Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-jr42d Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T20:45:02.464Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Can neuroscience help to understand narcissism? A systematic review of an emerging field

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 May 2021

Emanuel Jauk*
Affiliation:
Clinical Psychology and Behavioral Neuroscience, Faculty of Psychology, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany Department of Psychology, University of Graz, Graz, Austria
Philipp Kanske
Affiliation:
Clinical Psychology and Behavioral Neuroscience, Faculty of Psychology, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
*
Author for correspondence: Emanuel Jauk, Email: emanuel.jauk@tu-dresden.de
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Narcissism is a Janusian personality construct, associated with both grandiose self-assuredness and dominance, as well as vulnerable insecurity and reactivity. Central questions of intra- and interpersonal functioning in narcissism are still a matter of debate. Neuroscience could help to understand the paradoxical patterns of experience and behavior beyond the limitations of self-reports. We provide a systematic review of 34 neuroscience studies on grandiose, vulnerable, pathological narcissism, and Narcissistic Personality Disorder (NPD), spanning experimental investigations of intra- and interpersonal mechanisms, research on neurophysiological and neuroendocrine aspects of baseline function, and brain structural correlates. While neuroscience has scarcely directly studied vulnerable narcissism, grandiose narcissism is associated with heightened vigilance to ego threat and stress responses following ego threat, as well as heightened stress indicators in baseline measures. Such responses are not commonly observed in self-reports, highlighting the potential of neuroscience to augment our understanding of self-regulatory dynamics in narcissism. Interpersonal functioning is characterized by deficits in social–affective processes. Both involve altered activity within the salience network, pointing to a double dissociation regarding the expression of narcissism and self/other oriented situational focus. Findings are summarized in an integrative model providing testable hypotheses for future research along with methodological recommendations.

Type
Review Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s) 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

1. Narcissism: Concepts and Operationalizations

The phenomenon of narcissism has been described for around 2000 years when the myth of Narcissus was documented in ancient Greece. In this myth, the beautiful young hunter Narcissus rejected the love of the nymph Echo, which is why he was deemed by the gods to fall in love with his own mirror image. His self-centeredness ultimately led Narcissus to the fate of a tragic death. Thus, on a metaphorical level, this early myth already reflects two aspects of narcissism that are currently being extensively studied, namely grandiose and vulnerable narcissism. The ancient concept of narcissism was picked up and refined by psychodynamic theorists, who regarded narcissism as both a self-regulatory mechanism and a personality disposition (Freud, Reference Freud and Strachey1914; Kernberg, Reference Kernberg1975; Kohut, Reference Kohut1977), and was first included as a personality disorder in the third edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (American Psychiatric Association, 1980). At the same time, the concept of narcissism was also adapted for personality research within general, nonclinical personality variation (Raskin & Hall, Reference Raskin and Hall1979).

It was not until the 2000s, however, that research on narcissism attracted a broader scientific and public interest, which is presumably due to cultural changes in the new millennium. Popular writings such as Jean Twenge’s (Reference Twenge2006) “Generation Me” or Twenge and Campbell’s (Reference Twenge and Campbell2009) “Narcissism Epidemic” render a rather pessimistic picture of western societies becoming increasingly self-focused and entitled, while being less bound to social and moral norms. Large-scale studies of trait changes initiated a lively scientific debate on whether narcissism scores increase in western cultures (Barry & Lee-Rowland, Reference Barry and Lee-Rowland2015; Donnellan, Trzesniewski, & Robins, Reference Donnellan, Trzesniewski and Robins2009; Twenge, Konrath, Foster, Campbell, & Bushman, Reference Twenge, Konrath, Foster, Keith Campbell and Bushman2008; Wetzel et al., Reference Wetzel, Brown, Hill, Chung, Robins and Roberts2017) and are dependent on cultural orientation (Jauk, Breyer, Kanske, & Wakabayashi, Reference Jauk, Breyer, Kanske and Wakabayashi2021; Vater et al., Reference Vater, Moritz and Roepke2018). The strong interest in the past decades also fueled a thorough research program on what exactly constitutes narcissism as a personality trait and as a clinical disorder, what are its antecedents, and its personal and interpersonal consequences. To this end, only a few personality constructs have received attention from so many different perspectives as is the case for narcissism. Different but overlapping theories have been put forward from social/personality psychology, clinical psychology, and psychiatry (Cain et al., Reference Cain, Pincus and Ansell2008).

There is now an emerging consensus that narcissism is multifaceted in nature and different expressions of the phenomenon can be discerned (see Figure 1). While all forms of narcissism are characterized by pronounced feelings of self-importance and entitlement, grandiose and vulnerable narcissism have been recognized as separable yet related expressions of narcissism (Krizan & Herlache, Reference Krizan and Herlache2018; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Lynam, McCain, Few, Crego, Widiger and Campbell2016; Weiss et al., Reference Weiss, Campbell, Lynam and Miller2019). These manifest in distinct experiential and behavioral patterns of either self-assuredness and dominance, or self-consciousness and withdrawal. Despite their seemingly opposing manifestations, both share overtly or covertly expressed attitudes of being special and entitled to special privileges (Krizan & Herlache, Reference Krizan and Herlache2018; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Lynam, McCain, Few, Crego, Widiger and Campbell2016; Pincus & Lukowitsky, Reference Pincus and Lukowitsky2010). The overarching mechanism of narcissistic functioning is the maintenance of an inflated self by means of characteristic intra- and interpersonal self-regulatory strategies (Back et al., Reference Back, Küfner, Dufner, Gerlach, Rauthmann and Denissen2013; Morf & Rhodewalt, Reference Morf and Rhodewalt2001).

Figure 1. Structural model of narcissism, adapted and synthesized from the Trifurcated Model (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Lynam, McCain, Few, Crego, Widiger and Campbell2016; Weiss et al., Reference Weiss, Campbell, Lynam and Miller2019) and the Narcissism Spectrum Model (Krizan & Herlache, Reference Krizan and Herlache2018).

Also, in recent years, neuroscience has begun to unveil aspects of narcissism that are not commonly apparent in self-report research. Neuroscience methods offer a particularly promising view into narcissistic personality functioning as they might be less influenced by cognitive biases and response styles that can render pure self-report studies problematic. These include, among others, overestimation of emotion-related and social competencies in narcissism (Ames & Kammrath, Reference Ames and Kammrath2004; Jauk, Freudenthaler, et al., Reference Jauk, Freudenthaler and Neubauer2016; John & Robins, Reference John and Robins1994; Lobbestael et al., Reference Lobbestael, de Bruin, Kok and Voncken2016; Mota et al., Reference Mota, Leckelt, Geukes, Nestler, Humberg, Schröder-Abé, Schmukle and Back2019; Zajenkowski et al., Reference Zajenkowski, Maciantowicz, Szymaniak and Urban2018), which are directly relevant to the studies reviewed here.

The goal of this review is to summarize the current neuroscience investigations on narcissism and integrate them with conceptual models from personality and clinical psychology to highlight the possible contributions of neuroscience to the understanding of narcissism. In the first part, we will provide an overview of the different conceptions of narcissism and their commonalities, differences, and manifestations in different aspects of experience and behavior. We will then, in the second part, review neuroscience investigations of the different conceptions of narcissism, and highlight their potential to gain a deeper understanding of the respective constructs. Finally, in the third and fourth parts, we provide a conceptual integration and a model of the neural bases of self-related and interpersonal processes in narcissism, and close with recommendations for future research in the personality neuroscience of narcissism.

We structure our review in intrapersonal and interpersonal aspects of narcissism, as these are conceived as the two major self-regulatory pathways in research on narcissism (Back et al., Reference Back, Küfner, Dufner, Gerlach, Rauthmann and Denissen2013; Morf & Rhodewalt, Reference Morf and Rhodewalt2001), and more generally, two major dimensions of personality functioning in prevailing models of personality pathology (Alternative Model for Personality Disorders [AMPD] in the DSM-5; see Table 1; Bender, Morey, & Skodol, Reference Bender, Morey and Skodol2011; Personality Disorders Model in the ICD-11, Tyrer, Mulder, Kim, & Crawford, Reference Tyrer, Mulder, Kim and Crawford2019). Intrapersonal functioning, following the DSM-5 AMPD, concerns aspects of identity – a stable and coherent sense of self, stable self-esteem, and capacity to experience, tolerate, and regulate affect – and self-regulation – goal pursuit, utilization of internal standards of behavior, and self-reflection. Interpersonal functioning concerns aspects of empathy Footnote 1 – understanding others’ experiences and motivations, tolerance of differing perspectives, understanding of social causality – and intimacy – connection with others, desire and capacity for closeness, and cooperative behavior (American Psychiatric Association, 2013; Bender et al., Reference Bender, Morey and Skodol2011).

Table 1. Dimensions of intra- and interpersonal functioning

Personality functioning model is adapted from the DSM-5 Alternative Model of Personality Disorders (AMPD; American Psychiatric Association, 2013; Bender et al., Reference Bender, Morey and Skodol2011). The term “social cognition” was added by us based on prevailing neuroscience models (see section 2.1).

1.1 Grandiose narcissism

When hearing the term “narcissism”, most people think of exaggerated self-worth, feelings of superiority, admiration seeking, entitlement, and arrogance (Buss & Chiodo, Reference Buss and Chiodo1991). These common associations actually very closely match the definition of grandiose narcissism as a personality trait, which encompasses self-importance and entitlement – the antagonistic core of narcissism – alongside extraverted and socially dominant behavior (Krizan & Herlache, Reference Krizan and Herlache2018; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Lynam, McCain, Few, Crego, Widiger and Campbell2016).

The concept of narcissism as a trait was originally devised from the diagnostic criteria for Narcissistic Personality Disorder (NPD) in the third edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (American Psychiatric Association, 1980), and adapted for the assessment of narcissism in the general, nonclinical population in the Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI; Raskin & Hall, Reference Raskin and Hall1979, Reference Raskin and Hall1981). The original criteria included (1) a grandiose sense of self-importance, (2) fantasies of unlimited success, power, etc., (3) exhibitionism, (4) responding to criticism with either indifference or rage, (5) entitlement, (6) exploitativeness, (7) relationships vacillating between idealization and devaluation, and (8) lack of empathy. Importantly, these criteria focus strongly on narcissistic grandiosity, and only implicitly tap into narcissistic vulnerability (see section 1.2). Items of the NPI encompass statements such as “I think I am a special person”, “I am more capable than other people”, or “I have a natural talent for influencing people” (Raskin & Terry, Reference Raskin and Terry1988). The NPI has become the most widely used measure of grandiose narcissism, the total score displays good reliability and validity with respect to expert ratings (Miller et al., Reference Miller, McCain, Lynam, Few, Gentile, MacKillop and Campbell2014), and the vast majority of the neuroscience studies on grandiose narcissism reviewed here used the NPI as an indicator.

1.1.1 Intrapersonal characteristics

Individuals high in grandiose narcissism report high self-esteem (Campbell, Reference Campbell2001) and score high on different indicators of mental health (Sedikides et al., Reference Sedikides, Rudich, Gregg, Kumashiro and Rusbult2004) as well as life satisfaction (Egan et al., Reference Egan, Chan and Shorter2014). They display marked self-enhancement in various domains, which means that they rate themselves as more intelligent or attractive, for instance, than it might be expected on the basis of objective tests or others’ ratings (Campbell et al., Reference Campbell, Rudich and Sedikides2002; Gabriel et al., Reference Gabriel, Critelli and Ee1994; Grijalva & Zhang, Reference Grijalva and Zhang2016; Grosz et al., Reference Grosz, Lösch and Back2017). Self-enhancement is also evident regarding social and emotional capacities in grandiose narcissism (Ames & Kammrath, Reference Ames and Kammrath2004; Jauk, Freudenthaler, et al., Reference Jauk, Freudenthaler and Neubauer2016; John & Robins, Reference John and Robins1994; Lobbestael et al., Reference Lobbestael, de Bruin, Kok and Voncken2016; Mota et al., Reference Mota, Leckelt, Geukes, Nestler, Humberg, Schröder-Abé, Schmukle and Back2019; Zajenkowski et al., Reference Zajenkowski, Maciantowicz, Szymaniak and Urban2018), which indicates that individuals high in grandiose narcissism hold pronounced positive illusions about their intra- and interpersonal emotional abilities. In line with their superior and independent self-construal, individuals high in grandiose narcissism report discomfort only in light of achievement failure, but not in the light of social rejection (Besser & Priel, Reference Besser and Priel2010). Taken together, self-descriptions of individuals high in grandiose narcissism render a picture of highly self-assured individuals, which is most likely due to the agentic-extraverted aspects of narcissism (Kaufman et al., Reference Kaufman, Weiss, Miller and Campbell2020).

However, behavioral and experimental studies also unveil fragilities in the seemingly overly positive grandiose-narcissistic personality. These encompass, for instance, feelings of self-alienation or a weak sense of self (Kaufman et al., Reference Kaufman, Weiss, Miller and Campbell2020), proneness to addictive behaviors (Jauk & Dieterich, Reference Jauk and Dieterich2019), and heightened variability in mood and self-esteem (Geukes et al., Reference Geukes, Nestler, Hutteman, Dufner, Küfner, Egloff, Denissen and Back2017; Rhodewalt et al., Reference Rhodewalt, Madrian and Cheney1998). Particularly, experiencing negative interpersonal events can lead to sudden declines in self-esteem (Zuckerman & O’Loughlin, Reference Zuckerman and O’Loughlin2009). Variability in self-esteem is more related to antagonistic (i.e., disagreeable) than agentic (i.e., extraverted, socially dominant) aspects of narcissism (Geukes et al., Reference Geukes, Nestler, Hutteman, Dufner, Küfner, Egloff, Denissen and Back2017).

1.1.2 Interpersonal characteristics

Individuals high in grandiose narcissism are extraverted, socially bold (Holtzman, Vazire, & Mehl, Reference Holtzman, Vazire and Mehl2010), and also charming (e.g., Back, Schmukle, & Egloff, Reference Back, Schmukle and Egloff2010). Accordingly, they can be highly successful in social contexts, which is, for instance, evident in the domain of interpersonal attraction (Dufner et al., Reference Dufner, Rauthmann, Czarna and Denissen2013; Jauk, Neubauer, et al., Reference Jauk, Neubauer, Mairunteregger, Pemp, Sieber and Rauthmann2016). This picture conforms to the “happy face” of grandiose narcissism (Rose, Reference Rose2002), which is most likely due to its agentic-extraverted aspects (Kaufman et al., Reference Kaufman, Weiss, Miller and Campbell2020). However, the short-term social success of individuals high in grandiose narcissism is also likely to be accompanied by long-term interpersonal problems, such as, for instance, in romantic relationships (Wurst et al., Reference Wurst, Gerlach, Dufner, Rauthmann, Grosz, Küfner and Back2017). Interpersonal problems associated with grandiose narcissism are thought to arise from heightened self-focus and reduced empathy (Morf & Rhodewalt, Reference Morf and Rhodewalt2001), both of which are perceived as core pillars of the grandiose narcissism construct and are more related to its antagonistic aspects (Kaufman et al., Reference Kaufman, Weiss, Miller and Campbell2020). Though a number of studies addressed the relationship between grandiose narcissism and empathy (e.g., Giammarco & Vernon, Reference Giammarco and Vernon2014; Wai & Tiliopoulos, Reference Wai and Tiliopoulos2012), the exact nature of lowered empathy in narcissism is still a matter of debate (cf. Baskin-Sommers et al., Reference Baskin-Sommers, Krusemark and Ronningstam2014). Recent behavioral research points into the direction of a reduced propensity to share others’ emotional states, rather than a reduced ability to do so (Hepper et al., Reference Hepper, Hart and Sedikides2014).

1.2 Vulnerable narcissism

Besides grandiose narcissism, increasing attention has been paid to vulnerable narcissism in the past decades (e.g., Fossati et al., Reference Fossati, Borroni, Grazioli, Dornetti, Marcassoli, Maffei and Cheek2009). While vulnerable aspects of narcissism have long been hypothesized to be overt or covert parts of the broader phenomenon of narcissism (Pincus & Lukowitsky, Reference Pincus and Lukowitsky2010), the social/personality study of narcissism delineated vulnerable narcissism as an independent trait – the “second face” of narcissism (Wink, Reference Wink1991). Vulnerable narcissism is uncorrelated to grandiose narcissism at a trait level in the general population, but might blend into grandiosity at high levels of grandiose narcissism (Jauk & Kaufman, Reference Jauk and Kaufman2018; Jauk, Weigle, et al., Reference Jauk, Weigle, Lehmann, Benedek and Neubauer2017). To date, only two neuroscience studies explicitly addressed vulnerable narcissism in that they used independent measures to assess this construct. Nevertheless, the construct may also be relevant to the interpretation of studies on grandiose and pathological narcissism as well as NPD.

Individuals high in vulnerable narcissism typically present very differently from those high in grandiose narcissism: vulnerable narcissistic individuals are anxious, defensive, and avoidant (Hart et al., Reference Hart, Adams, Burton and Tortoriello2017; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Price, Gentile, Lynam and Campbell2012). Yet, despite the outward presentation of self-consciousness, those high in vulnerable narcissism also share the common antagonistic core of entitlement and self-importance (Krizan & Herlache, Reference Krizan and Herlache2018; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Lynam, McCain, Few, Crego, Widiger and Campbell2016). A commonly used measure of vulnerable narcissism, the Hypersensitive Narcissism Scale (HSNS; Hendin & Cheek, Reference Hendin and Cheek1997), encompasses items such as “I easily become wrapped up in my own interests and forget the existence of others” or “I am secretely [sic!] ‘put out’ when other people come to me with their troubles, asking me for my time and sympathy“ (p. 592).

1.2.1 Intrapersonal characteristics

While grandiose narcissism is closely tied to extraversion, vulnerable narcissism is closely related to neuroticism and – depending on the scale used – introversion (Jauk, Weigle, et al., Reference Jauk, Weigle, Lehmann, Benedek and Neubauer2017; Kaufman et al., Reference Kaufman, Weiss, Miller and Campbell2020; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Lynam, McCain, Few, Crego, Widiger and Campbell2016). Consequently, vulnerable narcissism is linked to experiencing less positive and more negative affect (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Hoffman, Gaughan, Gentile, Maples and Keith Campbell2011, Reference Miller, Lynam, Vize, Crowe, Sleep, Maples-Keller, Few and Campbell2018), and individuals high in vulnerable narcissism display a variety of internalizing symptoms such as anxiety and depression (Euler et al., Reference Euler, Stöbi, Sowislo, Ritzler, Huber, Lang, Wrege and Walter2018; Kaufman et al., Reference Kaufman, Weiss, Miller and Campbell2020; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Hoffman, Gaughan, Gentile, Maples and Keith Campbell2011, Reference Miller, Lynam, Vize, Crowe, Sleep, Maples-Keller, Few and Campbell2018). They report lower self-esteem (Brookes, Reference Brookes2015; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Lynam, Vize, Crowe, Sleep, Maples-Keller, Few and Campbell2018; Rose, Reference Rose2002) and lower life satisfaction (Rose, Reference Rose2002). Taken together, psychological and behavioral correlates of vulnerable narcissism clearly point to the “unhappy” face of narcissism (Rose, Reference Rose2002).

