeña, Gómez and Torres2012; Manzo et al., Reference Manzo, Gómez, Callejas-Aguilera, Fernández-Teruel, Papini and Torres2015). This makes RLA (vs. RHA) rats a translatable model to study innate differences in, and mechanisms of “frustration tolerance”, vulnerability to negative emotion-induced self-medication (and substance use/abuse) and neurobehavioral mechanisms mediating resilience to loss-induced anxiety/frustration.

  4. 4. Excessive anxiety of RLA rats in different tests/tasks is attenuated by anxiolytic drugs and increased by known anxiogenic drugs (see Supplementary Table S2 in Fernández-Teruel et al., 2021).

  5. 5. Augmenting of cortical visual-evoked potentials (VEPs) is considered a specific neurophysiological marker of high sensation-novelty seeking (and disinhibited) personality in humans. Remarkably, similar to what has been observed in disinhibited sensation-novelty seeker humans and cats, the RHA rats are cortical VEP augmenters, whereas RLA and Wistar rats are VEP-reducers like non-disinhibited humans (Zuckerman et al., Reference Zuckerman, Murtaugh and Siegel1974; Siegel et al., Reference Siegel, Sisson and Driscoll1993; Siegel & Driscoll, Reference Siegel and Driscoll1996).

  6. 6. Novelty seeking in RHA/RLA rats is inversely associated with postsynaptic dopamine D2 receptor availability in striatum, and similar findings have been reported in humans (reviewed by Giorgi et al., Reference Giorgi, Corda and Fernández-Teruel2019). Consistently, in a large sample of healthy and distressed humans the activity of the ventrolateral PFC and ventral striatum, during an ‘‘uncertain reward expectancy’’ paradigm, was positively related to impulsive-sensation seeking. This is also in line with the enhanced functional tone of the mesolimbic dopaminergic pathway (including the NAcc) in the impulsive-sensation/novelty seeker RHA rats (see Giorgi et al., Reference Giorgi, Corda and Fernández-Teruel2019).

  7. 7. In RHA (but not RLA) rats there is a high positive correlation between cortical 5-HT2A receptor density and premature (impulsive) responses in the “five choice serial reaction time” task. This is consistent with findings from studies in rodents and humans in which the pharmacological blockade of the 5-HT2A receptor produces reductions of several types of impulsive responses, and with the notion that 5-HT2A receptors may facilitate some forms of impulsive behavior (reviewed by Klein et al., Reference Klein, Ultved, Adamsen, Santini, Tobeña, Fernández-Teruel and Mikkelsen2014, and Giorgi et al., Reference Giorgi, Corda and Fernández-Teruel2019).

  8. 8. Similar to untreated patients with schizophrenia, RHA rats exhibit enhanced cortical 5-HT2A receptor density and a dramatic deficit of mGlu2 receptors, which has been proposed to be a crucial factor for the neurodevelopmental alterations and the rich schizophrenia-related phenotypes of this rat strain (Fernández-Teruel et al., Reference Fernández-Teruel, Oliveras, Cañete, Rio-Álamos, Tapias-Espinosa, Sampedro-Viana and Giorgi2021).

  9. 9. Both schizophrenia symptoms and drug addiction are related to increased mesolimbic dopaminergic function in humans and in animal models (e.g., reviewed by Giorgi et al., Reference Giorgi, Corda and Fernández-Teruel2019). Consistently, relative to the non-drug preferring RLA rats, RHAs exhibit schizophrenia-related phenotypes and vulnerability to drug addiction, which are paralleled by enhanced functional tone of the mesolimbic dopaminergic system in RHAs (reviewed by Giorgi et al., Reference Giorgi, Piras and Corda2007, Reference Giorgi, Corda and Fernández-Teruel2019; Dimiziani et al., Reference Dimiziani, Bellés, Tsartsalis, Millet, Herrmann and Ginovart2019).