1.2.2 Interpersonal characteristics

Contrary to those high in grandiose narcissism, those high in vulnerable narcissism are more sensitive to social rejection than achievement failure (Besser & Priel, Reference Besser and Priel2010). Paradoxically, while individuals high in vulnerable narcissism tend to put great weight on being accepted by others, they also display lowered empathy (Lannin et al., Reference Lannin, Guyll, Krizan, Madon and Cornish2014) and compassion (Luchner et al., Reference Luchner, Houston, Walker and Alex Houston2011) for others. Vulnerable narcissism is further related to lower self-report perspective-taking (Honeycutt et al., Reference Honeycutt, Pence, Gearhart and Besser2014) and emotion understanding performance (Vonk et al., Reference Vonk, Zeigler-Hill, Ewing, Mercer and Noser2015). Taken together, the pattern of interpersonal dynamics in vulnerable narcissism points to the desire of being accepted by others, accompanied by the fear of being rejected, which is in line with higher attachment anxiety in vulnerable narcissism (Rohmann et al., Reference Rohmann, Neumann, Herner and Bierhoff2012). Excessive self-focus and impaired interpersonal functioning likely amplify this fear in terms of negative social outcomes.

1.3 Pathological narcissism

Pathological narcissism, as an operationally defined and quantifiable construct, was introduced by Pincus and colleagues in recognition of the problem that previously existing self-report measures of (grandiose) narcissism mostly focused on adaptive qualities, and maladaptive as well as vulnerable aspects might have been underrepresented (Pincus et al., Reference Pincus, Ansell, Pimentel, Cain, Wright and Levy2009). Consequently, pathological narcissism is defined as comprising both grandiose and vulnerable aspects (Pincus et al., Reference Pincus, Ansell, Pimentel, Cain, Wright and Levy2009; Pincus & Lukowitsky, Reference Pincus and Lukowitsky2010). While these seemingly opposing experiential and behavioral tendencies may appear hard to reconcile at the first glance, Pincus and Lukowitsky (Reference Pincus and Lukowitsky2010) assert that grandiose and vulnerable self-states can fluctuate or co-occur in highly narcissistic individuals (Wright & Edershile, Reference Wright and Edershile2018), which is illustrated vividly in clinical case descriptions (Pincus et al., Reference Pincus, Cain and Wright2014). The current standard measure of pathological narcissism is the Pathological Narcissism Inventory (PNI; Pincus et al., Reference Pincus, Ansell, Pimentel, Cain, Wright and Levy2009). It encompasses facets related more to narcissistic grandiosity, namely entitlement rage (anger when entitled expectations are not met), exploitativeness (manipulative behavior in pursuit of own goals), grandiose fantasy (imagining grandiose self), and self-sacrificing self-enhancement (self-enhancement by means of seemingly altruistic behavior), as well as facets related more to narcissistic vulnerability, which are contingent self-esteem (dependence upon others’ regard), hiding the self (not showing own needs and weaknesses), and devaluing (devaluing others who do not meet their own needs). The grandiose and vulnerable factors are intrinsically related in the PNI (Pincus et al., Reference Pincus, Ansell, Pimentel, Cain, Wright and Levy2009). This makes it conceptually and empirically different from scales designed exclusively for the assessment of either grandiose or vulnerable narcissism, which are unrelated in the general population (Jauk, Weigle, et al., Reference Jauk, Weigle, Lehmann, Benedek and Neubauer2017).

1.3.1 Intrapersonal characteristics

Individuals high in pathological narcissism report, among others, lower self-esteem, higher aggression, and higher shame (Morf et al., Reference Morf, Schürch, Küfner, Siegrist, Vater, Back, Mestel and Schröder-Abé2017; Pincus et al., Reference Pincus, Ansell, Pimentel, Cain, Wright and Levy2009), higher psychological distress, depressive symptoms, and lower life satisfaction (Morf et al., Reference Morf, Schürch, Küfner, Siegrist, Vater, Back, Mestel and Schröder-Abé2017), as well as higher self-harming behavior (Dawood et al., Reference Dawood, Schroder, Donnellan and Pincus2018). Behavioral studies of pathological narcissism found more positive intrapersonal and future-oriented thoughts, but more negative thoughts in general (Kanske, Sharifi, Smallwood, Dziobek, & Singer, Reference Kanske, Sharifi, Smallwood, Dziobek and Singer2017). This reflects two important aspects of pathological narcissism, namely grandiose fantasy on the one hand, and on the other hand, vulnerable or devaluing aspects (Pincus et al., Reference Pincus, Ansell, Pimentel, Cain, Wright and Levy2009).

1.3.2 Interpersonal characteristics

Pathological narcissism is associated with fearful or preoccupied attachment (both characterized by a negative model of the self; Fossati, Feeney, Pincus, Borroni, & Maffei, Reference Fossati, Feeney, Pincus, Borroni and Maffei2015; Morf et al., Reference Morf, Schürch, Küfner, Siegrist, Vater, Back, Mestel and Schröder-Abé2017) and an array of self-report interpersonal problems ranging from cold dominant to overly nurturant behavior (Pincus et al., Reference Pincus, Ansell, Pimentel, Cain, Wright and Levy2009). In daily interactions, individuals high in pathological narcissism are reactive to both status threat and rejection sensitivity (Roche et al., Reference Roche, Pincus, Conroy, Hyde and Ram2013). Pathological narcissism goes along with serious treatment problems in clinical samples, including suicidal ideation and behavior (Pincus et al., Reference Pincus, Ansell, Pimentel, Cain, Wright and Levy2009). Pathological narcissism is negatively related to dispositional empathy (Pincus et al., Reference Pincus, Ansell, Pimentel, Cain, Wright and Levy2009), which can be explained along the line that both of its constituent features, grandiosity, and vulnerability, are accompanied by lowered empathy (see sections 1.1.2 and 1.2.2). Perspective-taking is lowered in pathological narcissism in social decision-making (Böckler et al., Reference Böckler, Sharifi, Kanske, Dziobek and Singer2017), but not in a self-report measure (Morf et al., Reference Morf, Schürch, Küfner, Siegrist, Vater, Back, Mestel and Schröder-Abé2017). This indicates that pathological narcissism goes along with similar self-serving bias as grandiose narcissism when it comes to interpersonal abilities (see section 1.1.1). Taken together, the experiential and behavioral findings related to pathological narcissism point to a pattern of psychological maladjustment characterized by opposing tendencies of grandiosity and vulnerability, significant personal distress, and maladaptive interpersonal behavior.

1.4 Narcissistic personality disorder

The diagnostic category of NPD emerged in the DSM-III mainly from psychodynamic theory and case studies (Ronningstam, Reference Ronningstam2009). Since its first inclusion, the diagnostic criteria (mentioned above) have been revised. In the latest edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association, 2013), the categorical diagnosis of NPD is defined by at least five out of the following nine criteria: (1) grandiose sense of self-importance, (2) fantasies of unlimited success, power, etc., (3) thinks he/she is special and unique, (4) requires excessive admiration, (5) entitlement, (6) exploitativeness, (7) lack of empathy, (8) envious/thinks others are envious, and (9) arrogance. Contrary to the developments in the research field, the definition of NPD in the DSM-5 is strongly focused on grandiosity (Pincus & Lukowitsky, Reference Pincus and Lukowitsky2010; Ronningstam, Reference Ronningstam2009) and might – in its present form – best be understood as an extreme manifestation of grandiose narcissism, which is supported by concurrent validity evidence (Miller et al., Reference Miller, McCain, Lynam, Few, Gentile, MacKillop and Campbell2014). This does not mean that vulnerable aspect cannot accompany grandiosity (which is also explicitly acknowledged in the DSM), but they do not constitute a necessary condition for an NPD diagnosis. For the purpose of the present review, it is most important to note that the terms pathological narcissism and NPD, despite their similar connotation, cannot be used interchangeably. While pathological narcissism, sensu Pincus and Lukowitsky (Reference Pincus and Lukowitsky2010), refers to concurrent grandiosity and vulnerability, NPD is a clinical diagnosis for individuals with extreme levels of grandiosity, which may or may not be accompanied by vulnerability.

The prevalence of NPD is generally considered low, as compared to other mental disorders. Prevalence estimates in the general population range between 0 and 1% (Roepke & Vater, Reference Roepke and Vater2014) or 0–5% (Ronningstam, Reference Ronningstam2009), the DSM-5 provides an estimated range between 0 and 6.2% in community samples (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Systematic empirical research on NPD is generally very sparse, putatively because patients are hard to find, and hard to recruit. We summarize some of the available evidence in the following.

1.4.1 Intrapersonal characteristics

One of the most prevailing notions on NPD is the view that outward grandiosity is a façade to hide an underlying, fragile self (Akhtar, Reference Akhtar1989), also known as the “mask” model of narcissism. One hypothesis that can be derived from such a mask model view posits that high explicit self-esteem should be accompanied by low implicit self-esteem in narcissism (cf. Kuchynka & Bosson, Reference Kuchynka, Bosson, Hermann, Brunell and Foster2018). Empirical studies, however, did not find evidence for lower implicit self-esteem among NPD patients (Marissen et al., Reference Marissen, Brouwer, Hiemstra, Deen and Franken2016; Vater et al., Reference Vater, Ritter, Schröder-Abé, Schütz, Lammers, Bosson and Roepke2013). On the contrary, one of the studies unexpectedly found lower explicit self-esteem of NPD patients as compared to healthy controls (Vater et al., Reference Vater, Ritter, Schröder-Abé, Schütz, Lammers, Bosson and Roepke2013). It has to be noted, though, that NPD patients do not commonly seek treatment until they experience a major breakdown, which might explain their lowered explicit self-esteem (Vater et al., Reference Vater, Ritter, Schröder-Abé, Schütz, Lammers, Bosson and Roepke2013).

1.4.2 Interpersonal characteristics

Reduced empathy is one of the diagnostic criteria of NPD. Consequently, NPD patients show less mirroring of emotions, and less emotional contagion than healthy controls (Ritter et al., Reference Ritter, Dziobek, Preißler, Rüter, Vater, Fydrich and Roepke2011). The picture is more diverse when it comes to measures of perspective-taking, also referred to as Theory of Mind (Frith & Frith, Reference Frith and Frith2005). One study found that NPD patients report lower perspective-taking, but do not differ from healthy controls on performance measures (Ritter et al., Reference Ritter, Dziobek, Preißler, Rüter, Vater, Fydrich and Roepke2011). However, a different study suggests that NPD patients perform worse than healthy controls on an emotion recognition task, and that they are unaware of this deficit (Marissen et al., Reference Marissen, Deen and Franken2012). Recently, a study on NPD patients reported lowered interview-assessed mindreading performance as compared to healthy controls, but similar performance to patients with other personality disorders (Bilotta et al., Reference Bilotta, Carcione, Fera, Moroni, Nicolò, Pedone, Pellecchia, Semerari and Colle2018). A review highlights the role of motivation-based disengagement (propensity) versus deficit-based (capacity) lack of interpersonal functions in NPD (Baskin-Sommers et al., Reference Baskin-Sommers, Krusemark and Ronningstam2014). It is argued that not all forms of reduced interpersonal functioning are created equal, and can either be a consequence of a marked self-focus despite generally intact social–affective and cognitive functions (reduced propensity), or also a lack of fundamental interpersonal skills (reduced capacity; ibid.).

The reduced interpersonal functioning of NPD patients can have significant consequences not only for people in their surroundings, but also for themselves. NPD patients with severe levels of pathology are known as being hard to treat (Kernberg, Reference Kernberg2007), and indeed, psychotherapists experience feeling unappreciated or devalued by these patients and report disengagement from the therapeutic process (Tanzilli et al., Reference Tanzilli, Colli, Muzi and Lingiardi2015).

1.5 Summary: grandiosity and vulnerability in narcissism

According to current models, narcissism entails both grandiose and vulnerable aspects, which manifest in experiential and behavioral patterns of self-assuredness and dominance on the one hand, and insecurity and reactivity on the other (see Figure 1; Krizan & Herlache, Reference Krizan and Herlache2018; Weiss et al., Reference Weiss, Campbell, Lynam and Miller2019). In the general population, self-reports of trait grandiosity and vulnerability are unrelated or only weakly positively related, which points to relatively independent personality configurations – sharing a common core of self-importance and entitlement/antagonism (Krizan & Herlache, Reference Krizan and Herlache2018; Weiss et al., Reference Weiss, Campbell, Lynam and Miller2019) – at a global level (Jauk & Kaufman, Reference Jauk and Kaufman2018; Jauk, Weigle, et al., Reference Jauk, Weigle, Lehmann, Benedek and Neubauer2017). However, it has been posited that those high in grandiosity can fluctuate between grandiose and vulnerable states (e.g., Wright & Edershile, Reference Wright and Edershile2018), which is evident in informant reports (Gore & Widiger, Reference Gore and Widiger2016), increases in correlations among trait measures of grandiosity and vulnerability with increasing grandiosity (Jauk & Kaufman, Reference Jauk and Kaufman2018; Jauk, Weigle, et al., Reference Jauk, Weigle, Lehmann, Benedek and Neubauer2017), and ecological momentary assessment of state grandiosity and vulnerability (Edershile & Wright, Reference Edershile and Wright2020). Grandiosity and vulnerability are further intrinsically related in measures assessing primarily maladaptive, pathological aspects of narcissism (such as the PNI; Pincus et al., Reference Pincus, Ansell, Pimentel, Cain, Wright and Levy2009), and vulnerable self-states can be observed in NPD (American Psychiatric Association, 2013; Ronningstam, Reference Ronningstam2009), a personality disorder characterized by extreme grandiosity (Miller et al., Reference Miller, McCain, Lynam, Few, Gentile, MacKillop and Campbell2014). Taken together, while grandiose and vulnerable narcissism can be described in terms of dissociable personality configurations at a trait level, there is also increasing evidence that these two “faces” of narcissism go hand in hand – particularly at high levels of grandiosity. An interesting question for the neuroscience of intrapersonal functioning in narcissism might thus be to identify conditions under which individuals high in grandiose narcissism display reactions which are indicative of vulnerability, even if not directly evident in self-reports. Another question concerns interpersonal functioning, which is altered in all expressions in narcissism discussed above. We elaborate on these questions in the following section 2.1.

2. Neuroscience of Narcissism

2.1 Current research questions for the neuroscience of narcissism

The overarching goal of this review is to highlight the possible contributions of neuroscience to the understanding of narcissism in its different expressions. Though self-report and behavioral research on narcissism have acquired a great deal of knowledge in the past decades, some questions still remain open. Broadly, these concern two fundamental aspects of personality functioning: (1) intrapersonal, self-related characteristics of narcissism and (2) interpersonal, other related characteristics of narcissism.

(1) It has long been hypothesized that narcissism, particularly grandiose narcissism and NPD as its clinical expression, entail vulnerable aspects, which need not necessarily be overtly expressed (e.g., Pincus & Lukowitsky, Reference Pincus and Lukowitsky2010). The mask model of narcissism posits that grandiosity is a façade to mask an underlying fragile self (Akhtar, Reference Akhtar1989); that is, that grandiosity compensates for underlying vulnerability. Whether or not grandiosity is indeed causally related to underlying vulnerability, as this model implies, clinicians working with narcissistic patients are inclined to agree with their coexistence (in correlational terms). In their seminal review, Pincus and Lukowitksy (Reference Pincus and Lukowitsky2010) assert that “many contemporary clinical experts on narcissistic personality disorder now recognize that grandiose self-states oscillate or co-occur with vulnerable self-states and affective dysregulation” (p. 428). Similarly, in her review on NPD, Ronningstam (Reference Ronningstam2009) states that: “the narcissistic individual may fluctuate between assertive grandiosity and vulnerability” (p. 113). Following this perspective, one might assume that individuals high in grandiose narcissism should hold implicit negative self-views along their explicit positive self-views (cf. Kuchynka & Bosson, Reference Kuchynka, Bosson, Hermann, Brunell and Foster2018). However, this view is not commonly supported by systematic research. Self-report studies on grandiose narcissism find positive correlations with explicit (Campbell et al., Reference Campbell, Rudich and Sedikides2002) and also implicit self-esteem (Campbell et al., Reference Campbell, Bosson, Goheen, Lakey and Kernis2007), and research on NPD suggests no difference in implicit self-esteem between NPD patients and controls (Marissen et al., Reference Marissen, Brouwer, Hiemstra, Deen and Franken2016; Vater et al., Reference Vater, Ritter, Schröder-Abé, Schütz, Lammers, Bosson and Roepke2013).

The question remains, thus, if and how negative self-views are represented in individuals high in grandiose narcissism, and neuroscience might help to shed light on the respective mechanisms. In the long run, this might also contribute to understanding state changes between grandiosity and vulnerability discussed above (e.g., Edershile & Wright, Reference Edershile and Wright2020). Neuroscience studies targeting intrapersonal functions can investigate the neurophysiological reactions to situations in which the individual is exposed to ego threat, for instance, in terms of achievement failure, social exclusion, or confrontation with self-referential stimuli. These might induce stronger involuntary stress responses in individuals with higher levels of narcissism (cf. Coleman et al., Reference Coleman, Pincus and Smyth2019). Indicators that are known to be sensitive to these responses include, among others, autonomic measures such as skin conductance (Jacobs et al., Reference Jacobs, Friedman, Parker, Tofler, Jimenez, Muller and Stone1994) or blood pressure (Ulrich-Lai & Herman, Reference Ulrich-Lai and Herman2009) as well as neuroendocrine markers such as cortisol level (Kirschbaum et al., Reference Kirschbaum, Pirke and Hellhammer1993). Using neuroimaging methods, activity within regions of the salience network (comprising the anterior insula [AI] and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex [dACC]; Bressler & Menon, Reference Bressler and Menon2010) has been associated with the processing of aversive stimuli that induce conflict, pain, or negative affect (de la Vega et al., Reference de la Vega, Chang, Banich, Wager and Yarkoni2016; Shackman et al., Reference Shackman, Salomons, Slagter, Fox, Winter and Davidson2011; Somerville et al., Reference Somerville, Heatherton and Kelley2006).Footnote 2 These indicators might thus unveil involuntary vulnerable reactions and are less prone to cognitive bias than pure self-report measures. Moreover, habitual experiential and behavioral tendencies related to narcissism might manifest in chronic functional alterations in the mentioned systems, which can be revealed by studies of baseline function.

(2) Another controversial aspect of narcissism concerns the exact nature of altered interpersonal functioning observed in different expressions of narcissism. Interpersonal functioning includes – alongside more complex constructs such as attachment (see Table 1) – fundamental social–affective and social–cognitive functions frequently termed empathy and perspective-taking. Empathy refers to sharing others’ affective and emotional states, whereas perspective-taking (or Theory of Mind) circumscribes the capacity to cognitively represent others’ mental states (de Vignemont & Singer, Reference de Vignemont and Singer2006; Frith & Frith, Reference Frith and Frith2005). While both are sometimes subsumed under empathy as an umbrella term (being also referred to as affective and cognitive empathy; e.g., Baron-Cohen & Wheelwright, Reference Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright2004), they can be selectively impaired and draw on distinct neural networks (Stietz et al., Reference Stietz, Jauk, Krach and Kanske2019; see also below). Importantly, empathy and perspective-taking display considerable state variation and can depend on contextual and also motivational factors (ibid.), raising the question of whether altered interpersonal functioning in narcissism is more a question of reduced capacity (lower ability) or propensity (lower engagement; cf. Baskin-Sommers et al., Reference Baskin-Sommers, Krusemark and Ronningstam2014).

Narcissistic individuals are known to display lowered empathy (indeed, limited empathy is among the defining criteria of grandiose narcissism; see section 1.1), and there is also evidence for impaired social cognition in NPD (Marissen et al., Reference Marissen, Deen and Franken2012). However, behavioral evidence suggests that limited empathy in narcissism might be more due to lower propensity than capacity (Hepper et al., Reference Hepper, Hart and Sedikides2014), and similar mechanisms are being discussed for putative perspective-taking deficits in NPD (Baskin-Sommers et al., Reference Baskin-Sommers, Krusemark and Ronningstam2014). It might thus be the case that altered interpersonal functioning in narcissistic individuals is more related to the consequences of a self-focused and antagonistic interpersonal style (Krizan & Herlache, Reference Krizan and Herlache2018; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Lynam, McCain, Few, Crego, Widiger and Campbell2016) than ability deficits per se. Again, neuroscience might help to gain a closer understanding of the mechanisms involved in interpersonal functioning in narcissism. A neuroscience approach seems particularly fruitful given that global self-reports of empathy and perspective-taking (as measured by the frequently used Interpersonal Reactivity Index, for instance; Davis, Reference Davis1983) display only low-to-moderate correlation with task-based ratings and corresponding neural activation (Hildebrandt et al., Reference Hildebrandt, Jauk, Lehmann, Maliske and Kanske2021), which points to differences between global self-perceptions of typical behavior and interpersonal capacities assessed under laboratory conditions. This gap might be even stronger for individuals high in narcissism, whose self-ratings of emotional competencies, in general, deviate substantially from task-based assessments (Jauk, Freudenthaler, et al., Reference Jauk, Freudenthaler and Neubauer2016; Mota et al., Reference Mota, Leckelt, Geukes, Nestler, Humberg, Schröder-Abé, Schmukle and Back2019).

Neuroscience research on interpersonal functioning has delineated empathy from perspective-taking; the two main capacities that enable access to others’ inner, mental states, thus, providing crucial ground for successful social interaction. Empathic reactions to others’ emotional states are commonly observed in the same regions that are activated during the firsthand experiences of these states (Kanske, Reference Kanske2018). Thus, for instance, empathy for another’s aversive experience, be it pain, disgust or unfair treatment, activates the AI and the (middle) anterior cingulate cortex (parts of the salience network; see above), which are also activated during firsthand aversive stimulation (Corradi-Dell’Acqua et al., Reference Corradi-Dell’Acqua, Tusche, Vuilleumier and Singer2016; Oosterwijk et al., Reference Oosterwijk, Snoek, Rotteveel, Barrett and Scholte2017). This supports the view that empathy for others’ emotional states might be regarded as an internalization of these states (Kanske, Reference Kanske2018), or in other words, we literally feel what others are feeling when empathizing. With respect to narcissism, this might make alexithymia, in terms of the general incapacity to experience emotions, a prime candidate as a mediating factor (Valdespino et al., Reference Valdespino, Antezana, Ghane and Richey2017).

Perspective-taking, or Theory of Mind, draws on a different neuronal network than empathy (Kanske, Böckler, Trautwein, & Singer, Reference Kanske, Böckler, Trautwein and Singer2015). This network comprises medial prefrontal regions, the temporoparietal junction, the precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex, as well as the temporal poles and superior temporal sulcus (Bzdok et al., Reference Bzdok, Schilbach, Vogeley, Schneider, Laird, Langner and Eickhoff2012; Molenberghs et al., Reference Molenberghs, Johnson, Henry and Mattingley2016; Schurz et al., Reference Schurz, Radua, Tholen, Maliske, Margulies, Mars and Kanske2021); structures that partially overlap with the default mode network (Mars et al., Reference Mars, Neubert, Noonan, Sallet, Toni and Rushworth2012). Activation within the network implicated in perspective-taking directly corresponds to task performance (Kanske et al., Reference Kanske, Böckler, Trautwein and Singer2015). Most importantly for the present review, neurophysiological research showed that empathy and perspective-taking are clearly independent and distinguishable processes, both on a behavioral and neurophysiological basis (Kanske, Böckler, Trautwein, Parianen Lesemann, & Singer, Reference Kanske, Böckler, Trautwein, Parianen Lesemann and Singer2016). Intra- and interindividual variations in the two capacities are independent as well (Stietz et al., Reference Stietz, Jauk, Krach and Kanske2019). This means that empathic functioning and perspective-taking could be selectively altered in different expressions of narcissism. Neuroscience research might unveil alterations in the involved networks during empathy and perspective-taking tasks, during rest, and in structural measures.

2.2 Literature search and inclusion strategy

For this review, we considered quantitative empirical journal articles on grandiose and vulnerable narcissism, pathological narcissism, and NPD using neuroimaging, neurophysiological, or neuroendocrine measures. For the inclusion, we required a standardized assessment of narcissism that targets one of the constructs introduced above. We searched the academic databases Scopus and PubMed, complemented by Google Scholar searches, for the following terms in the article abstracts: narciss* + neuro*, bio*, physio*, EEG, *MRI, and *imaging. After the exclusion of 3 empirical articles using qualitative methods and 4 studies that did not use neuroscience methods, we obtained a set of 35 empirical journal articles. Of those journal articles, one (Yang, Sedikides, Gu, Luo, Wang, Yang, et al., Reference Yang, Sedikides, Gu, Luo, Wang, Yang and Cai2018) targeted the relatively novel construct of communal narcissism (an expression of agentic, narcissistic strivings through communal means, such as being the most helpful person; Gebauer, Sedikides, Verplanken, & Maio, Reference Gebauer, Sedikides, Verplanken and Maio2012). As this study is to date the only neuroscience investigation of the communal narcissism construct, and the construct itself is still relatively new,Footnote 3 we include this study in the discussion section, rather than the main part of the review. Another study (Kelsey et al., Reference Kelsey, Ornduff, Reiff and Arthur2002) used subscales of a multidimensional personality scale, the Bell Object Relations and Reality Testing Inventory (BORRTI; Bell, Reference Bell1995) for the assessment of overt and covert narcissism (which were sometimes equated with grandiose and vulnerable narcissism). As it was not possible to evaluate the convergent validity of the used subscales (egocentricity and alienation) with more widely used measures of grandiose and vulnerable narcissism, and also the authors themselves discuss the validity of the measure critically (Kelsey et al., Reference Kelsey, Ornduff, Reiff and Arthur2002), we excluded this study from our review. We excluded one further study as the indicator of narcissism was too indirect to allow for comparisons with the rest of the literature (peer ratings of grandiosity were transformed into measures of sociometric preference in the crucial analysis involving a neuroendocrine marker; Bukowski, Schwartzman, Santo, Bagwell, & Adams, Reference Bukowski, Schwartzman, Santo, Bagwell and Adams2009). Table 2 summarizes the remaining 32 studies. In addition to these published studies, we also found two unpublished doctoral dissertations meeting the abovementioned criteria (Krusemark, Reference Krusemark2009; Noser, Reference Noser2017), which we also included in the main part of this review and Table 2, leading to a total of 34 studies. Finally, our literature search identified one hypothesis proposal (Jankowiak-Siuda & Zajkowski, Reference Jankowiak-Siuda and Zajkowski2013), two review articles that each cover parts of the research reviewed here (Coleman et al., Reference Coleman, Pincus and Smyth2019; Di Sarno et al., Reference Di Sarno, Di Pierro and Madeddu2018), one conceptual paper (George & Short, Reference George and Short2018), and four book chapters (Konrath & Bonadonna, Reference Konrath, Bonadonna and Besser2014; Elizabeth A. Krusemark, Reference Krusemark, Campbell and Miller2012, Reference Krusemark, Hermann, Brunell and Foster2018; Schulze & Roepke, Reference Schulze, Roepke, Mulert and Shenton2014), which do not present empirical data but rather integrate previous studies. We include these works in section 3 of the present review.

Table 2. Summary of neuroscience studies on narcissism

The studies are described in section 2 of the main text. F, female; M, male. Abbreviations of narcissism measures: CNS, Childhood Narcissism Scale; DTDD, Dark Triad Dirty dozen; HSNS, Hypersensitive Narcissism Scale; NARQ, Narcissistic Admiration and Rivalry Questionnaire; NI, Narcissism Inventory; NPD, narcissistic personality disorder; NPI, Narcissistic Personality Inventory; NPI-16, abbreviated 16-item – version; PNI, Pathological Narcissism Inventory; SCATI, Short Coolidge Axis II inventory; SCID II, structured clinical interview for DSM-IV, personality disorders; SDT, short dark triad. Abbreviations of neuroscience measures: ECG, Electrocardiogram; EEG, Electroencephalogram; EEG, Electromyogram; DTI, diffusion tensor imaging; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging; rs-fMRI, resting-state fMRI; VBM, voxel-based morphometry. Abbreviations of brain structures: ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; dACC, dorsal ACC; vACC, ventral ACC; sgACC, subgenual ACC; AI, anterior insula; mPFC, medial prefrontal cortex.

As Table 2 shows, among the 34 studies, 21 studies used experimental paradigms, in which participants were confronted with stimuli or tasks. Ten studies used baseline measures of brain activity or neuroendocrine markers, and five studies used structural brain imaging measures.Footnote 4 We classified the experimental paradigm studies as either primarily intrapersonal (self-related paradigms) or interpersonal (other related paradigms) in focus. This classification is in line with social/personality models of narcissism (e.g., Morf & Rhodewalt, Reference Morf and Rhodewalt2001) and with prevailing models of personality functioning, such as the DSM-5 AMPD (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Though this distinction may not appear straightforward in each case (such as, for instance, the touch anticipation paradigm used by Scalabrini et al., Reference Scalabrini, Huang, Mucci, Perrucci, Ferretti, Fossati and Ebisch2017), we capitalized on whether the main focus of the paradigm is to manipulate intrapersonal, self-regulatory (e.g., emotion regulation), or interpersonal (e.g., empathizing, emotion recognition) psychological processes.

As Table 2 further shows, the vast majority of neuroscience research investigated the construct of grandiose narcissism (25 studies). Three studies investigated pathological narcissism.Footnote 5 Only two studies, to date, explicitly focused on vulnerable narcissism in which separate measures were used to assess vulnerability (though some discuss narcissistic vulnerability as a potential mechanism of the observed results). All studies on grandiose or pathological narcissism used nonclinical samples, mostly comprised of students. Six studies investigated NPD in patients.

In the following, we will summarize the findings of these studies grouped in functional, baseline, and structural studies of the different narcissism constructs. Table 2 provides an overview of all studies including sample characteristics. We will then integrate this research into a general discussion, in which we also consider prior reviews and book chapters on the topic.

2.3 Experimental studies

2.3.1 Studies targeting intrapersonal functions

Almost all among the 15 studies of intrapersonal functions in narcissism reviewed here investigated the construct of grandiose narcissism in nonclinical samples; one study also assessed vulnerable narcissism, and one study focused on pathological narcissism. Of those studying grandiose narcissism, all but three used the NPI (see section 1.1) as an indicator of grandiosity. One of the central questions in neuroscience studies targeting intrapersonal functions in narcissism is if and to what extent individuals high in narcissism are sensitive to ego threat in that they display stronger stress responses facing potentially threatening self-relevant information (see section 2.1). The majority of studies investigating intrapersonal, self-related characteristics of grandiose narcissism demonstrate that individuals high in grandiose narcissism are indeed sensitive to situations involving actual or anticipated ego threat, as discussed in the following.

Sommer and colleagues (Reference Sommer, Kirkland, Newman, Estrella and Andreassi2009) studied the consequences of interpersonal rejection and found that individuals higher in the entitlement/exploitativeness facet of the NPI displayed stronger stress reactions in terms of increases in systolic blood pressure when listening to rejection stories and completing rejection stories, as opposed to acceptance stories. At a subjective level, when rejection by a romantic partner had to be imagined (as opposed to rejection by a friend), participants scoring high on the NPI reported greater anger. Similarly, Cascio and colleagues (Reference Cascio, Konrath and Falk2015) had male adolescents perform a Cyberball game during functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Participants believed they were interacting with real others, while actually the game was programmed to first include, and then exclude participants. Stronger activation for exclusion was found in a network referred to by the authors as the “social pain network” (p. 335) – largely overlapping with the salience network – comprising the dACC, the AI, and the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex in more narcissistic young men. Interestingly, while previous self-report research showed that individuals high in grandiose narcissism are sensitive to achievement failure but not social exclusion (Besser & Priel, Reference Besser and Priel2010), and narcissism was not related to self-reports of social exclusion in this study, grandiose narcissism correlated with a brain activity pattern previously shown to relate to feelings of social exclusion (Eisenberger, Reference Eisenberger2003). Activation in the dACC was also related to social exclusion on a meta-analytic basis (Rotge et al., Reference Rotge, Lemogne, Hinfray, Huguet, Grynszpan, Tartour and Fossati2015), though a recent meta-analysis observed dACC activation only in a minority of studies (Mwilambwe-Tshilobo & Spreng, Reference Mwilambwe-Tshilobo and Spreng2021), and the amount of dACC involvement might depend on the used task (Mwilambwe-Tshilobo & Spreng, Reference Mwilambwe-Tshilobo and Spreng2021; Rotge et al., Reference Rotge, Lemogne, Hinfray, Huguet, Grynszpan, Tartour and Fossati2015). The activation patterns observed in this study thus led the authors to conclude that “narcissists’ social pain [is] seen only in the brain” (Cascio et al., Reference Cascio, Konrath and Falk2015, p. 335). The neuroimaging results obtained for social exclusion in this study are further similar to those observed for social exclusion in people with low self-esteem (Onoda et al., Reference Onoda, Okamoto, Nakashima, Nittono, Yoshimura, Yamawaki, Yamaguchi and Ura2010), and individuals with borderline personality disorder (Wrege et al., Reference Wrege, Ruocco, Euler, Preller, Busmann, Meya and Walter2019), which may suggest similar neural mechanisms for narcissistic individuals and those with low self-esteem or severe deficits in personality functioning. The social exclusion line of research was continued in a later study using the Cyberball paradigm (Chester & DeWall, Reference Chester and DeWall2016). Focusing again on the dACC as part of the salience network, referred to as the brain’s “alarm system” (p. 362) by the authors, it was found that dACC activation was higher during social exclusion, and that grandiose narcissism and dACC activation interactively predicted the willingness to administer loud noise blasts to the bogus Cyberball partners. While narcissism did not show a significant main effect on dACC activation in this study, there was a significant interaction between narcissism and anxious attachment on dACC activation. The authors conclude that grandiose narcissism, paired with neural markers of rejection distress, leads to aggressive behavior.

Another study demonstrated the increased sensitivity of individuals high in grandiose narcissism to ego threat using a neuroendocrine marker: Edelstein, Yim, and Quas (Reference Edelstein, Yim and Quas2010) had participants either undergo the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST; Kirschbaum, Pirke, & Hellhammer, Reference Kirschbaum, Pirke and Hellhammer1993) or a control condition. The TSST reliably induces strong stress responses by requesting participants to perform a public speech and a mental arithmetic task in front of an audience. Still, individual differences in the extent of these stress responses can be related to psychological variables. Stress responses were quantified by means of saliva cortisol levels at several time points. Highly narcissistic men showed higher peaks and prolonged stress responses as compared to low narcissistic men in the TSST. No difference was found in women. Similar, albeit weaker results were obtained for ratings of negative affect. A similar study investigated the effects of a mock job interview, as a social stressor, on respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) in young adults (Zhang, Wang, You, Lü, & Luo, Reference Zhang, Wang, You, Lü and Luo2015). Participants’ task was to introduce themselves to a committee of strangers, and were told that they would be videotaped and evaluated afterward. Pre-, peri-, and post-interview measures of RSA were assessed as an indicator of emotion regulation capacity. Of note, this study is to date one of the two, which used independent indicators of grandiose and vulnerable narcissism (NPI and HSNS). RSA was not per se related to either narcissism measure, but vulnerable narcissism moderated the associations between RSA reactivity and a self-report measure of emotion regulation in the way that those higher in vulnerable narcissism who also displayed lower RSA decreases showed stronger emotion regulation difficulties. The authors interpret these effects in terms of RSA as a potentially protective factor in vulnerable narcissism.

Finally, using a related experimental approach, yet a different physiological measure, Grapsas and colleagues (Reference Grapsas, Denissen, Lee, Bos and Brummelman2020) recently investigated the effects of social stress in the form of status threat. A large sample of children (mean age of 10 years) performed a social hierarchy task on a simulated social media platform, being either randomly assigned a low or a high social status in the bogus ranking. Children with higher levels of grandiose narcissism (assessed via the Childhood Narcissism Scale, a measure drawing strongly on grandiose narcissism; Thomaes et al., Reference Thomaes, Stegge, Bushman, Olthof and Denissen2008) reacted with stronger increases in negative affect (measured via electromyography of the corrugator supercilii, involved in frowning) during status loss, and stronger increases in positive affect (measured via electromyography of zygomaticus major, involved in smiling) and negative affect during status gain. The results show that sensitivity to social status in relation to narcissism can already be observed at a young age.