A considerable number of pharmacological studies, with anxiolytic, antidepressant and anxiogenic drugs, or with psychostimulants, pro-psychotic and antipsychotic compounds, among others, have been carried out in the Roman rat lines/strains (e.g., Oliveras et al., Reference Oliveras, Sánchez-González, Sampedro-Viana, Piludu, Río-Alamos, Giorgi and Fernández-Teruel2017; Sampedro-Viana et al., Reference Sampedro-Viana, Cañete, Sanna, Oliveras, Lavín, Torrecilla and Fernández-Teruel2023; see reviews by Fernández-Teruel et al., Reference Fernández-Teruel, Oliveras, Cañete, Rio-Álamos, Tapias-Espinosa, Sampedro-Viana and Giorgi2021, and Giorgi et al, Reference Giorgi, Corda and Fernández-Teruel2019, and further references). While these studies globally give support to the predictive validity of RLAs as a model of anxiety (i.e., threat sensitivity) and RHAs as a model of schizophrenia-relevant phenotypes and drug-addiction vulnerability (i.e., reward sensitivity), much more psycho- and neuro-pharmacological research is still necessary. Future efforts should focus on (i) expanding the number of relevant behavioral phenotypes affected by drugs in both rat strains, (ii) exploring neurobiological/molecular mechanisms affected by treatments shown to be effective in changing particular phenotypes and, (iii) extending behavioral, pharmacological and neurobiological research to both sexes (Fernández-Teruel et al., 2021).

Collectively, the findings reviewed here provide an overview of the major traits that distinguish RLA vs. RHA rats, from internalizing to externalizing spectra, from neuroticism- and threat-sensitivity-linked vulnerabilities (RLA) to impulsivity and reward sensitivity-related traits (RHA), from good attention/cognitive capacities (RLA) to schizophrenia-linked phenotypes (RHA). Our aim has been to highlight (also giving several examples of translatability) that these two rat strains/lines may constitute important tools for translational research on personality neuroscience and neuroscience-based psychopathology.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the anonymous reviewers and the editors of this Special Issue for their constructive comments, which importantly helped to improve the quality of this review article.

Financial support

The authors receive support from grants PID2020–114697GB-I00 (ref. AEI/10.13039/501100011033) and 2021-SGR-00557.

Competing Interests

The authors have nothing to disclose.