So far, it can be concluded that situations involving social rejection or exclusion, threat of evaluation (TSST), or status threat induce stronger stress responses (increased blood pressure, saliency network activation, cortisol release) or aggressive behavior (administering noise blasts) in individuals high in grandiose narcissism. One study, however, did not find such an effect using RSA (Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Wang, You, Lü and Luo2015). Of note, grandiose narcissism amplifies negative affective responses in face of ego threat even in children (Grapsas et al., Reference Grapsas, Denissen, Lee, Bos and Brummelman2020). While this research points to increased stress responses for individuals higher in narcissism in situations that are supposed to be threatening for most of us, further research shows that even stimuli that are not intrinsically threatening induce similar responses: Jauk, Benedek, and colleagues (Reference Jauk, Benedek, Koschutnig, Kedia and Neubauer2017) had extreme groups of individuals either high or low in grandiose narcissism perform a visual self-viewing paradigm during fMRI. Viewing own faces, as compared to viewing friends’ or strangers’ faces, was accompanied by higher activation in the dACC and also the ventral ACC (vACC) in highly narcissistic men – findings that are similar to those described above for social exclusion (Cascio et al., Reference Cascio, Konrath and Falk2015). The authors concluded that self-referential stimulation induces conflict (expectancy violation) or negative affect in grandiose-narcissistic men. Though this interpretation seems to be generally in line with the understanding of the dACC as a region implicated in adaptive control in the light of potential threat (de la Vega, Chang, Banich, Wager, & Yarkoni, Reference de la Vega, Chang, Banich, Wager and Yarkoni2016; Kanske & Kotz, Reference Kanske and Kotz2011; Shackman et al., Reference Shackman, Salomons, Slagter, Fox, Winter and Davidson2011; Somerville, Heatherton, & Kelley, Reference Somerville, Heatherton and Kelley2006), it needs to be noted that neither the dACC nor the salience network, in general, does have a single, unitary function (Seeley, Reference Seeley2019). The findings can thus only be interpreted in the particular context, which – considered together with the above-reviewed studies on social exclusion (Cascio et al., Reference Cascio, Konrath and Falk2015; Chester & DeWall, Reference Chester and DeWall2016) – suggests that individuals high in narcissism show stronger saliency-related responses in the light of self-referential material, putatively more indicative of negative than positive anticipatory processes (de la Vega et al., Reference de la Vega, Chang, Banich, Wager and Yarkoni2016; Shackman et al., Reference Shackman, Salomons, Slagter, Fox, Winter and Davidson2011). A recent EEG study by Mück and colleagues (Reference Mück, Ohmann, Dummel, Mattes, Thesing and Stahl2020) used a similar self-face viewing paradigm: participants either viewed pictures of their own face, a celebrity, or a stranger. EEG differences were investigated in two different components: the P1 component, which reflects an early stage of face processing and is higher when valence is ascribed to the stimulus, and the N170, which reflects a later stage of face processing and is commonly higher for the own face as compared to others’ faces.Footnote 6 The moderating effect of agentic and antagonistic aspects of grandiose narcissism (admiration and rivalry; see Back et al., Reference Back, Küfner, Dufner, Gerlach, Rauthmann and Denissen2013) was investigated. Individuals high on agentic narcissism or low on antagonistic narcissism displayed a lower P1 response (for self vs. celebrity), and individuals higher on antagonistic narcissism displayed a lower N170 response (for self vs. stranger). The authors put forward different interpretations for these findings, the first of which is that agentic narcissism is associated with an inhibition of early attention toward the own face in order to maintain a grandiose self-image, whereas antagonistic narcissism is associated with increased attention at later stages for similar self-protection goals. While the different psychological and neuroscience measures used in this study and the previously discussed study by Jauk, Benedek, and colleagues (Reference Jauk, Benedek, Koschutnig, Kedia and Neubauer2017) make it hard to reconcile both studies’ results, it can be tentatively hypothesized that the effects observed for antagonistic narcissism, indicative of increased later stage processing facing potential ego threat, might be related to the increased activity in parts of the saliency network observed earlier. In any case, the results of both studies show that narcissism modulates neural responses to self-related material (own face), presumably in terms of increased sensitivity or vigilance for ego threat, even though this material is not intrinsically threatening.

This interpretational bias might even be stronger when it comes to pathological narcissism: Stenstrom and colleagues (Reference Stenstrom, Dinsmore, Kunstman and Vohs2018) investigated the effects of money exposure on testosterone level in young men varying on pathological narcissism. Participants’ task was to sort either banknotes (experimental condition, which was hypothesized to increase testosterone levels) or paper (control condition; between-Ss). Only men scoring low on pathological narcissism showed the expected increases in testosterone after sorting banknotes, but those high on pathological narcissism did not. The authors interpret this as a result of the intimidating effect the money exposure might have on men high on pathological narcissism, who perform a downward comparison threatening their ego. Finally, the effect of anticipated ego threat is apparent in a study by Woodman and colleagues (Reference Woodman, Roberts, Hardy, Callow and Rogers2011), who had participants perform a physical performance task, cycling on an ergometer, either under conditions of low or high social identifiability. Students were cycling in groups of three; in the low-identifiability condition, they were told that only the group performance would be made public at the university, whereas in the high-identifiability condition, they were told that individual performance would also be published. Cycling performance and heart rate increased under the high-identifiability condition for those high in grandiose narcissism. While this is not directly an indicator of experienced stress (as increases in heart rate might be explained by increases in performance), it indicates that individuals higher in narcissism are willing to invest more in the light of social evaluation, which is in line with general approach orientation in relation to grandiose narcissism (Spencer et al., Reference Spencer, Foster and Bedwell2018).

However, stimuli that are self-related but not self-relevant do not generally elicit stronger reactions in relation to narcissism: the first physiological study of grandiose narcissism investigated autonomic responses during active and passive coping with stressful stimuli (Kelsey et al., Reference Kelsey, Ornduff, McCann and Reiff2001). Extreme groups of men low versus high in grandiose narcissism were exposed to aversive auditory stimuli (loud tones), which could either be avoided (active coping condition) or were unavoidable (passive coping condition). Men high in grandiose narcissism displayed – at the same time – lower anxiety (greater skin conductance habituation) and greater vigilance (stronger heart rate deceleration) in response to aversive stimuli, which was consistent with their lower self-reported state anxiety. Interestingly, for these men, pre-ejection period shortening did not differ between active and passive coping, which the authors interpreted as fight/flight response independent of actual coping demands. Results indicate that men high in grandiose narcissism display cool but vigilant reactions to stressful stimuli, which bear similarities to the psychopathy construct (Kelsey et al., Reference Kelsey, Ornduff, McCann and Reiff2001). This line of research was extended by Sylvers and colleagues (Reference Sylvers, Brubaker, Alden, Brennan and Lilienfeld2008), who used a countdown-to-aversive-stimuli (white noise blast) task to study physiological reactions related to grandiose narcissism. Grandiose narcissism was assessed using both structured interviews and self-reports based on the currentFootnote 7 DSM criteria. In contrast to the authors’ expectations and the findings reported by Kelsey and colleagues (Reference Kelsey, Ornduff, McCann and Reiff2001), narcissism was not associated with any physiological changes, which might indicate lowered distress (skin conductance hyporeactivity or pre-ejection period shortening). Instead, antisocial traits were associated with these indicators. The authors attribute the discrepant results to differences in the assessment of narcissism, and critically discuss the use of clinical assessments in nonclinical samples. For the purpose of the present review, however, it seems most important to note that none of the studies observed stronger stress reactions in relation to narcissism.Footnote 8 Stimuli that are self-related but not self-relevant, such as loud tones or noise blasts, do not lead to stress reactions (Kelsey et al., Reference Kelsey, Ornduff, McCann and Reiff2001; Sylvers et al., Reference Sylvers, Brubaker, Alden, Brennan and Lilienfeld2008), or even elicit emotionally cool, prepared reactions (Kelsey et al., Reference Kelsey, Ornduff, McCann and Reiff2001), which points to some overlap between narcissism and the psychopathy construct, traditionally characterized by fearlessness (Lykken, Reference Lykken1995; see also Hare & Neumann, Reference Hare and Neumann2008).

This is further evident in two studies targeting deceptive and risky behavior: Dane, Jonason, and McCaffrey (Reference Dane, Jonason and McCaffrey2018) recently investigated the neuroendocrine reactions of men differing in grandiose narcissism (NPI) to the effects of lying. Participants were instructed to behave like effective liars, and were videotaped while telling two true and one lie statement about themselves.Footnote 9 The pre-post – comparison of salivary cortisol and testosterone revealed that (1) men higher in narcissism showed a pattern of higher baseline cortisol (which is in line with the finding of Reinhard, Konrath, Lopez, & Cameron, Reference Reinhard, Konrath, Lopez and Cameron2012 as well as Pfattheicher, Reference Pfattheicher2016, discussed in section 2.4) and post-lie decreases in cortisol, though this effect was not significant in the small sample. These results thus differ from those reported by Edelstein, Yam, and Quas (Reference Edelstein, Yim and Quas2010), who observed increases following the TSST, and point to the moderating influence of the subjective stress level, or also level of enjoyability induced by the task. While the TSST was experienced as stressful by more narcissistic male participants (Edelstein et al., Reference Edelstein, Yim and Quas2010), the lying task was actually perceived as enjoyable by more narcissistic participants (Dane et al., Reference Dane, Jonason and McCaffrey2018). An opposite pattern of results was observed for testosterone in this study, with more narcissistic men displaying increases in testosterone, and less narcissistic men displaying decreases, which points to enjoyment and the self-aggrandizing function of deceiving others. One recent EEG study investigated risk-taking processes in relation to grandiose narcissism (Yang et al., Reference Yang, Sedikides, Gu, Luo, Wang, Yang and Cai2018). Participants performed a monetary gambling task, where they could choose between low- and high-risk options. At the behavioral level, individuals higher in narcissism made more high-risk choices in trials that were preceded by other high-risk trials. At the neurophysiological level, no differences in feedback-related negativity emerged between more and less narcissistic individuals, which means that error monitoring did not differ between individuals with lower and higher narcissism. However, highly narcissistic individuals showed a weaker P3 response following outcomes of high-risk decisions, which the authors interpret as evidence for reduced action updating in narcissism leading to further risk-taking behavior as a consequence of reduced feedback learning. Taken together, these studies again point to some overlap with the psychopathy construct, which is also characterized by a bold interpersonal style (including lying; Hare & Neumann, Reference Hare and Neumann2008) and the reduced ability to learn from previous experiences (Galang, Reference Galang2010).

Finally, an unpublished EEG study investigating the neural correlates of self-enhancement (Krusemark, Reference Krusemark2009) found that individuals high in grandiose narcissism make more self-serving attributions after false performance feedback in a working memory task (i.e., attributing positive feedback internally, irrespective of actual task performance), and that this is accompanied by relatively weaker event-related activity in widespread regions of the cortex. Activation related to the feedback itself, on the contrary, was accompanied by stronger neural responses. The author interprets this pattern of results as a more shallow processing in relation to biased attributions, but a greater responsiveness to evaluative information. Considered together with the studies reviewed above, this may be seen as supporting the picture of grandiose-narcissistic individuals displaying overly confident, hyporeactive responses when it comes to decision-making (Yang, Sedikides, Gu, Luo, Wang, & Cai, Reference Yang, Sedikides, Gu, Luo, Wang and Cai2018) and attribution of success (Krusemark, Reference Krusemark2009), alongside increased responsiveness to potentially self-relevant information (Grapsas et al., Reference Grapsas, Denissen, Lee, Bos and Brummelman2020; Jauk, Benedek, et al., Reference Jauk, Benedek, Koschutnig, Kedia and Neubauer2017; Krusemark, Reference Krusemark2009).

2.3.2 Studies targeting interpersonal functions

Six studies investigated the neuroscience correlates of interpersonal functions in narcissism. Contrary to the studies reviewed above, which mostly focused on grandiose narcissism, these works span a more diverse palette of different expressions of narcissism including also pathological narcissism and NPD, and use heterogeneous experimental paradigms. We review these studies beginning with more high-level emotion processing paradigms (e.g., empathize with emotional faces), turning then to more low-level paradigms (e.g., sensory processing of emotional stimuli), and finally to social–psychological accounts (e.g., interactions among experimental variations, personality, and neuroendocrine markers).

Fan and colleagues (Reference Fan, Wonneberger, Enzi, de Greck, Ulrich, Tempelmann, Bogerts, Doering and Northoff2011) conducted the first fMRI study on interpersonal functions in narcissism, focusing on the well-documented empathic alterations. The study used extreme groups of low and high narcissistic individuals according to the Narcissism Inventory (NI; Denecke & Hilgenstock, Reference Denecke and Hilgenstock1989), which likely assesses pathological narcissism.Footnote 10 Participants either viewed emotional faces or, as a control condition, smoothed faces, and were instructed to empathize with these faces. Highly narcissistic individuals showed activation differences in the right AI, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, premotor cortex, and in the posterior cingulate cortex; regions overlapping with those identified by a meta-analysis for empathy; particularly in the AI (Lamm et al., Reference Lamm, Decety and Singer2011). In the right AI, highly narcissistic individuals showed a lower positive signal change from the control to the empathy condition (Fan et al., Reference Fan, Wonneberger, Enzi, de Greck, Ulrich, Tempelmann, Bogerts, Doering and Northoff2011). Importantly, the highly narcissistic group also showed higher alexithymia scores, which lead the authors to conclude that “the apparent difficulty in accessing the own emotions, as suggested by our finding of increased alexithymia, may lead to problems in simulating the other’s affective states as our neural findings may be interpreted” (p. 1649). This may point to a general difficulty of experiencing emotional states as a basis of altered empathic functioning in narcissism, as expressed in a recent model (Valdespino et al., Reference Valdespino, Antezana, Ghane and Richey2017), and as is the case with increased alexithymia in other conditions as well (Bird et al., Reference Bird, Silani, Brindley, White, Frith and Singer2010; Hoffmann et al., Reference Hoffmann, Banzhaf, Kanske, Gärtner, Bermpohl and Singer2016). A further fMRI study investigated the effects of social touch, which may be considered another high-level interpersonal paradigm, in narcissism: Scalabrini and colleagues (Reference Scalabrini, Huang, Mucci, Perrucci, Ferretti, Fossati and Ebisch2017) had a sample of male participants either touch a human hand or a mannequin hand in the MRI scanner. Human touch anticipation was accompanied by lower activation in the right AI in individuals scoring high on grandiose narcissism (grandiose factor of the PNIFootnote 11 ). Interestingly, narcissistic grandiosity not only correlated with task-related, but also with resting-state activity. Narcissistic grandiosity mediated the association between spontaneous, resting-state, and task-related activity in the AI. The authors thus concluded that “narcissism could function as a factor mediating between internal processing, related to the self, and external sensory information related to the social world” and “may be related to an increased internal predisposition accompanied by a motivation-based disengagement from social processing” (p. 10). Taken together, both studies show alterations of activity in the AI, a key node of empathic processing within the salience network (Lamm et al., Reference Lamm, Decety and Singer2011).

Turning now to more low-level emotion processing paradigms, Marcoux and colleagues (Reference Marcoux, Michon, Lemelin, Voisin, Vachon-Presseau and Jackson2014) used a somatosensory gating paradigm to investigate emotional perceptiveness in NPD. In this paradigm, a steady-state stimulation (repetitive tactile stimulation on the hand) is modulated by viewing either images depicting a painful situation of another person, which should evoke empathic reactions, or non-painful images as a control condition. Somatosensory gating is operationalized by the reduction of somatosensory activity related to the tactile stimulation, indexed by the EEG P3 amplitude, in painful versus non-painful images; i.e., individuals who are supposed to display weaker empathic reactions should show weaker gating effects. Contrary to what could be expected, the male NPD patients showed an increased somatosensory gating effect compared to controls, and this effect was not attributable to different subjective pain thresholds. The authors interpreted their findings in the way that narcissistic men were “feeling but not caring” (p. 341); i.e., they displayed stronger somatosensory resonance, but did not report higher empathic concern or distress, and lower perspective-taking, in a trait self-report measure. These results may be seen as supporting the notion of reduced propensity, not capacity, of empathic alterations in NPD (Baskin-Sommers et al., Reference Baskin-Sommers, Krusemark and Ronningstam2014) in a low-level sensory processing paradigm. In other words, it can be tentatively concluded that low-level other directed processing capability is not generally affected in NPD, but is probably attenuated for motivational reasons. In a similar study, Zhang and colleagues (Reference Zhang, Shen, Zhu, Ma and Wang2016) investigated EEG event-related potentials during facial emotion processing in different personality disorder groups. The sample of NPD patients – who displayed higher anxiety and, partly, higher depression than controls in this study – were found to take longer time to respond to happy faces (but not other emotions), which were intermixed with landscape pictures. At a neurophysiological level, though no main effects for narcissism were found across different electrodes, components, and measures (latency, amplitude), P2 amplitude to neutral and happy faces was negatively related to depression in NPD patients. The authors interpreted the effects in the way that depression amplifies self-focus in narcissism. This self-focus reduces other directed processing particularly in the light of neutral or positive emotion, which is noncongruent to the own depressed state. This interpretation is consistent with the notion of state-dependent alterations – in this case, depression level – of interpersonal functioning in narcissism (Baskin-Sommers et al., Reference Baskin-Sommers, Krusemark and Ronningstam2014) for a low-level paradigm. Thus, taken together with the previous study by Marcoux and colleagues (Reference Marcoux, Michon, Lemelin, Voisin, Vachon-Presseau and Jackson2014), it can be concluded that low-level processing of other related stimuli is not generally impaired in NPD (Marcoux et al., Reference Marcoux, Michon, Lemelin, Voisin, Vachon-Presseau and Jackson2014; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Shen, Zhu, Ma and Wang2016), but can be impaired in states of amplified self-focus, such as in depression (Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Shen, Zhu, Ma and Wang2016), particularly when emotions are positive or noncongruent to the own emotion (Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Shen, Zhu, Ma and Wang2016; for similar findings from depression research, see Hoffmann et al., Reference Hoffmann, Banzhaf, Kanske, Gärtner, Bermpohl and Singer2016).

Turning now to the social–psychological line of research on narcissism, Lobbestael and colleagues (Reference Lobbestael, Baumeister, Fiebig and Eckel2014) investigated behavioral aggression and testosterone in grandiose and vulnerable narcissism: a sample of young men performed an adapted Taylor Aggression Paradigm, in which participants were made believe that they were playing a competitive game against a human opponent. After each round, noise blasts of varying intensities and durations could be administered to the bogus opponent. Results showed that young men higher in grandiose, but not vulnerable narcissism, displayed more aggression in administering noise blasts, and along with that also increases in testosterone level. The authors interpret these findings in terms of a higher propensity of grandiose-narcissistic individuals to act aggressively toward others, which also reflects in characteristic neuroendocrine changes. Finally, one recent study also took a social–psychological approach to study interpersonal functioning in narcissism in relation to testosterone: Mead and colleagues (Reference Mead, Baumeister, Stuppy and Vohs2018) assigned participants varying on grandiose narcissism (NPI) randomly to either a power condition, where they were made believe they would exert control over others in the following group task, or a control condition, where they were just informed that they were going to take part in the group task. Those participants in the power condition, who additionally had high baseline testosterone, displayed higher scores on the entitlement/exploitativeness factor of the NPI, and reported higher willingness to misuse power. Thus, contrary to other studies reviewed here, this study speaks to the antecedents of grandiose narcissism in terms of individual biological disposition (testosterone level) and social influences (power manipulation). It thus shows that grandiose narcissism cannot only be regarded as a trait that exerts its influence on other variables, but also as a state that can itself be influenced by biological and social factors. This finding is in line – though opposite in causal direction – with the results of Dane and colleagues (Reference Dane, Jonason and McCaffrey2018) as well as Lobbestael and colleagues (Reference Lobbestael, Baumeister, Fiebig and Eckel2014), who reported increases in testosterone levels in more narcissistic men after lying to others/behaving aggressively toward others. Taken together, these studies point to dynamic interactions between narcissism, testosterone, and situations involving the exertion of social power, deception, or aggression.

2.4 Baseline function studies

We identified 10 studies that investigated different biophysiological measures of baseline functioning in narcissism: six studies used measures of neuroendocrine markers including cortisol, other neuroendocrine markers targeting habitual stress levels, or testosterone, three investigated resting-state fMRI (one of them is the already discussed social touch study by Scalabrini et al., Reference Scalabrini, Huang, Mucci, Perrucci, Ferretti, Fossati and Ebisch2017), and one study used auditory evoked potentials in EEG.Footnote 12 Except for two studies, which were carried out on NPD patients, all other studies investigated continuous differences in grandiose narcissism in relatively large samples.

Studies targeting neuroendocrine markers of habitual stress mostly speak to elevated stress in relation to narcissism, partially moderated by other variables: Reinhard and colleagues (Reference Reinhard, Konrath, Lopez and Cameron2012) carried out the first study on the association between a standardized measure of grandiose narcissism (NPI) and baseline cortisol levels. In a relatively large sample of young individuals, they observed an interaction among sex and narcissism in the way that narcissistic men have higher baseline cortisol across two time points separated by a 25-minute period. The effect sizes were substantial with correlations around r = .40. Critically, this effect could not be explained by potentially confounding psychological variables (positive/negative mood, stress, social support, or relationship status). The effects were stronger for the entitlement/exploitativeness factor than for other factors of the NPI, and were also evident in females when considering only this factor. This shows that particularly those aspects of grandiose narcissism that are most maladaptive (Ackerman et al., Reference Ackerman, Witt, Donnellan, Trzesniewski, Robins and Kashy2011) are associated with chronic stress. Subsequently, two studies attempted to indirectly or directly replicate the results observed by Reinhard and colleagues (Reference Reinhard, Konrath, Lopez and Cameron2012): the first study successfully replicated the associations between grandiose narcissism and baseline measures of cortisol, and extended it by measures of testosterone among young men (Pfattheicher, Reference Pfattheicher2016). It was found that higher levels of narcissism – but not of its near neighbor’s psychopathy or Machiavellianism – were related to higher levels of both baseline cortisol and baseline testosterone as assessed by two salivary probes separated by 6 min. The author interprets the findings in the way that men with higher levels of narcissism are at the same time responsive to social threat (cortisol) and socially dominant (testosterone). The second study, however, did not replicate the findings: Wardecker and colleagues (Reference Wardecker, Chopik, LaBelle and Edelstein2018) investigated the association of grandiose narcissism (NPI) and baseline cortisol in a large sample of young women and men. They did neither observe any overall associations between narcissism and cortisol, nor interactions with sex or effects for specific NPI factors, as reported in the original study. Though the replication attempt was well powered, methodological differences such as the saliva sampling procedure could still have contributed to the failure to replicate. Finally, an unpublished study replicated the associations between grandiose narcissism (assessed using the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen, which focuses on grandiosity, but also correlates with vulnerability; Jonason & Webster, Reference Jonason and Webster2010; Küfner et al., Reference Küfner, Dufner and Back2015) and baseline testosterone: Noser (Reference Noser2017) observed a correlation (r = 0.22) of narcissism and salivary testosterone in a large sample of adult men, but not of its neighboring personality dimensions psychopathy or Machiavellianism.

Two more studies investigated cortisol and further neuroendocrine markers in narcissism: Cheng and colleagues (Reference Cheng, Tracy and Miller2013) studied the relationships between cortisol, alpha-amylase, and grandiose narcissism in young women who provided saliva samples and ratings of positive and negative affect for three consecutive days. Both cortisol and alpha-amylase were higher in those who were high in grandiose narcissism and experienced more negative affect. The authors conclude that narcissism acts as a moderator in the relationship between perceived stress and negative health outcomes, as evident in cortisol (related to activity of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis) and alpha-amylase (related to sympathetic nervous system activity). Finally, a recent study (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Gozal, Coccaro and Fanning2020) investigated baseline levels of 8-hydroxy-2'-deoxyguanosine (8-OH-DG), an indicator of oxidative stress at a cellular level (which has been associated with a variety of mental disorders; e.g., Salim, Reference Salim2014), in patients with different personality disorders, other mental disorders, and controls. Results showed higher levels of 8-OH-DG in patients with NPD and borderline personality disorder; no such effects were observed for other personality disorders. The effects were independent of relevant covariates including age, gender, alcohol, and cigarette use, major depression, and posttraumatic stress disorder. The authors interpret the findings in terms of increased biological stress responses to interpersonal hypersensitivity. Considered together with the studies by Reinhard and colleagues (Reference Reinhard, Konrath, Lopez and Cameron2012), Pfattcheicher (Reference Pfattheicher2016), Cheng and colleagues (Reference Cheng, Tracy and Miller2013), as well as the TSST study by Edelstein and colleagues (Reference Edelstein, Yim and Quas2010), these results provide relatively consistent evidence for exaggerated biological stress responses (cortisol, alpha-amylase, and 8-OH-DG) in relation to grandiose narcissism or NPD as its extreme expression, though one study did not find such associations (Wardecker et al., Reference Wardecker, Chopik, LaBelle and Edelstein2018). The exact nature of stress responses might depend on sex or gender (see section 4), the expression of entitlement/exploitativeness, which most closely reflects the antagonistic core of the construct (Krizan & Herlache, Reference Krizan and Herlache2018; Weiss et al., Reference Weiss, Campbell, Lynam and Miller2019) and is considered most maladaptive (Ackerman et al., Reference Ackerman, Witt, Donnellan, Trzesniewski, Robins and Kashy2011), and subjective stress levels as moderating factors.

Three studies investigated resting-state functional brain connectivity by means of fMRI. The first of these (Yang et al., Reference Yang, Cun, Du, Yang, Wang, Wei and Qiu2015) studied functional connectivity differences in relation to grandiose narcissism (NPI) in a large sample of young women and men. The authors identified brain structural correlates of narcissism in the first step (see section 2.5), and then used regions in the right superior parietal lobe and the frontal eye fields – parts of the dorsal attention network – as seeds for subsequent functional connectivity analyses. In women, increased anticorrelations between the dorsal attention network and the default mode network were found as a function of narcissism. In men, the opposite pattern emerged, with decreased anticorrelation between the dorsal attention network and the default mode network as a function of narcissism. The authors interpret the effects in terms of a proneness toward outward-directed admiration seeking in more narcissistic women and loose boundaries between internal and external events in more narcissistic men. The second and most recent resting-state fMRI study of grandiose narcissism (NPI, Chinese Likert scale adaption) attempted to predict trait narcissism in a large sample of young individuals by means of machine learning applied to functional connectivity maps (Feng et al., Reference Feng, Yuan, Geng, Gu, Zhou, Wu and Luo2018). A network specific for subjects high in grandiose narcissism consisting of key nodes in the amygdala, the lateral and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), and the anterior cingulate cortex could be identified. Validation analyses showed that connectivity information within this network predicts grandiose narcissism scores in an independent sample of participants. While the network identified in this study is complex, interestingly, it encompasses some of the regions that were also found to discriminate between lower and higher narcissistic individuals in experimental functional imaging studies, above all the dACC (Cascio et al., Reference Cascio, Konrath and Falk2015; Chester & DeWall, Reference Chester and DeWall2016; Jauk, Benedek, et al., Reference Jauk, Benedek, Koschutnig, Kedia and Neubauer2017) as part of the salience network. Individuals high in grandiose narcissism further display altered connectivity within regions commonly implicated in emotion processing (given the role of the amygdala, as part of the limbic system, in fear conditioning; LeDoux, Reference LeDoux2000) and regions belonging to the default mode network (mPFC), for which structural changes can be observed (see section 2.5). Finally, in the already discussed combined resting-state and task-based fMRI study by Scalabrini and colleagues (Reference Scalabrini, Huang, Mucci, Perrucci, Ferretti, Fossati and Ebisch2017), a power law exponent analysis, measuring long-range temporal correlations in resting-state data, was conducted in ROIs that were functionally identified in the social touch experiment (human hand vs. mannequin hand: left postcentral gyrus, right culmen, and right AI; the latter only in interaction with grandiose narcissism). A significant positive association between grandiose narcissism and long-range temporal correlation was found in the AI, which the authors interpret in terms of increased self-focus in more grandiose individuals, particularly concerning aspects of the bodily and interoceptive self. Taken together, these studies show that grandiose narcissism can be related to intrinsic functional connectivity differences in the resting brain, and that the involved regions – parts of the default mode and salience networks – overlap with those implicated in imaging studies of intra- and interpersonal functioning (see section 3.3 for integrative discussion).

Finally, one EEG study used auditory evoked potentials to investigate cerebral information processing differences in several personality disorder groups, among them a group of NPD patients (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Wang, Fu, Liu, He, Dong and Jang2006). The neural responses to auditory stimuli of different amplitude were studied. It was hypothesized that certain personality disorder groups would display an “augmenting” evoked potential pattern indicative of sensation seeking and low serotonin levels. Such a pattern was found for patients with histrionic personality disorder, but no differences emerged for NPD. As with the studies reviewed in sections 2.3.1 and 2.3.2 above, it seems that low-level sensory processing is not generally altered in narcissism.

2.5 Structural imaging studies

Four structural imaging studies on narcissism were carried out so far, investigating gray matter volume, cortical thickness, and integrity of white matter tracts in grandiose narcissism, pathological narcissism, and NPD. Also, one of the abovementioned studies on resting-state functional connectivity in grandiose narcissism identified structural correlates (Yang et al., Reference Yang, Cun, Du, Yang, Wang, Wei and Qiu2015). We will first review results on gray matter, followed by results on white matter.

The first structural imaging study by Schulze and colleagues (Reference Schulze, Dziobek, Vater, Heekeren, Bajbouj, Renneberg, Heuser and Roepke2013) investigated gray matter volume in a group of NPD patients compared to matched healthy controls by means of voxel-based morphometry (VBM). Gray matter volume differences were found in the left AI and in the rostral anterior and posterior cingulate cortex, as well as the dorsolateral and mPFC. The region of gray matter volume reduction in the AI overlapped with a region identified as a structural correlate of self-reported empathy in the whole sample (though no significant empathy differences were found in the self-report measure). The other regions also overlap with those commonly implicated in empathy (Lamm et al., Reference Lamm, Decety and Singer2011), which, taken together, corroborates the notion of reduced empathic functioning in NPD at the level of brain structure. A subsequent study compared gray matter volume in six male NPD patients to controls (Nenadic et al., Reference Nenadic, Güllmar, Dietzek, Langbein, Steinke and Gaser2015). Lower gray matter volume was found in the right middle frontal gyrus, left medial prefrontal/anterior cingulate cortex, left middle occipital cortex, left fusiform/inferior temporal cortex, right superior temporal cortex, and in the left lingual gyrus. While some of these areas overlap with those reported by Schulze and colleagues (Reference Schulze, Dziobek, Vater, Heekeren, Bajbouj, Renneberg, Heuser and Roepke2013), this study did not find reduced gray matter volume in the AI. A further study investigated structural correlates of pathological narcissism (PNI) in a nonclinical sample: Mao and colleagues (Reference Mao, Sang, Wang, Hou, Huang, Wei, Zhang and Qiu2016) studied cortical thickness and volume using surface-based morphometry in a large sample of young individuals. They found lower cortical thickness and volume in the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Cortical thickness was further negatively associated with pathological narcissism in the right inferior frontal cortex, cortical volume was negatively associated in the right postcentral gyrus and the left mPFC. Finally, the aforementioned resting-state connectivity study by Yang and colleagues (Yang et al., Reference Yang, Cun, Du, Yang, Wang, Wei and Qiu2015) also reported a positive structural correlation between grandiose narcissism and regional gray matter volume in the right superior parietal lobe, which was evident only in women. The authors interpreted this effect in terms of increased attention seeking in women high in grandiose narcissism, as the superior parietal lobe is part of the dorsal attention network.

Turning now from gray matter to white matter, two studies investigated the integrity of fiber tracts using diffusion-tensor imaging: the aforementioned VBM study by Nenadic and colleagues (Reference Nenadic, Güllmar, Dietzek, Langbein, Steinke and Gaser2015) additionally investigated fractional anisotropy, a marker of integrity of white matter tracts (Basser et al., Reference Basser, Mattiello and LeBihan1994), in NPD. Fractional anisotropy was lower in the right frontal lobe the right anterior thalamic radiation, the right anterior temporal lobe, the left anterior/lateral temporal lobe, and the right brain stem. The authors state that “white matter pathology might contribute to the structural cortical deficits [but] obviously, these findings are in need of replication” (p. 185). Such a replication can be seen in the findings of Chester and colleagues (Reference Chester and DeWall2016), who also investigated the integrity of white matter tracts using fractional anisotropy as an indicator, and grandiose narcissism as a predictor. In a sample of young women and men, the a priori assumed negative relationship of grandiose narcissism and fractional anisotropy in the frontostriatal pathway, connecting the ventral striatum and the medial prefrontal cortex, was confirmed. The authors interpret this finding in terms of a neural disconnect between regions involved in reward (striatal) and intrapersonal (mPFC) processing, which might underlie narcissistic strivings for admiration from the social environment (Back et al., Reference Back, Küfner, Dufner, Gerlach, Rauthmann and Denissen2013; Morf & Rhodewalt, Reference Morf and Rhodewalt2001).

Summarizing the structural findings, the first thing that catches the eye is that all studies find negative associations between grandiose narcissism, pathological narcissism, or NPD and different indicators of brain structure or integrity (gray matter volume, cortical volume, and thickness, fractional anisotropy of fiber tracts), with the sole exception of Yang and colleagues (Reference Yang, Cun, Du, Yang, Wang, Wei and Qiu2015), who find a positive association between grandiose narcissism and gray matter volume in women. Though it could be hypothesized that increased self-focus – a core aspect of narcissism – might be accompanied by positive structural correlates in self-related areas such as key nodes of the default mode network, or also the anterior cingulate cortex, this does not seem to be the case, based on the existing evidence. This evidence rather exclusively points to dysfunctional aspects of narcissism in terms of reduced neuronal integrity. To this end, the most consistent finding across the existing studies seems to be lowered gray matter volume in the mPFC (Mao et al., Reference Mao, Sang, Wang, Hou, Huang, Wei, Zhang and Qiu2016; Nenadic et al., Reference Nenadic, Güllmar, Dietzek, Langbein, Steinke and Gaser2015; Schulze et al., Reference Schulze, Dziobek, Vater, Heekeren, Bajbouj, Renneberg, Heuser and Roepke2013).

3. Summary and Conclusions

3.1 Intrapersonal functions in narcissism

One of the central conclusions that can be drawn from the reviewed neuroscience research on intrapersonal functions in narcissism is that individuals with high levels of grandiose narcissism display exaggerated neurophysiological and neuroendocrine stress reactions to situations involving actual or anticipated ego threat, and also display habitually heightened stress indicators. This could be seen as supporting the long-standing mask model of narcissism, which posits that outward displays of grandiosity serve to compensate for an underlying, fragile, and vulnerable self (Akhtar, Reference Akhtar1989). While clinicians working with narcissistic patients widely acknowledge the coexistence of grandiose and vulnerable aspects (Pincus & Lukowitsky, Reference Pincus and Lukowitsky2010; Ronningstam, Reference Ronningstam2009), this view is not without controversy in quantitative research on narcissism. Conflicting findings have been obtained for the relationship between explicit and implicit self-esteem in grandiose narcissism, partially supporting (Zeigler-Hill, Reference Zeigler-Hill2006) or disproving (e.g., Campbell et al., Reference Campbell, Bosson, Goheen, Lakey and Kernis2007) the mask model view. The neuroscience of narcissism might provide an alternative resolution to this long-standing controversy, as it focuses on observable neurophysiological or neuroendocrine reactions to particular stimuli, or measures established baseline indicators, rather contrasting the global positivity/negativity of implicit or explicit self-concepts all at once, such as, for instance, when comparing measures of implicit and explicit self-esteem. This line of research might allow drawing specific inferences on latent, maybe implicit processes that can be formulated in terms of situational contingencies (cf. Roche et al., Reference Roche, Pincus, Conroy, Hyde and Ram2013; Wright & Edershile, Reference Wright and Edershile2018), such as “when confronted with situations that involve actual or anticipated ego threat, individuals with high grandiose narcissism react with increased vigilance and stress responses”. Narcissism, hereby, acts as a moderating factor that can impact either the strength or also the direction of effects. This view is consistent with the threatened egotism model of narcissism (Baumeister et al., Reference Baumeister, Smart and Boden1996; Bushman & Baumeister, Reference Bushman and Baumeister1998), which posits that individuals high in narcissism are highly sensitive to potential ego threat, assign higher self-relevance to potentially threatening situations, and display exaggerated responses facing such threat. Higher subjective self-relevance can also be observed for approach-driven goals (Spencer et al., Reference Spencer, Foster and Bedwell2018), as evident in the ergometer study (Woodman et al., Reference Woodman, Roberts, Hardy, Callow and Rogers2011).