References

Aguilar, R., Gil, L., Tobeña, A., Escorihuela, R. M., & Fernández-Teruel, A. (2000). Differential effects of cohort removal stress on the acoustic startle response of the Roman/Verh rat strains. Behavior Genetics, 30, 7175. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1002042711672 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amsel, A. (1992). Frustration theory: an analysis of dispositional learning and memory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andión, O., Caseras, X., Fullana, M.A., Fernández-Teruel, A., Ferrer, M., Casas, M., & Torrubia, R. (2013). Cued aversive classical conditioning in humans: The role of trait-anxiety. Open Journal of Psychiatry, 3, 230237. https://doi.org/10.4236/ojpsych.2013.32021 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bahari-Javan, S., Varbanov, H., Halder, R., Benito, E., Kaurani, L., Burkhardt, S., … Fischer, A. (2017). HDAC1 links early life stress to schizophrenia-like phenotypes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences-USA, 114, E4686E4694. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1613842114 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrós-Loscertales, A., Meseguer, V., Sanjuán, A., Belloch, V., Parcet, M. A., Torrubia, R. & Avila, C. (2006). Behavioral Inhibition System activity is associated with increased amygdala and hippocampal gray matter volume: A voxel-based morphometry study. Neuroimage, 33, 10111015. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.07.025 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bellés, L., Dimiziani, A., Tsartsalis, S., Millet, P., Herrmann, F. R., & Ginovart, N. (2021a). Dopamine D2/3 receptor availabilities and evoked dopamine release in striatum differentially predict impulsivity and novelty preference in Roman high- and Low-avoidance rats. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 24, 239251. https://doi.org/10.1093/ijnp/pyaa084 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bellés, L., Dimiziani, A., Herrmann, F. R., & Ginovart, N. (2021b). Early environmental enrichment and impoverishment differentially affect addiction-related behavioral traits, cocaine-taking, and dopamine D2/3 receptor signaling in a rat model of vulnerability to drug abuse. Psychopharmacology, 238, 35433557. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-021-05971-z CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bellés, L., Arrondeau, C., Urueña-Méndez, G., & Ginovart, N. (2023). Concurrent measures of impulsive action and choice are partially related and differentially modulated by dopamine D1- and D2-like receptors in a rat model of impulsivity. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 222, 173508. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pbb.2022.173508 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bentareha, R., Araujo, F., Ruano, D., Driscoll, P., Escorihuela, R.M., Tobeña, A., … Vitorica, J. (1998). Pharmacological properties of the GABA(A) receptor complex from brain regions of (hypoemotional) Roman high- and (hyperemotional) low-avoidance rats. European Journal of Pharmacology, 354, 9197. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0014-2999(98)00428-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bignami, G. (1965). Selection for high rates and low rates of avoidance conditioning in the rat. Animal Behavior, 13, 221227. https://doi.org/10.1016/0003-3472(65)90038-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Broadhurst, P. L., & Bignami, G. (1965). Correlative effects of psychogenetic selection: A study of the roman high and low avoidance strains of rats. Behavior Research and Therapy, 2, 273280. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(64)90033-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carrasco, J., Márquez, C., Nadal, R., Tobeña, A., Fernández-Teruel, A., & Armario, A. (2008). Characterization of central and peripheral components of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis in the inbred Roman rat strains. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 33, 437445. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.01.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 319333. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.319 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castanon, N., & Mormède, P. (1994). Psychobiogenetics: Adapted tools for the study of the coupling between behavioral and neuroendocrine traits of emotional reactivity. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 19, 257282. https://doi.org/10.1016/0306-4530(94)90065-5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costumero, V., Barrós-Loscertales, A., Fuentes, P., Rosell-Negre, P., Bustamante, J. C., & Ávila, C. (2016). BAS-drive trait modulates dorsomedial striatum activity during reward response-outcome associations. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 10, 869879. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-015-9466-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuenya, L., Sabariego, M., Donaire, R., Fernández-Teruel, A., Tobeña, A., Gómez, M. J., … Torres, C. (2012). The effect of partial reinforcement on instrumental successive negative contrast in inbred Roman High- (RHA-I) and Low- (RLA-I) Avoidance rats. Physiology and Behavior, 105, 11121116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.12.