Interestingly, recent research on communal narcissism (agentic strivings expressed in the communal domain; Gebauer et al., Reference Gebauer, Sedikides, Verplanken and Maio2012) has unveiled similar discrepancies between behavioral and neurophysiological indicators, as it was found in the ultimatum game that individuals high in communal narcissism are not more likely to reject unfair offers, but display stronger electrophysiological responses (P3 amplitude), indicative of greater emotional sensitivity to these offers (Yang, Sedikides, Gu, Luo, Wang, Yang, et al., Reference Yang, Sedikides, Gu, Luo, Wang, Yang and Cai2018). The study of diverging behavioral and neural responses might thus add another layer to understanding the complex relations between self-report, behavior, and involuntary neurophysiological reactions.

Neurophysiological research on the narcissistic self has investigated grandiose narcissism in relation to social rejection and found that imagined or actually experienced rejection goes along with higher systolic blood pressure as a stress indicator (Sommer et al., Reference Sommer, Kirkland, Newman, Estrella and Andreassi2009) and stronger activation in the dACC, AI, and subgenual ACC, regions overlapping largely with the salience network and referred to by the authors as the social pain network (Cascio et al., Reference Cascio, Konrath and Falk2015). These findings complement self-report research on social exclusion in grandiose narcissism, in which individuals high in grandiose narcissism do not report feeling threatened by social exclusion (Besser & Priel, Reference Besser and Priel2010). Neurophysiological research on social exclusion in narcissism thus reveals involuntary stress reactions that might either not be reported, or also not be accessible to the narcissistic individuals themselves. Stronger physiological responses in response to social status threats can already be observed in children (Grapsas et al., Reference Grapsas, Denissen, Lee, Bos and Brummelman2020). Importantly, these reactions are not without social consequences, as Chester and DeWall (Reference Chester and DeWall2016) report that those who are habitually high in grandiose narcissism and experience social rejection, as indicated by dACC activity, also display aggressive behavior toward others, as predicted by the threatened egotism model (Bushman & Baumeister, Reference Bushman and Baumeister1998). Similarly, as Lobbestael and colleagues (Reference Lobbestael, Baumeister, Fiebig and Eckel2014) reported, individuals high in grandiose narcissism have a higher tendency to behave aggressively toward others, which is accompanied by testosterone increase. This shows that a neuroscience approach to understanding narcissism may shed light on mechanisms underlying highly socially relevant phenomena, such as aggressive behavior, which may not be apparent in self-report research. Finally, men high in grandiose narcissism also display exaggerated stress responses as evident in high cortisol levels following performance-related ego threat (as induced by the TSST; Edelstein et al., Reference Edelstein, Yim and Quas2010), which is in line with self-report findings (Besser & Priel, Reference Besser and Priel2010). Whether or not potentially stressful situations induce stress reactions likely depends on the degree of ego threat (in terms of the possibility to fail), as lying about the self is accompanied by effects in the opposite direction (decreases in cortisol, increases in testosterone; Dane et al., Reference Dane, Jonason and McCaffrey2018). More generally, stimuli that are aversive and self-related, but not self-relevant in any way, such as the loud tones used by Kelsey and colleagues (Reference Kelsey, Ornduff, McCann and Reiff2001), do not induce stress responses, but rather evoke “cool” and prepared reactions, which reminds of fearlessness traditionally associated with the psychopathy construct (Lykken, Reference Lykken1995; for contemporary model, see Hare & Neumann, Reference Hare and Neumann2008).

While the abovementioned experimental paradigms directly challenge the self via exposure to threatening situations, the self-face viewing paradigm used in Jauk, Benedek, and colleagues (Reference Jauk, Benedek, Koschutnig, Kedia and Neubauer2017) cannot be considered intrinsically threatening. Nevertheless, self-face viewing also induced saliency-related activation in the dACC (overlapping with the results reported by Cascio et al., Reference Cascio, Konrath and Falk2015), which indicates that presumably neutral, or potentially even rewarding stimuli can induce higher vigilance to potential ego threat in highly narcissistic men if they are both self-related (in this case also self-referential) and self-relevant (in terms of subjective value). A recent EEG study showed that sensory processing of self-related stimuli is modulated by antagonistic and agentic aspects of narcissism, which further underpins the notion of increased self-relevance for individuals high on narcissism (Mück et al., Reference Mück, Ohmann, Dummel, Mattes, Thesing and Stahl2020). Finally, regarding pathological narcissism, which also includes narcissistic vulnerability at an explicit level, even situations that might be expected to induce feelings of power might be perceived as threatening (Stenstrom et al., Reference Stenstrom, Dinsmore, Kunstman and Vohs2018).

Regarding the interpretation of studies showing activation differences in regions of the salience network – particularly the dACC – for self-relevant stimuli in narcissism, it is important to note that salience network activation cannot per se be attributed to a unitary psychological process (Seeley, Reference Seeley2019). According to the adaptive control hypothesis, the dACC integrates signals from multiple systems under conditions of uncertainty to enable rapid action (Shackman et al., Reference Shackman, Salomons, Slagter, Fox, Winter and Davidson2011). This adaptive control is likely more relevant facing potential threat than potential reward, also from an evolutionary perspective (de la Vega et al., Reference de la Vega, Chang, Banich, Wager and Yarkoni2016; Shackman et al., Reference Shackman, Salomons, Slagter, Fox, Winter and Davidson2011), but the implication of the dACC in processes with positive valence has also been discussed (Shackman et al., Reference Shackman, Salomons, Slagter, Fox, Winter and Davidson2011). While the observed differences can thus not unequivocally be interpreted in terms of valence, one plausible interpretation of dACC activity in the reviewed studies seems to be that individuals high in narcissism show stronger saliency-related responses facing self-relevant stimuli or situations. On the basis of the understanding of dACC function and the reviewed literature, this seems to be more likely for potentially threatening than rewarding situations, but interpretations might also vary between studies.

The notion of exaggerated physiological reactions in grandiose narcissism is further supported by baseline studies showing that cortisol levels are habitually higher in narcissistic men (Pfattheicher, Reference Pfattheicher2016; Reinhard et al., Reference Reinhard, Konrath, Lopez and Cameron2012) and also women who score high on the entitlement/exploitativeness factor of the NPI (Reinhard et al., Reference Reinhard, Konrath, Lopez and Cameron2012), which most closely marks the core of the narcissism construct (Krizan & Herlache, Reference Krizan and Herlache2018; Weiss et al., Reference Weiss, Campbell, Lynam and Miller2019). Women high in grandiose narcissism also display elevated stress levels (cortisol and alpha-amylase) when negative affect is high (Cheng et al., Reference Cheng, Tracy and Miller2013). Recent research shows that stress levels are even higher on a cellular level in NPD (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Gozal, Coccaro and Fanning2020). Taken together, this is indicative of narcissistic men, and to some extent women, experiencing higher chronic stress levels, presumably due to higher sensitivity to potentially threatening self-relevant situations.

Concerning sex differences in relation to narcissism, available evidence suggests that men display stronger maladaptive reactions than women (Edelstein et al., Reference Edelstein, Yim and Quas2010; Jauk, Benedek, et al., Reference Jauk, Benedek, Koschutnig, Kedia and Neubauer2017; Reinhard et al., Reference Reinhard, Konrath, Lopez and Cameron2012). However, there is also evidence that women display strong reactions when perceived stress is high (Cheng et al., Reference Cheng, Tracy and Miller2013). Sex differences were additionally observed at the level of intrinsic brain network connectivity and might point to different experiential and behavioral tendencies between men and women high in grandiose narcissism (Yang et al., Reference Yang, Cun, Du, Yang, Wang, Wei and Qiu2015). While some of the reviewed findings support the notion that narcissism might be more maladaptive in men than in women (cf. Jauk, Freudenthaler, et al., Reference Jauk, Freudenthaler and Neubauer2016; Morf & Rhodewalt, Reference Morf and Rhodewalt2001), it is currently unknown whether and to which extent this is due to biological or social factors. Though some research points to associations between sex-specific hormones (testosterone; Noser, Reference Noser2017; Pfattheicher, Reference Pfattheicher2016) and narcissism, other research also demonstrates the dynamic nature of these associations, which is dependent upon social factors (Dane et al., Reference Dane, Jonason and McCaffrey2018; Mead et al., Reference Mead, Baumeister, Stuppy and Vohs2018). In section 4, we provide suggestions on how future research might address the question of sex and gender differences in neuroscience studies of narcissism.

The notion of an implicit negative self-view in grandiose narcissism receives partial support from structural imaging studies: Chester, Lynam, and colleagues (Reference Chester, Lynam, Powell and DeWall2016) reported lower integrity of the frontostriatal pathway connecting the ventral striatum and the mPFC. These findings point to a neural disconnect between rewarding and intrapersonal brain areas, which the authors interpret in the way that reduced intrinsically rewarding activity might lead to admiration seeking in the social environment, as posited by self-regulatory models of grandiose narcissism (Back et al., Reference Back, Küfner, Dufner, Gerlach, Rauthmann and Denissen2013; Morf & Rhodewalt, Reference Morf and Rhodewalt2001). This interpretation is consistent with neuroscience research on self-esteem, demonstrating that individuals with high self-esteem display positive rather than negative associations with white matter integrity in the frontostriatal pathway (Chavez & Heatherton, Reference Chavez and Heatherton2017). This might point to a neural dissociation between narcissistically inflated self-esteem and genuinely high self-esteem. This dissociation is particularly interesting given that self-esteem and narcissism are moderately positively related in self-report studies (Campbell, Reference Campbell2001). Neuroscience can thus add a level of analysis that might inform about the underlying psychological processes.

Further structural imaging studies were carried out in NPD patients and groups differing in grandiose and pathological narcissism. The most striking parallel of those studies is that all but one report negative associations between narcissism and different indicators of brain structure; positive associations are hardly evident in the literature. Schulze and colleagues (Reference Schulze, Dziobek, Vater, Heekeren, Bajbouj, Renneberg, Heuser and Roepke2013) found lower gray matter volume in the left AI (for a discussion that also includes other personality disorders, see Schulze & Roepke, Reference Schulze, Roepke, Mulert and Shenton2014) and in the anterior and posterior cingulate cortex, as well as the dorsolateral and mPFC. Nenadic and colleagues (Reference Nenadic, Güllmar, Dietzek, Langbein, Steinke and Gaser2015) found lower gray matter volume in the right middle frontal gyrus, left medial prefrontal/anterior cingulate cortex, left middle occipital cortex, left fusiform/inferior temporal cortex, right superior temporal cortex, and in the left lingual gyrus. Mao and colleagues (Reference Mao, Sang, Wang, Hou, Huang, Wei, Zhang and Qiu2016) found negative relations between pathological narcissism and cortical thickness as well as volume in the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Cortical thickness was further negatively associated with pathological narcissism in the right inferior frontal cortex, cortical volume was negatively associated in the right postcentral gyrus and the left mPFC. Though most of these regions are nonoverlapping, all three studies find effects in the mPFC, which was also implicated in one of the white matter integrity studies discussed above (Chester et al., Reference Chester, Lynam, Powell and DeWall2016). The mPFC, as part of the default mode network (Gusnard et al., Reference Gusnard, Akbudak, Shulman and Raichle2001), is considered central to intrapersonal processing (Wagner et al., Reference Wagner, Haxby and Heatherton2012), specifically to modulating self-referential processing within the default mode network (Davey et al., Reference Davey, Pujol and Harrison2016). Lower gray matter volume in the mPFC is associated with cumulative lifetime adversity and might reflect general disintegration of intrapersonal functions at a brain structural level (Ansell et al., Reference Ansell, Rando, Tuit, Guarnaccia and Sinha2012). These findings are further consistent with multivariate connectivity differences among the mPFC, dACC, and amygdala in grandiose narcissism (Feng et al., Reference Feng, Yuan, Geng, Gu, Zhou, Wu and Luo2018). Though the exact nature of these differences is yet to be uncovered, they point to alterations regarding self-referential processes within the default mode network (mPFC), saliency-related processes (dACC), and emotional processes (fear processing in the amygdala; LeDoux, Reference LeDoux2000). One structural imaging study, however, did not find differences in the mPFC, but reports a positive association between grandiose narcissism in the right superior parietal lobe for women (Yang et al., Reference Yang, Cun, Du, Yang, Wang, Wei and Qiu2015). On the basis of the current research, the findings of this study appear hard to reconcile with the rest. The main differences between this study and the others are that it investigated grandiose narcissism (NPI), whereas the other studies investigated either NPD patients or pathological narcissism (PNI), and that structural correlates were studied in interaction with sex.

When considering all of the available evidence of functional, baseline, and structural studies together, the following main points can be summarized from the available evidence:

  1. (1) Grandiose narcissism is related to neurophysiological stress reactions indicative of vigilance to actual or anticipated ego threat, which points to vulnerable aspects of grandiose narcissism that are not commonly apparent in self-report research. These reactions manifest in increased salience network activation in the dACC, AI, and partly, also in the ventral/subgenual ACC, as well as increased autonomic (sympathetic; systolic blood pressure) and neuroendocrine (cortisol) indicators. Stressful responses to stimuli occur only for self-relevant material; reactions to aversive stimuli that are self-related but not self-relevant can be “cool”.

  2. (2) Grandiose narcissism and NPD are related to altered baseline functioning. This manifests in higher cortisol or 8-OH-DG levels, and distinguishable resting-state connectivity patterns among regions that are also implicated in experimental research (dACC, salience network) as well as the mPFC (default mode network) and the amygdala (limbic system).

  3. (3) Grandiose narcissism, pathological narcissism, and NPD are related to brain structural changes, which almost exclusively manifest in negative associations. The most consistent finding is lower cortical volume/gray matter volume in the mPFC for pathological narcissism/NPD. White matter in the frontostriatal pathway, connecting the ventral striatum and the mPFC, displays lower integrity related to narcissistic grandiosity. Prefrontal white matter tracts are also weakened in NPD.

3.2 Interpersonal functions in narcissism

The study of interpersonal functions in narcissism mostly targets the long-lasting notion of empathy deficits in narcissism, as included in the diagnostic criteria for NPD since its first mention in the DSM-III (American Psychiatric Association, 1980). While lowered empathic functioning, in terms of reduced sharing of others’ affective states, is well documented for all expressions of narcissism (e.g., Lannin et al., Reference Lannin, Guyll, Krizan, Madon and Cornish2014; Pincus et al., Reference Pincus, Ansell, Pimentel, Cain, Wright and Levy2009; Ritter et al., Reference Ritter, Dziobek, Preißler, Rüter, Vater, Fydrich and Roepke2011; Wai & Tiliopoulos, Reference Wai and Tiliopoulos2012), the exact nature of reduced empathy in narcissism is still a matter of debate. Specifically, it has been questioned whether reduced interpersonal functioning is more indicative of reduced capacity or reduced propensity (Baskin-Sommers et al., Reference Baskin-Sommers, Krusemark and Ronningstam2014), with some evidence pointing into the latter direction (as empathic reactions can be increased by interventions; Hepper et al., Reference Hepper, Hart and Sedikides2014). The neuroscience study of narcissism might contribute to an understanding of empathic alterations, as it provides objective assessments of the respective activations complementary to self-report data.

From the literature reviewed above, it can be concluded that individuals with higher levels of pathological and grandiose narcissism generally display lowered activation in brain regions typically implicated in empathizing, particularly the AI, when using high-level social affect paradigms (Fan et al., Reference Fan, Wonneberger, Enzi, de Greck, Ulrich, Tempelmann, Bogerts, Doering and Northoff2011; Scalabrini et al., Reference Scalabrini, Huang, Mucci, Perrucci, Ferretti, Fossati and Ebisch2017). These findings were, on the one hand, explained along the line of greater alexithymia in pathological narcissism (Fan et al., Reference Fan, Wonneberger, Enzi, de Greck, Ulrich, Tempelmann, Bogerts, Doering and Northoff2011), and on the other hand , as greater self-related attention in grandiose narcissism (Scalabrini et al., Reference Scalabrini, Huang, Mucci, Perrucci, Ferretti, Fossati and Ebisch2017).

Regarding the first of these interpretations, alexithymia, in terms of a general incapacity to identify emotions, was also observed to mediate empathy deficits in aggressive offenders (Winter et al., Reference Winter, Spengler, Bermpohl, Singer and Kanske2017), and is currently being discussed as a general, transdiagnostic functional mechanism of reduced empathy, which could also play a role in borderline personality disorder or psychopathy (Valdespino et al., Reference Valdespino, Antezana, Ghane and Richey2017), depression (Hoffmann et al., Reference Hoffmann, Banzhaf, Kanske, Gärtner, Bermpohl and Singer2016), and autism (Bird et al., Reference Bird, Silani, Brindley, White, Frith and Singer2010). With respect to the question of reduced propensity or capacity for empathic reactions in narcissism, the findings of Fan and colleagues (Reference Fan, Wonneberger, Enzi, de Greck, Ulrich, Tempelmann, Bogerts, Doering and Northoff2011) could, tentatively speaking, be seen as more in line with the capacity than the propensity hypothesis (see also Baskin-Sommers et al., Reference Baskin-Sommers, Krusemark and Ronningstam2014). Participants were explicitly instructed to empathize with the faces they saw, and high versus low narcissistic groups gave similar empathy ratings. Yet, they differed in their neural activation, which means that different levels of brain activation were observed under similar conditions. This notion is substantiated by the observation that empathy ratings and activation in the respective neural network are usually positively associated (Kanske et al., Reference Kanske, Böckler, Trautwein and Singer2015). However, alternative explanations cannot be ruled out. For instance, different activation strengths might be accompanied by similar affective experiences (for a parallel from cognitive research, see Neubauer & Fink, Reference Neubauer and Fink2009), or individuals with higher narcissism levels might provide biased empathy ratings, as this is the case for self-perceptions of general emotional competence (Jauk, Freudenthaler, et al., Reference Jauk, Freudenthaler and Neubauer2016; Mota et al., Reference Mota, Leckelt, Geukes, Nestler, Humberg, Schröder-Abé, Schmukle and Back2019). While these interpretative problems are partially epistemological in nature, future research could compare spontaneous and deliberate empathic reactions at a neurophysiological level to further elucidate the nature of reduced empathy in narcissism.