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delacour, J., Houcine, O., & Monmaur, P. (1978). Durées de veille et de sommeil chez les rats des souches Roman. Physiology and Behavior, 21, 717719. https://doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(78)90009-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Díaz-Morán, S., Palència, M., Mont-Cardona, C., Cañete, T., Blázquez, G., Martínez- Membrives, E., …. Fernández-Teruel, A. (2013a). Gene expression in hippocampus as a function of differential trait anxiety levels in genetically heterogeneous NIH-HS rats. Behavioural Brain Research, 257, 129139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2013.09.041 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Díaz-Morán, S., Palència, M., Mont-Cardona, C., Cañete, T., Blázquez, G., Martínez- Membrives, E., … Fernández-Teruel, A. (2013b). Gene expression in amygdala as a function of differential trait anxiety levels in genetically heterogeneous NIH-HS rats. Behavioural Brain Research, 252, 422431. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2013.05.066 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dimiziani, A., Bellés, L., Tsartsalis, S., Millet, P., Herrmann, F., & Ginovart, N. (2019). Differential involvement of D2 and D3 receptors during reinstatement of cocaine-seeking behavior in the Roman high- and low-avoidance rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 133, 7785. https://doi.org/10.1037/bne0000281 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doherty, T. S., Forster, A., & Roth, T.L. (2016). Global and gene-specific DNA methylation alterations in the adolescent amygdala and hippocampus in an animal model of caregiver maltreatment. Behavioural Brain Research, 298, 5561. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2015.05.028 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Driscoll, P., & Bättig, K. (1982). Behavioral, emotional and neurochemical profiles of rats selected extreme differences in active, two-way avoidance performance. In Lieblich, I. (Ed.), Genetics of the brain (pp. 95123), Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Driscoll, P., Escorihuela, R. M., Fernández-Teruel, A., Giorgi, O., Schwegler, H., Steimer, T.H., … Tobeña, A. (1998). Genetic selection and differential stress responses the roman lines/strains of rats. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 851, 501510. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1998.tb09029.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Driscoll, P., Ferré, P., Fernández-Teruel, A., Levi de Stein, M., Wolfman, C., Medina, J., … Escorihuela, R.M. (1995). Effects of prenatal diazepam on two-way avoidance behavior, swimming navigation and brain levels of benzodiazepine-like molecules in male Roman high- and low-avoidance rats. Psychopharmacology, 122, 5157. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02246441 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Durcan, M. J., Wraight, K. B., & Fulker, D. W. (1984). The current status of two sublines of the Roman High and Low Avoidance strains. Behavior Genetics, 14, 559569. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01068126 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elfving, B., Müller, H. K., Oliveras, I., Østerbøg, T. B., Rio-Alamos, C., Sánchez-González, A., … Aznar, S. (2019). Differential expression of synaptic markers regulated during neurodevelopment in a rat model of schizophrenia-like behavior. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 115, 128138. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2019.109669 Google Scholar
Escorihuela, R. M., Tobeña, A., Driscoll, P., & Fernández-Teruel, A. (1995). Effects of training, early handling, and perinatal flumazenil on shuttle box acquisition in Roman low-avoidance rats: Toward overcoming a genetic deficit. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 19, 353367. https://doi.org/10.1016/0149-7634(94)00051-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernández-Teruel, A., Escorihuela, R. M., Tobeña, A., & Driscoll, P. (1991). Stress and putative endogenous ligands for benzodiazepine receptors: The importance of characteristics of the aversive situation and of differential emotionality in experimental animals. Experientia, 47, 10511056. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01923340 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernández-Teruel, A., Escorihuela, R. M., Núñez, J. F., Gomà, M., Driscoll, P., & Tobeña, A. (1992). Early stimulation effects on novelty-induced behavior in two psychogenetically-selected rat lines with divergent emotionality profiles. Neuroscience Letters, 137, 185188. https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-3940(92)90400-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernández-Teruel, A., Escorihuela, R. M., Castellano, B., González, B., & Tobeña, A. (1997). Neonatal handling and environmental enrichment effects on emotionality, novelty/reward seeking, and age-related cognitive and hippocampal impairments: Focus on the Roman rat lines. Behavior Genetics, 27, 513526. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021400830503 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernández-Teruel, A., Giménez-Llort, L., Escorihuela, R. M., Gil, L., Aguilar, R., Steimer, T., & Tobeña, A. (2002a). Early-life handling stimulation and environmental enrichment: Are some of their effects mediated by similar neural mechanisms? Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 73, 233245. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0091-3057(02)00787-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernández-Teruel, A., Driscoll, P., Gil, L., Aguilar, R., Tobeña, A., & Escorihuela, R.M. (2002b). Enduring effects of environmental enrichment on novelty seeking, saccharin and ethanol intake in two rat lines (RHA/Verh and RLA/Verh) differing in incentive seeking behavior. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 73, 225231. https://doi.org/ 10.1016/S0091-3057(02)00784-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernández-Teruel, A. (2021). Conflict between threat sensitivity and sensation seeking in the adolescent brain: Role of the hippocampus, and neurobehavioural plasticity induced by pleasurable early enriched experience. Brain Sciences, 11, 268. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11020268 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernández-Teruel, A., Oliveras, I., Cañete, T., Rio-Álamos, C., Tapias-Espinosa, C., Sampedro-Viana, D., … Giorgi, O. (2021). Neurobehavioral and neurodevelopmental profiles of a heuristic genetic model of differential schizophrenia- and addiction-relevant features: The RHA vs. RLA rats. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 131, 597617. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.09.042 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernández-Teruel, A., & Tobeña, A. (2020). Revisiting the role of anxiety in the initial acquisition of two-way active avoidance: Pharmacological, behavioural and neuroanatomical convergence. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 118, 739758. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.08.014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernández-Teruel, A., & McNaughton, N. (2023). Post-encounter freezing during approach-avoidance conflict: The role of the hippocampus. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 24, 451452. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-023-00703-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fomsgaard, L., Moreno, J. L., de la Fuente-Revenga, M., Brudek, T., Adamsen, D., Rio-Alamos, C., … Aznar, S. (2018). Differences in 5-HT2A and mGlu2 receptor expression levels and repressive epigenetic modifications at the 5-HT2A promoter region in the Roman Low- (RLA-I) and High- (RHA-I) Avoidance rat strains. Molecular Neurobiology, 55, 19982012. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12035-017-0457-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Francis, D., Diorio, J., Liu, D., & Meaney, M. J. (1999) Nongenomic transmission across generations of maternal behavior and stress responses in the rat. Science, 286, 11551158. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.286.5442.115 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garcia-Falgueras, A., Castillo-Ruiz, M. M., Put, T., Tobeña, A., & Fernández-Teruel, A. (2012). Differential hippocampal neuron density between inbred Roman high- (low anxious) and low-avoidance (high anxious) rats. Neuroscience Letters, 522, 4146. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2012.06.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giorgi, O., Orlandi, M., Escorihuela, R. M., Driscoll, P., Lecca, D., & Corda, M. G. (1994). GABAergic and dopaminergic transmission in the brain of Roman high-avoidance and Roman low-avoidance rats. Brain Research, 638, 133138. https://doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(94)90642-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giorgi, O., Piras, G., Lecca, D., Hansson, S., Driscoll, P., & Corda, M. G. (2003). Differential neurochemical properties of central serotonergic transmission in Roman high- and low-avoidance rats. Journal of Neurochemistry, 86, 422431. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1471-4159.2003.01845.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giorgi, O., Piras, G., & Corda, M. G. (2007). The psychogenetically selected Roman high- and low-avoidance rat lines: A model to study the individual vulnerability to drug addiction. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 31, 148163. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.07.008 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giorgi, O., Corda, M. G., & Fernández-Teruel, A. (2019). A genetic model of impulsivity, vulnerability to drug abuse and schizophrenia-relevant symptoms with translational potential: The roman high- vs. Low-avoidance rats. Frontiers in Behavioural Neuroscience, 13, 145. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00145 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gómez, M. J., Morón, I., Torres, C., Esteban, F. J., de la Torre, L., Cándido, A., … Escarabajal, M. D. (2009). One-way avoidance acquisition and cellular density in the basolateral amygdala: Strain differences in Roman high- and low-avoidance rats. Neuroscience Letters, 450, 317320. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2008.10.112 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, J. A., & McNaughton, N. (2000). The Neuropsychology of Anxiety: An Enquiry Into the Funtions of the Septo-Hippocampal System (2nd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Guitart-Masip, M., Johansson, B., Fernández-Teruel, A., Cañete, T., Tobeña, A., Terenius, L., & Giménez-Llort, L. (2006). Divergent anatomical pattern of D1 and D3 binding and dopamine- and cyclic AMP-regulated phosphoprotein of 32 kDa mRNA expression in the Roman rat strains: Implications for drug addiction. Neuroscience, 142, 12311243. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.07.041 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, A. B., Ultved, L., Adamsen, D., Santini, M. A., Tobeña, A., Fernández-Teruel, A., … Mikkelsen, J. D. (2014). 5-HT(2A) and mGlu2 receptor binding levels are related to differences in impulsive behavior in the Roman Low- (RLA) and High- (RHA) avoidance rat strains. Neuroscience, 263, 3645. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.12.063 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
López-Aumatell, R., Blázquez, G., Gil, L., Aguilar, R., Cañete, T., Giménez-Llort, L., … Fernández-Teruel, A. (2009). The Roman High- and Low-Avoidance rat strains differ in fear-potentiated startle and classical aversive conditioning. Psicothema, 21, 2732. https://reunido.uniovi.es/index.php/PST/article/view/8791 Google ScholarPubMed