In the second high-level social affect study of narcissism that has been conducted to date, Scalabrini and colleagues (Reference Scalabrini, Huang, Mucci, Perrucci, Ferretti, Fossati and Ebisch2017) also observed lower activation in the right AI during anticipation of touching a human versus mannequin hand. This is generally in line with the previously discussed findings of Fan and colleagues (Reference Fan, Wonneberger, Enzi, de Greck, Ulrich, Tempelmann, Bogerts, Doering and Northoff2011), as both points to reduced responses in the AI to social stimuli in narcissism. The interpretation, however, differs from the previous one: Scalabrini and colleagues used complemental resting-state scans, which indicated a higher long-range temporal correlation in the AI in individuals higher in grandiose narcissism. The authors interpret this finding in terms of increased self-focus during resting state, particularly with respect to interoceptive and bodily self-aspects. This can be seen as at least partially supporting a hypothesis proposed by Jankowiak-Siuda and Zajkowksi (Reference Jankowiak-Siuda and Zajkowski2013), according to which defective function of the AI, as part of the salience network, leads to a higher likelihood of engaging default mode, self-referential processes in more narcissistic individuals. However, in the study by Scalabrini and colleagues (Reference Scalabrini, Huang, Mucci, Perrucci, Ferretti, Fossati and Ebisch2017), it was the AI itself, which displayed increased long-range temporal correlation during resting state. Finally, the alexithymia interpretation by Fan and colleagues (Reference Fan, Wonneberger, Enzi, de Greck, Ulrich, Tempelmann, Bogerts, Doering and Northoff2011) and the interoception – interpretation by Scalabrini and colleagues (Reference Scalabrini, Huang, Mucci, Perrucci, Ferretti, Fossati and Ebisch2017) might be hard to reconcile, as alexithymia and interoceptive abilities are themselves negatively related (Brewer et al., Reference Brewer, Cook and Bird2016). Thus, for now, it can be concluded that both pathological and grandiose narcissism (grandiose factor of the PNI) are associated with relative deactivation of the AI during high-level social affect paradigms, though the exact nature of this phenomenon must remain subject to future studies.

Turning now to low level, more basic experimental paradigms, two EEG studies found that sensory processing of other related stimuli is not generally impaired in NPD (Marcoux et al., Reference Marcoux, Michon, Lemelin, Voisin, Vachon-Presseau and Jackson2014; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Shen, Zhu, Ma and Wang2016). The first of these studies showed that NPD patients even display an increased somatosensory gating effect to observing others’ pain, that is, a stronger reduction of own somatosensory activity (P3 component) when anticipating or observing others’ pain (Marcoux et al., Reference Marcoux, Michon, Lemelin, Voisin, Vachon-Presseau and Jackson2014). The second study found no neurophysiological differences in the processing of emotional faces between NPD patients and healthy controls, though the amount of depression reported by NPD negatively modulated P2 amplitudes to neutral and happy faces, which shows that depression amplifies self-focus in NPD (Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Shen, Zhu, Ma and Wang2016). Taken together, these studies show that NPD patients do not generally show low-level sensory deficits in the processing of social stimuli, but comorbid symptoms must be considered when studying NPD patients.

Finally, the social–psychological line of neuroscience research on narcissism points to dynamic interactions between narcissism, testosterone, and the subjective experience of power: those with higher baseline testosterone levels were found to score higher on the NPI following a social power manipulation (Mead et al., Reference Mead, Baumeister, Stuppy and Vohs2018), which aligns with the finding that – the other way around – testosterone increases in more narcissistic men after acting deceptively (Dane et al., Reference Dane, Jonason and McCaffrey2018). Testosterone is also correlated with narcissism at baseline in men (Noser, Reference Noser2017; Pfattheicher, Reference Pfattheicher2016). Taken together, these findings point to the unbowed socially assertive and antagonistic face of narcissism, which likely arises in the absence of a subjective threat to the self.

When considering all of the available evidence together, the following main points can be summarized regarding interpersonal aspects:

  1. (1) Individuals with higher levels of pathological and grandiose narcissism display lowered reactions to social stimuli in the AI in high-level paradigms. The AI also shows altered baseline activity in relation to narcissism, and is implicated in intrapersonal processing in narcissism. Though the results seem to be more in line with the notion of a reduced capacity than a reduced propensity for empathic reactions, more research will be needed to elucidate the involved mechanisms.

  2. (2) Low-level social information processing, as investigated in electrophysiological studies, is likely not affected by narcissism. However, when studying patients with NPD, comorbid disorders, such as depression, might also alter these processes. This is consistent with the notion that interpersonal functioning in narcissism can substantially vary between different states.

  3. (3) The assertive and antagonistic interpersonal style associated with grandiose narcissism is evident in neuroendocrine indicators such as increased testosterone levels. Research on neuroendocrine markers associated with narcissism also points to dynamic situational interactions.

3.3 Overall conclusion and conceptual integration

One of the main conclusions that can be drawn from the reviewed research is that intrapersonal functioning in grandiose narcissism is accompanied by heightened vigilance to ego threat and stress responses following subjective ego threat, and also heightened stress indicators at a baseline level (Cascio et al., Reference Cascio, Konrath and Falk2015; Cheng et al., Reference Cheng, Tracy and Miller2013; Edelstein et al., Reference Edelstein, Yim and Quas2010; Jauk, Benedek, et al., Reference Jauk, Benedek, Koschutnig, Kedia and Neubauer2017; Reinhard et al., Reference Reinhard, Konrath, Lopez and Cameron2012; Sommer et al., Reference Sommer, Kirkland, Newman, Estrella and Andreassi2009). These findings are consistent with the threatened egotism model of narcissism (Bushman & Baumeister, Reference Bushman and Baumeister1998) and demonstrate that ego threat may not only have negative consequences for others (in terms of aggressive behavior) but also for the self. Importantly, stress reactions are not necessarily apparent in self-report research and point to vulnerable aspects of grandiose narcissism, which might either not be accessible to, or not reported by individuals high in narcissism. This is in line with behavioral research demonstrating overestimation of emotion-related abilities in narcissism (Ames & Kammrath, Reference Ames and Kammrath2004; Jauk, Freudenthaler, et al., Reference Jauk, Freudenthaler and Neubauer2016; John & Robins, Reference John and Robins1994; Lobbestael et al., Reference Lobbestael, de Bruin, Kok and Voncken2016; Mota et al., Reference Mota, Leckelt, Geukes, Nestler, Humberg, Schröder-Abé, Schmukle and Back2019; Zajenkowski et al., Reference Zajenkowski, Maciantowicz, Szymaniak and Urban2018). Also, it is in line with the conclusion of a recent review of stress reactivity in narcissism (Coleman et al., Reference Coleman, Pincus and Smyth2019), and shows that it extends to a broader spectrum of research paradigms and methods. The conclusion is also mostly in line with earlier chapters, which could not yet draw on a large amount of studies specific to narcissism (Konrath & Bonadonna, Reference Konrath, Bonadonna and Besser2014; Krusemark, Reference Krusemark, Campbell and Miller2012), and a recent chapter (Krusemark, Reference Krusemark, Hermann, Brunell and Foster2018). In particular, the present review shows that self-relevant stimuli, even when not necessarily stressful, can lead to similar reactions as those that are considered intrinsically stressful or threatening. Individuals high in grandiose narcissism show similar neural reactions in parts of the salience network and also similar “hard wiring” of the frontostriatal pathway as do individuals with low self-esteem (Chavez & Heatherton, Reference Chavez and Heatherton2017; Chester et al., Reference Chester, Lynam, Powell and DeWall2016; Jauk, Benedek, et al., Reference Jauk, Benedek, Koschutnig, Kedia and Neubauer2017; Onoda et al., Reference Onoda, Okamoto, Nakashima, Nittono, Yoshimura, Yamawaki, Yamaguchi and Ura2010).

Importantly, self-relevance is a crucial situational variable in this equation, as higher narcissistic individuals do not show stress reactivity (Kelsey et al., Reference Kelsey, Ornduff, McCann and Reiff2001; Sylvers et al., Reference Sylvers, Brubaker, Alden, Brennan and Lilienfeld2008), or even cool and prepared reactions facing potentially stress-inducing stimuli which that are not of relevance to the self (Kelsey et al., Reference Kelsey, Ornduff, McCann and Reiff2001). Individuals higher in grandiose narcissism also make riskier decisions, accompanied by weaker neurophysiological reactions (Yang, Sedikides, Gu, Luo, Wang, & Cai, Reference Yang, Sedikides, Gu, Luo, Wang and Cai2018), and show reactions indicative of lower stress or even enjoyment following deceptive behavior (Dane et al., Reference Dane, Jonason and McCaffrey2018). These latter findings might point to common mechanisms underlying certain aspects of narcissism and the psychopathy construct (traditionally characterized by “fearlessness”; Hare & Neumann, Reference Hare and Neumann2008).

Interpersonal personality functioning, particularly empathic reactions, is reduced at a behavioral and neurophysiological level when assessed by high-level paradigms and measures (Fan et al., Reference Fan, Wonneberger, Enzi, de Greck, Ulrich, Tempelmann, Bogerts, Doering and Northoff2011; Scalabrini et al., Reference Scalabrini, Huang, Mucci, Perrucci, Ferretti, Fossati and Ebisch2017). More low-level, sensory processing of other related information appears to be intact in NPD (Marcoux et al., Reference Marcoux, Michon, Lemelin, Voisin, Vachon-Presseau and Jackson2014; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Shen, Zhu, Ma and Wang2016), in terms of the most extreme expression of grandiose narcissism, though comorbid disorders (depression) might also alter low-level processes (Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Shen, Zhu, Ma and Wang2016).

Interestingly, intra- and interpersonal functioning in narcissism partially draw on similar neural networks, as evident in functional, baseline, and structural imaging studies (see Figure 2). The dACC, as part of the salience network (Bressler & Menon, Reference Bressler and Menon2010), shows relatively higher activation in intrapersonal processing of actual or anticipated ego threat in individuals with higher grandiose narcissism (Cascio et al., Reference Cascio, Konrath and Falk2015; Jauk, Benedek, et al., Reference Jauk, Benedek, Koschutnig, Kedia and Neubauer2017). The AI, which is also considered as a part of the salience network (Bressler & Menon, Reference Bressler and Menon2010) and implicated in empathic processes (Lamm et al., Reference Lamm, Decety and Singer2011), shows relatively lower activation during interpersonal paradigms in highly grandiose and pathologically narcissistic individuals (Fan et al., Reference Fan, Wonneberger, Enzi, de Greck, Ulrich, Tempelmann, Bogerts, Doering and Northoff2011; Scalabrini et al., Reference Scalabrini, Huang, Mucci, Perrucci, Ferretti, Fossati and Ebisch2017), and it shows altered baseline functioning in individuals higher in grandiose narcissism (Scalabrini et al., Reference Scalabrini, Huang, Mucci, Perrucci, Ferretti, Fossati and Ebisch2017). Taken together, a very outright interpretation of these findings could be that processing of intrapersonal stimuli in narcissism is accompanied by increased saliency, particularly when these are self-relevant and thus go along with potential ego threat. Processing of other related, interpersonal stimuli, however, seems to be associated with decreased saliency. Activation differences within the salience network thus point to a double dissociation regarding the expression of narcissism and self- and other relatedness of the situation (see Figure 2). This interpretation is partially consistent with a hypothesis proposed by Jankowiak-Siuda and Zajkowski (Reference Jankowiak-Siuda and Zajkowski2013), who also ascribed the salience network a central role in relation to narcissism (though other predictions cannot be evaluated on the basis of the present research), and with a conceptual article pointing to the associations among AI function, narcissism, empathy, and general emotional awareness (i.e., alexithymia; George & Short, Reference George and Short2018), which could also be seen as one potential underlying mechanism (see above; Valdespino et al., Reference Valdespino, Antezana, Ghane and Richey2017).

Figure 2. Schematic depiction of central findings of neuroscience studies on narcissism. Dashed lines indicating the moderating influence of narcissism (see section 3) on particular effects. Activation within central nodes of the salience network (orange) and reactions in autonomic (sympathetic) or neuroendocrine markers are higher in situations involving intrapersonal focus when self-relevance is high (potential ego threat). Reactions to aversive self-related stimuli of low self-relevance can be “cool”. Situations involving interpersonal focus (empathy, social touch) lead to down-regulations. At a brain structural and baseline function level, narcissism is related to alterations in regions of the default mode network (cyan) and in the frontostriatal pathway (purple) as well as neuroendocrine markers.

Interestingly, the dACC was also among the key nodes found to display altered patterns of resting-state connectivity in individuals with higher levels of grandiose narcissism (Feng et al., Reference Feng, Yuan, Geng, Gu, Zhou, Wu and Luo2018). Among the other nodes in this study was also the dorsal and ventral mPFC (see Figure 2). The mPFC displayed lower gray matter volume in three out of the four morphometric studies (NPD and pathological narcissism), and is connected to the ventral striatum via the frontostriatal pathway (see Figure 2), which showed decreased white matter integrity in relation to grandiose narcissism (Chester et al., Reference Chester, Lynam, Powell and DeWall2016). The mPFC is considered as a part of the default mode network (Gusnard et al., Reference Gusnard, Akbudak, Shulman and Raichle2001) and central to intrapersonal processing (Davey et al., Reference Davey, Pujol and Harrison2016; Wagner et al., Reference Wagner, Haxby and Heatherton2012). Lower mPFC volume could indicate a general disintegration of intrapersonal functions, as it is also related to cumulative lifetime adversity (Ansell et al., Reference Ansell, Rando, Tuit, Guarnaccia and Sinha2012). Lower white matter integrity between the ventral striatum and the mPFC could further indicate a neural disconnect between regions associated with reward and self-processing (Chester et al., Reference Chester, Lynam, Powell and DeWall2016).

Taken together, neuroimaging findings point to functional and structural alterations of regions within the salience network (dACC, AI) and the default mode network (mPFC). Activity within the salience network points to a double dissociation of narcissism and self- versus other related processes.

Some of the conclusions presented here for intra- and interpersonal functioning in narcissism have, from different perspectives or with different emphases, also been expressed in previous integrative works. While we cannot reiterate all points addressed in previous reviews here, we want to highlight some central agreements with recent works: Coleman and colleagues (Reference Coleman, Pincus and Smyth2019) have recently reviewed studies investigating stress reactivity (psychological, biological, and behavioral) in narcissism, concluding that grandiosity, as well as vulnerability, are generally associated with increased stress reactivity. Krusemark (Reference Krusemark, Hermann, Brunell and Foster2018) came to a similar conclusion, stating that “grandiose narcissism involves cardiovascular reactivity in response to aversive stimuli and achievement-related stressors” and “vulnerable narcissism and exploitativeness/entitlement are generally associated with heightened cardiovascular reactivity to interpersonal rejection” (Reference Krusemark, Hermann, Brunell and Foster2018, p. 213). However, Coleman and colleagues further argue that “considerable nuance in these associations appears to exist (e.g., what contexts/stressors) […], and under certain stressful conditions narcissism (particularly grandiosity) may confer some level of resilience” (Reference Coleman, Pincus and Smyth2019, p. 61). We here refer to these moderating factors as self-relatedness and self-relevance, putting forward the hypothesis that increased salience network up-regulation or stress responses are only observed when both conditions are met. Potentially aversive stimuli or situations that are self-related, but not self-relevant might lead to unaffected responses, or as Coleman and colleagues (Reference Coleman, Pincus and Smyth2019) further assert, individuals high in grandiosity might have advantages in such situations. Similarly, Krusemark referred to these unaffected responses as a “hypo-reactive physiological profile” (Reference Krusemark, Hermann, Brunell and Foster2018, p. 215). Importantly, given that each of us experiences potentially ego-threatening situations, and – as we have argued – individuals high in narcissism might be even more prone to assign self-relevance to such situations, we hypothesize that this might be evident in chronic alterations of neuroendocrine systems or also brain structure in individuals high in narcissism. Krusemark (Reference Krusemark, Hermann, Brunell and Foster2018) also asserts that increased physiological reactivity associated with narcissism is accompanied by a heightened risk for cardiovascular disease, and similarly, Coleman and colleagues state that “maladaptive stress-reactivity, particularly when repeated often and over long periods, may cause wear-and-tear on bodily systems that ultimately hasten disease onset and progression” (Reference Coleman, Pincus and Smyth2019, p. 67). In addition to that, as Konrath and Bonadonna (Reference Konrath, Bonadonna and Besser2014) further note, narcissism is a “risk-positive personality style” (such as, for instance, also evident in the gambling study reviewed here; Yang, Sedikides, Gu, Luo, Wang, & Cai, Reference Yang, Sedikides, Gu, Luo, Wang and Cai2018), which is accompanied by a broad variety of potentially self-defeating, health-relevant behaviors such as sexual risk-taking, substance use, or risky driving. Thus, in addition to physical health risks imposed by increased stress reactivity, narcissism is also associated with a variety of lifestyle factors that might lead to serious health risks over the lifespan (Konrath & Bonadonna, Reference Konrath, Bonadonna and Besser2014).

Regarding interpersonal functioning in narcissism, George and Short (Reference George and Short2018) as well as Di Sarno and colleagues (Reference Di Sarno, Di Pierro and Madeddu2018) recently highlighted the role of the AI regarding empathic alterations in NPD. George and Short (Reference George and Short2018) further pinpoint that the AI is also involved in general emotional awareness, making alexithymia a possible common mechanism of self- and other related emotional deficits in narcissism. As discussed earlier (see section 3.2), while the AI and more generally the salience network might play a key role in self- versus other related processing in narcissism, the alexithymia interpretation (also originally put forward by Fan et al., Reference Fan, Wonneberger, Enzi, de Greck, Ulrich, Tempelmann, Bogerts, Doering and Northoff2011) might be hard to reconcile with the notion of increased interoception (Scalabrini et al., Reference Scalabrini, Huang, Mucci, Perrucci, Ferretti, Fossati and Ebisch2017), which is why the exact mechanisms of emotion-related alterations in narcissism must remain subject to future study.

Given these areas of overlap with related works, we think that the main contribution of our review is to integrate the available neuroscience literature on narcissism in a theoretical model spanning a broad variety of relevant psychological processes as well as a broad variety of indicators used in neuroscience research. Though such a model cannot adequately depict every finding obtained so far, it is an attempt to identify key topics which are evident across a wide range of studies and previous theoretical works, condense them into testable hypotheses, and put them into a larger perspective, thus moving one step beyond approaches which are either confined to a specific psychological process or a specific set of research methods. Whether or not the assumptions expressed in this model will hold true, we think that providing testable hypotheses is a crucial next step for future neuroscience works on narcissism, which we hope our work does encourage (see following section 4).