Manzo, L., Gómez, M. J., Callejas-Aguilera, J. E., Fernández-Teruel, A., Papini, M. R. & Torres, C. (2015). Partial reinforcement reduces vulnerability to anti-anxiety self-medication during appetitive extinction. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 28, 18. https://doi.org/10.46867/ijcp.2015.28.00.07 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNaughton, N., & Corr, P. J. (2004). A two-dimensional neuropsychology of defense: Fear/anxiety and defensive distance. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 28, 285305. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.03.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyza, K. Z., Boguszewski, P. M., Nikolaev, E., & Zagrodzka, J. (2009). Diverse sensitivity of RHA/Verh and RLA/Verh rats to emotional and spatial aspects of a novel environment as a result of a distinct pattern of neuronal activation in the fear/anxiety circuit. Behavior Genetics, 39, 4861. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-008-9234-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Michelini, G., Palumbo, I. M., DeYoung, C. G., Latzman, R. D., & Kotov, R. (2021). Linking RDoC and HiTOP: A new interface for advancing psychiatric nosology and neuroscience. Clinical Psychology Review, 86, 102025. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2021.102025 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moreno, M., Cardona, D., Gómez, M. J., Sánchez-Santed, F., Tobeña, A., Fernández-Teruel, A., … Flores, P. (2010). Impulsivity characterization in the roman high- and low-avoidance rat strains: Behavioral and neurochemical differences. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35, 11981208. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2009.224 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mowrer, O. H., & Lamoreaux, R.R. (1946). Fear as an intervening variable in avoidance conditioning. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 39, 2950. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0060150 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oliveras, I., Sánchez-González, A., Piludu, M. A., Gerbolés, C., Río-Álamos, C., Tobeña, A., & Fernández-Teruel, A. (2016). Divergent effects of isolation rearing on prepulse inhibition, activity, anxiety and hippocampal-dependent memory in Roman high- and low-avoidance rats: A putative model of schizophrenia-relevant features. Behavioural Brain Research, 314, 615. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2016.07.047 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oliveras, I., Sánchez-González, A., Sampedro-Viana, D., Piludu, M.A., Río-Alamos, C., Giorgi, O., … Fernández-Teruel, A. (2017). Differential effects of antipsychotic and propsychotic drugs on prepulse inhibition and locomotor activity in Roman high- (RHA) and low-avoidance (RLA) rats. Psychopharmacology, 234, 957975. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-017-4534-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oliveras, I., Cañete, T., Sampedro-Viana, D., Río-Álamos, C., Tobeña, A., Corda, M. G., … Fernández-Teruel, A. (2023). Neurobehavioral profiles of six genetically-based rat models of schizophrenia related symptoms. Current Neuropharmacology, 21, 19341952. https://doi.org/10.2174/1570159X21666230221093644 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Río-Álamos, C., Oliveras, I., Piludu, M. A., Gerbolés, C., Cañete, T., Blázquez, G., … Fernández-Teruel, A. (2017). Neonatal handling enduringly decreases anxiety and stress responses and reduces hippocampus and amygdala volume in a genetic model of differential anxiety: Behavioral-volumetric associations in the Roman rat strains. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 27, 146158. https://doi.org/10.1016/j CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Río-Álamos, C., Piludu, M. A., Gerbolés, C., Barroso, D., Oliveras, I., Sánchez-González, A., … Fernández-Teruel, A. (2019). Volumetric brain differences between the Roman rat strains: Neonatal handling effects, sensorimotor gating and working memory. Behavioural Brain Research, 361, 7485. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2018.12.033 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sabariego, M., Morón, I., Gómez, M. J., Donaire, R., Tobeña, A., Fernández-Teruel, A., … Torres, C. (2013). Incentive loss and hippocampal gene expression in inbred Roman high- (RHA-I) and Roman low- (RLA-I) avoidance rats. Behavioural Brain Research, 257, 6270. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2013.09.025 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sallés, J., López de Jesús, M., Goñi, O., Fernández-Teruel, A., Driscoll, P., Tobeña, A., & Escorihuela, R.M. (2001). Transmembrane signaling through phospholipase C in cortical and hippocampal membranes of psychogenetically selected rat lines. Psychopharmacology, 154, 115125. https://doi.org/10.1007/s002130000621 Google ScholarPubMed
Sampedro-Viana, D., Cañete, T., Sanna, F., Soley, B., Giorgi, O., Corda, M. G., … Fernández-Teruel, A. (2021). Decreased social interaction in the RHA rat model of schizophrenia-relevant features: Modulation by neonatal handling. Behavioural Processes, 188, 104397. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2021.104397 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sampedro-Viana, D., Cañete, T., Sanna, F., Oliveras, I., Lavín, V., Torrecilla, P., … Fernández-Teruel, A. (2023). Atypical antipsychotics attenuate MK801-induced social withdrawal and hyperlocomotion in the RHA rat model of schizophrenia-relevant features. Psychopharmacology, Jul 13, 2023. Online ahead of print. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-023-06411-w CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sánchez-González, A., Oliveras, I., Río-Álamos, C., Piludu, M. A., Gerbolés, C., Tapias-Espinosa, C., … Fernández-Teruel, A. (2020). Dissociation between schizophrenia-relevant behavioral profiles and volumetric brain measures after long-lasting social isolation in Roman rats. Neuroscience Research, 155, 4355. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neures.2019.07.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sánchez-González, A., Thougaard, E., Tapias-Espinosa, C., Cañete, T., Sampedro-Viana, D., Saunders, J. M., … Fernández-Teruel, A. (2021). Increased thin-spine density in frontal cortex pyramidal neurons in a genetic rat model of schizophrenia-relevant features. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 44, 7991. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2021.01.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Satinder, K. P. (1971). Genotype-dependent effects of d-amphetamine sulphate and caffeine on escape-avoidance behavior of rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 76, 359364. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0031385 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwegler, H., Pilz, P.K.D., Koch, M., Fendt, M., Linke, R., & Driscoll, P. (1997). The acoustic startle response in inbred Roman high- and low-avoidance rats. Behavior Genetics, 27, 579582. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021465217299 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegel, J., Sisson, D. F., & Driscoll, P. (1993). Augmenting and reducing of visually evoked potentials in Roman high- and low-avoidance rats. Physiology and Behavior, 54, 707711. https://doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(93)90080-Y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegel, J., & Driscoll, P. (1996). Recent developments in an animal model of visual evoked potential augmenting/reducing and sensation seeking behavior. Neuropsychobiology, 34, 130135. https://doi.org/10.1159/000119304 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sønderstrup, M., Batiuk, M. Y., Mantas, P., Tapias-Espinosa, C., Oliveras, I., Cañete, T., … Aznar, S. (2023). A maturational shift in the frontal cortex synaptic transcriptional landscape underlies schizophrenia-relevant behavioural traits: A congenital rat model. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 74, 3246. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2023.05.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steimer, T., & Driscoll, P. (2003). Divergent stress responses and coping styles in psychogenetically selected Roman high-(RHA) and low-(RLA) avoidance rats: Behavioural, neuroendocrine and developmental aspects. Stress, 6, 87100. https://doi.org/10.1080/1025389031000111320 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tapias-Espinosa, C., Sánchez-González, A., Cañete, T., Sampedro-Viana, D., Castillo-Ruiz, M. D. M., Oliveras, I., … Fernández-Teruel, A. (2023). Decreased activation of parvalbumin interneurons in the medial prefrontal cortex in intact inbred Roman rats with schizophrenia-like reduced sensorimotor gating. Behavioural Brain Research, 437, 114113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2022.114113 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tapias-Espinosa, C., Río-Álamos, C., Sánchez-González, A., Oliveras, I., Sampedro-Viana, D., Castillo-Ruiz, M.D.M., … Fernández-Teruel, A. (2019). Schizophrenia-like reduced sensorimotor gating in intact inbred and outbred rats is associated with decreased medial prefrontal cortex activity and volume. Neuropsychopharmacology, 44, 19751984. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-019-0392-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Torres, C., Escarabajal, M.D., Cándido, A., de la Torre, L., Gómez, M.J., Maldonado, A., … Fernández-Teruel, A. (2007). One-way avoidance learning and diazepam in female roman high-avoidance and low-avoidance rats. Behavioral Pharmacology, 18, 251–153. https://doi.org/10.1097/FBP.0b013e328157f43c CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Torrubia, R., Ávila, C., Moltó, J., & Caseras, X. (2001). The sensitivity to punishment and sensitivity to reward questionnaire (SPSRQ) as a measure of Gray’s anxiety and impulsivity dimensions. Personality and Individual Differences, 31, 837862. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00183-5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tournier, B. B., Steimer, T., Millet, P., Moulin-Sallanon, M., Vallet, P., Ibañez, V., & Ginovart, N. (2013). Innately low D2 receptor availability is associated with high novelty-seeking and enhanced behavioural sensitization to amphetamine. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 16, 18191834. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1461145713000205 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weaver, I. C., D’Alessio, A. C., Brown, S. E., Hellstrom, I. C., Dymov, S., Sharma, S., … Meaney, M. J. (2007). The transcription factor nerve growth factor-inducible protein a mediates epigenetic programming: Altering epigenetic marks by immediate-early genes. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 17561768. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4164-06.2007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zuckerman, M., Murtaugh, T., & Siegel, J. (1974). Sensation seeking and cortical augmenting-reducing. Psychophysiology, 11, 535542. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1974.tb01109.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Two-way active avoidance acquisition in the shuttle box (Medical Psychology Unit, Dept. Psychiatry & Forensic Medicine, Autonomous University of Barcelona). The rat is crossing from the right compartment to the opposite one to escape/avoid the shock but is also faced with the memory of having to escape from the left compartment on the previous trial. For further explanation see text.

Figure 1

Table 1. Overview of neurobehavioral traits of RLA and RHA rats, correspondence with RDoC domains, the HiTOP psychopathology spectra (Michelini et al., 2021) and the “Big Five” and “Eysenck-Gray” personality factors