4. Future Directions

Though existing neuroscience research could already contribute significantly to the understanding of narcissism, there are several ways in which future studies could corroborate and extend the present findings. Here, we provide suggestions on how this might be accomplished:

  1. (1) Our main suggestion for future research is to consider the specificity of findings within the multifaceted narcissism construct, which is why we recommend using measures of grandiosity and vulnerability when assessing narcissism. This can either be achieved by using separate scales, such as the long-standing standard measures NPI and HSNS, or by using more recently developed composite measures, such as the Five-Factor Narcissism Inventory (FFNI), which allows for a comprehensive assessment of different aspects of narcissism grounded in the Five-Factor Model dimensions (Glover et al., Reference Glover, Miller, Lynam, Crego and Widiger2012). The recent EEG study by Mück and colleagues (Mück et al., Reference Mück, Ohmann, Dummel, Mattes, Thesing and Stahl2020) exemplifies the necessity of considering these, as opposing results were observed for agentic and antagonistic narcissism. For researchers who seek guidance in the choice of scales, Krizan and Herlache (Reference Krizan and Herlache2018) provide an in-depth discussion of the available inventories. In most measures, grandiosity and vulnerability are essentially unrelated in the general population, which easily allows for the sampling of orthogonal factors or groups. Of note, some research suggests that the correlation between grandiosity and vulnerability increases toward the upper end of the grandiosity distribution, which was observed for the NPI/HSNS (Jauk, Weigle, et al., Reference Jauk, Weigle, Lehmann, Benedek and Neubauer2017) as well as for the FFNI (Jauk & Kaufman, Reference Jauk and Kaufman2018). This implies that extreme group comparisons, such as performed in some of the reviewed works (Fan et al., Reference Fan, Wonneberger, Enzi, de Greck, Ulrich, Tempelmann, Bogerts, Doering and Northoff2011; Jauk, Benedek, et al., Reference Jauk, Benedek, Koschutnig, Kedia and Neubauer2017; Kelsey, Ornduff, McCann & Reiff, Reference Kelsey, Ornduff, McCann and Reiff2001; Krusemark, Reference Krusemark2009), might lead to a confounding of grandiose and vulnerable aspects. Similar, yet even more intense confounding might arise when studying NPD patients, who frequently have comorbid disorders such as depression (Vater et al., Reference Vater, Ritter, Schröder-Abé, Schütz, Lammers, Bosson and Roepke2013; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Shen, Zhu, Ma and Wang2016). Also, when using the PNI as a measure of pathological narcissism in neuroscience research, one should bear in mind that the close conceptual relation of grandiosity and vulnerability in this construct goes along with high empirical correlations of the respective factors (cf. Wright, Lukowitsky, Pincus, & Conroy, Reference Wright, Lukowitsky, Pincus and Conroy2010), which might not allow to disentangle their specific effects unless using very large samples. Finally, given potential state fluctuations in narcissism, future neuroscience research could also use momentary assessments of grandiosity and vulnerability, either in the lab or in everyday life.

  2. (2) Our second suggestion for future research is to consider the specificity of findings with respect to neighboring personality constructs such as psychopathy or antisocial personality, Machiavellianism, borderline personality, or histrionic personality. More generally speaking, a task for future research in the field might be to investigate whether the effects observed for narcissism are really specific to this particular personality construct, or might also be explained along the lines of general impairments in personality functioning. To date, only a few among the neuroscience studies of narcissism assessed related personality dimensions (Dane et al., Reference Dane, Jonason and McCaffrey2018; Pfattheicher, Reference Pfattheicher2016; Sylvers et al., Reference Sylvers, Brubaker, Alden, Brennan and Lilienfeld2008) or disorders (Marcoux et al., Reference Marcoux, Michon, Lemelin, Voisin, Vachon-Presseau and Jackson2014; Schulze et al., Reference Schulze, Dziobek, Vater, Heekeren, Bajbouj, Renneberg, Heuser and Roepke2013; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Wang, Fu, Liu, He, Dong and Jang2006; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Shen, Zhu, Ma and Wang2016) to probe the specificity of the findings. It might be the case that particularly those findings which cannot unequivocally be related to a specifically narcissistic self-regulatory or interpersonal mechanism, such as baseline alterations of neuroendocrine markers or measures of brain structure, might rather be indicative of general impairments in personality functioning than narcissism per se.

  3. (3) Regarding sex differences that are evident in some of the reviewed studies (Edelstein et al., Reference Edelstein, Yim and Quas2010; Jauk, Benedek, et al., Reference Jauk, Benedek, Koschutnig, Kedia and Neubauer2017; Reinhard et al., Reference Reinhard, Konrath, Lopez and Cameron2012; Yang et al., Reference Yang, Cun, Du, Yang, Wang, Wei and Qiu2015), future research will need to replicate and extend these findings. This concerns on the one hand their consistency, and on the other hand, the extent to which differences are associated with biological sex or social gender. Though some of the findings reviewed here suggest that biological mechanisms might be involved (Noser, Reference Noser2017; Pfattheicher, Reference Pfattheicher2016), others show that these associations also depend upon social factors (Dane et al., Reference Dane, Jonason and McCaffrey2018; Mead et al., Reference Mead, Baumeister, Stuppy and Vohs2018). Thus, the social component in terms of gender role orientation must not be missed, and only studies assessing both dimensions can inform about their relative significance.

  4. (4) The study of interpersonal personality functioning in narcissism could systematically investigate the extent to which social affect and cognition, particularly empathy and perspective-taking, which are increasingly recognized as two different mental processes with different neural correlates (Kanske et al., Reference Kanske, Böckler, Trautwein, Parianen Lesemann and Singer2016, Reference Kanske, Böckler, Trautwein and Singer2015), are reduced or impaired in grandiose and vulnerable narcissism. This could be accomplished by contrasting behavioral ratings of empathy and performance measures of perspective-taking with activation in the respective neural networks (see sections 2.1 and 3.2), which might provide some hints on the question of whether reductions in interpersonal functioning are more a matter of reduced propensity or capacity. A possible next step could then be to extend this line of research by considering differences between spontaneous and deliberate social–affective and cognitive processes, which could be accomplished by a variation of test instructions, for instance.

  5. (5) Though this might apply to many fields of neuroscience investigations, it needs to be stated that currently, experimental and also baseline and brain structural studies on narcissism used nearly as many different research paradigms or methods as there are studies. To obtain dependable knowledge, future research should thus perform replication and extension studies using established paradigms and research methods. From the present perspective, particularly fruitful next steps would be to systematically disentangle the effects of grandiosity and vulnerability as well as their constituent Five-Factor Model dimensions (see the first point) on well-established ego threat paradigms, such as, for instance, the TSST or the Cyberball paradigm, and well-established social affect and cognition paradigms. Quasi-experimental studies using measures of brain structure or baseline functions could contribute most effectively to acquiring a robust base of knowledge by using data analysis methods corresponding exactly to those of previous studies (at least complemental to new methods) in large datasets. While such studies have recently been carried out for the Five-Factor Model dimensions and yield a rather pessimistic picture regarding the consistency of personality neuroscience findings (Avinun et al., Reference Avinun, Israel, Knodt and Hariri2020; Valk et al., Reference Valk, Hoffstaedter, Camilleri, Kochunov, Thomas Yeo and Eickhoff2019), it might be that narcissism, as a more specific personality construct, might still display consistent correlates.

  6. (6) Finally, it needs to be stated that many neuroscience studies of narcissism used small or very small samples to investigate genuinely subtle associations between personality and physiological or biological indicators, which sometimes include complex relationships such as moderation. This may naturally raise doubt about the robustness and replicability of the reported findings, and we think that such doubt is vital for state-of-the-art planning of future research. However, despite the use of small samples – which is a common problem particularly in earlier neuroscience work – the relatively high degree of consistency across different studies still allows to deduce testable hypotheses, which can inform future confirmatory research using state-of-the-art designs. The model displayed in Figure 2 is an attempt to provide such hypotheses based on the available literature, and we hope it will stimulate future research, which is in line with current methodological standards.

Acknowledgments

The comments provided by the journal editor and the anonymous reviewers during the revision of this manuscript are gratefully acknowledged. We thank Sophie Jacob for her very valuable support in literature search and final corrections, and Ilona Croy for hosting a writing retreat during which part of this text evolved.

Conflict of interest

None.

Financial support

This research was supported by a grant from the Austrian Science Fund (FWF): J 4344 and by a grant from the German Research Foundation (DFG): CRC940/C07.

Footnotes

1 “Empathy” is used for both, social–affective and social–cognitive functions in the AMPD. We will differentiate these functions below.

2 Note, however, that the salience network as such likely does not have a single unitary function (Seeley, Reference Seeley2019), and the results reported here can only be interpreted in the particular context. Please see sections 2.3.1 and 3.1 for a more in-depth discussion.

3 However, the grandiosity factor of the PNI comprises the conceptually related self-sacrificing self-enhancement scale (see section 1.3).

4 Scalabrini et al. (Reference Scalabrini, Huang, Mucci, Perrucci, Ferretti, Fossati and Ebisch2017) used experimental and baseline measures; Yang et al. (Reference Yang, Cun, Du, Yang, Wang, Wei and Qiu2015) used structural and baseline measures.

5 One more study used the PNI, but only its grandiose subscale (Scalabrini et al., Reference Scalabrini, Huang, Mucci, Perrucci, Ferretti, Fossati and Ebisch2017).

6 For a comprehensive discussion of both components, see Mück et al., Reference Mück, Ohmann, Dummel, Mattes, Thesing and Stahl2020.

7 Diagnostic criteria in the categorical system have remained unchanged from DSM-IV to DSM-5.

8 In the second experiment, which was part of this study, the same participants saw affective picture slides. Here, an unexpected positive finding emerged, as those higher in grandiose narcissism showed stronger negative emotional reactions (as evident in negative relations with respiratory sinus arrhythmia and pre-ejection period shortening) toward happy pictures, which the authors interpreted as signs of negative reactions or envy toward others’ happiness. Subjective measures of momentary affect were not assessed in this study.

9 We classified this study as mainly intrapersonal in nature, as participants were videotaped during lying, and not supposed to engage social–affective and cognitive capabilities toward a real other person. However, given that the task likely involves anticipation of imagined others’ reactions, this study could also fit in the interpersonal category.

10 The NI comprises the four dimensions: (1) threatened self, (2) “classical” narcissistic self, (3) idealist self, and (4) hypochondric self, each with several subscales. Though convergent validity information between the NI and scales that are more widely used in psychological research (e.g., NPI, HSNS, PNI) is relatively sparse, the scale descriptions and correlations suggest that the NI measures pathological narcissism, encompassing both aspects of grandiosity (mean correlation with NPI: r = .35; Rohmann, Hanke, & Bierhoff, Reference Rohmann, Hanke and Bierhoff2019) and, to a somewhat stronger extent, vulnerability (correlation with HSNS: r = .54; Footnote ibid.).

11 Note that the PNI grandiosity subscale likely encompasses more vulnerable aspects than other measures of grandiose narcissism, such as for instance, the NPI, as “structural analyses suggest that even the purported grandiose aspects are closer to the entitlement-vulnerability side of the spectrum” (Krizan & Herlache, Reference Krizan and Herlache2018, p. 18). See also section 4.

12 We categorized this study as baseline as the experimental paradigm employed low-level, non-self-related, and non-social auditory stimuli and no voluntary response was required from participants.

References

Ackerman, R. A., Witt, E. A., Donnellan, M. B., Trzesniewski, K. H., Robins, R. W., & Kashy, D. A. (2011). What does the narcissistic personality inventory really measure? Assessment, 18, 6787. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191110382845 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Akhtar, S. (1989). Narcissistic personality disorder: Descriptive features and differential diagnosis. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 12, 505529. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0193-953X(18)30411-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). American Psychiatric Association. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596 Google Scholar
Ames, D. R., & Kammrath, L. K. (2004). Mind-reading and metacognition: Narcissism, not actual competence, predicts self-estimated ability. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 28, 187209. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:JONB.0000039649.20015.0e CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ansell, E. B., Rando, K., Tuit, K., Guarnaccia, J., & Sinha, R. (2012). Cumulative adversity and smaller gray matter volume in medial prefrontal, anterior cingulate, and insula regions. Biological Psychiatry, 72, 5764. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.11.022 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Avinun, R., Israel, S., Knodt, A. R., & Hariri, A. R. (2020). Little evidence for associations between the big five personality traits and variability in brain gray or white matter. NeuroImage, 220, 117092. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117092 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Back, M. D., Küfner, A. C. P., Dufner, M., Gerlach, T. M., Rauthmann, J. F., & Denissen, J. J. A. (2013). Narcissistic admiration and rivalry: Disentangling the bright and dark sides of narcissism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 105, 10131037. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034431 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Back, M. D., Schmukle, S. C., & Egloff, B. (2010). Why are narcissists so charming at first sight? Decoding the narcissism–popularity link at zero acquaintance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 132145. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016338 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2004). The empathy quotient: an investigation of adults with asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 163175. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:JADD.0000022607.19833.00 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barry, C. T., & Lee-Rowland, L. M. (2015). Has there been a recent increase in adolescent narcissism? Evidence from a sample of at-risk adolescents (2005–2014). Personality and Individual Differences, 87, 153157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.07.038 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baskin-Sommers, A., Krusemark, E., & Ronningstam, E. (2014). Empathy in narcissistic personality disorder: From clinical and empirical perspectives. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 5, 323333. https://doi.org/10.1037/per0000061 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Basser, P. J., Mattiello, J., & LeBihan, D. (1994). MR diffusion tensor spectroscopy and imaging. Biophysical Journal, 66, 259267. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3495(94)80775-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumeister, R. F., Smart, L., & Boden, J. M. (1996). Relation of threatened egotism to violence and aggression: The dark side of high self-esteem. Psychological Review, 103, 533. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.103.1.5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bell, M. (1995). Bell object relations and reality testing inventory (BORRTI) manual. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
Bender, D. S., Morey, L. C., & Skodol, A. E. (2011). Toward a model for assessing level of personality functioning in DSM–5, part I: A review of theory and methods. Journal of Personality Assessment, 93, 332346. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2011.583808 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Besser, A., & Priel, B. (2010). Grandiose narcissism versus vulnerable narcissism in threatening situations: Emotional reactions to achievement failure and interpersonal rejection. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 29, 874902. https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2010.29.8.874 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bilotta, E., Carcione, A., Fera, T., Moroni, F., Nicolò, G., Pedone, R., Pellecchia, G., Semerari, A., & Colle, L. (2018). Symptom severity and mindreading in narcissistic personality disorder. PLoS One, 13, e0201216. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0201216 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bird, G., Silani, G., Brindley, R., White, S., Frith, U., & Singer, T. (2010). Empathic brain responses in insula are modulated by levels of alexithymia but not autism. Brain, 133, 15151525. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awq060 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Böckler, A., Sharifi, M., Kanske, P., Dziobek, I., & Singer, T. (2017). Social decision making in narcissism: Reduced generosity and increased retaliation are driven by alterations in perspective-taking and anger. Personality and Individual Differences, 104, 17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.07.020 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bressler, S. L., & Menon, V. (2010). Large-scale brain networks in cognition: Emerging methods and principles. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 277290. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.04.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brewer, R., Cook, R., & Bird, G. (2016). Alexithymia: A general deficit of interoception. Royal Society Open Science, 3, 150664. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.150664 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brookes, J. (2015). The effect of overt and covert narcissism on self-esteem and self-efficacy beyond self-esteem. Personality and Individual Differences, 85, 172175. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.05.013 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bukowski, W. M., Schwartzman, A., Santo, J., Bagwell, C., & Adams, R. (2009). Reactivity and distortions in the self: Narcissism, types of aggression, and the functioning of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis during early adolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 21, 12491262. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579409990149 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bushman, B. J., & Baumeister, R. F. (1998). Threatened egotism, narcissism, self-esteem, and direct and displaced aggression: Does self-love or self-hate lead to violence? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 219229. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.75.1.219 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buss, D. M., & Chiodo, L. M. (1991). Narcissistic acts in everyday life. Journal of Personality, 59, 179215. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1991.tb00773.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bzdok, D., Schilbach, L., Vogeley, K., Schneider, K., Laird, A. R., Langner, R., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2012). Parsing the neural correlates of moral cognition: ALE meta-analysis on morality, theory of mind, and empathy. Brain Structure and Function, 217, 783796. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-012-0380-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cain, N. M., Pincus, A. L., & Ansell, E. B. (2008). Narcissism at the crossroads: Phenotypic description of pathological narcissism across clinical theory, social/personality psychology, and psychiatric diagnosis. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 638656. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2007.09.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, W. K. (2001). A meta-analysis of the narcissism–self-esteem link. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Campbell, W. K., Bosson, J. K., Goheen, T. W., Lakey, C. E., & Kernis, M. H. (2007). Do narcissists dislike themselves “deep down inside”? Psychological Science, 18, 227229. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01880.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, W. K., Rudich, E. A., & Sedikides, C. (2002). Narcissism, self-esteem, and the positivity of self-views: Two portraits of self-love. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 358368. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167202286007 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cascio, C. N., Konrath, S. H., & Falk, E. B. (2015). Narcissists’ social pain seen only in the brain. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10, 335341. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsu072 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chavez, R. S., & Heatherton, T. F. (2017). Structural integrity of frontostriatal connections predicts longitudinal changes in self-esteem. Social Neuroscience, 12, 280286. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2016.1164753 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheng, J. T., Tracy, J. L., & Miller, G. E. (2013). Are narcissists hardy or vulnerable? The role of narcissism in the production of stress-related biomarkers in response to emotional distress. Emotion, 13, 10041011. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034410 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chester, D. S., & DeWall, C. N. (2016). Sound the alarm: The effect of narcissism on retaliatory aggression is moderated by dACC reactivity to rejection. Journal of Personality, 84, 361368. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12164 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chester, D. S., Lynam, D. R., Powell, D. K., & DeWall, C. N. (2016). Narcissism is associated with weakened frontostriatal connectivity: A DTI study. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11, 10361040. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsv069 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coleman, S. R. M., Pincus, A. L., & Smyth, J. M. (2019). Narcissism and stress-reactivity: A biobehavioural health perspective. Health Psychology Review, 13, 3572. https://doi.org/10.1080/17437199.2018.1547118 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corradi-Dell’Acqua, C., Tusche, A., Vuilleumier, P., & Singer, T. (2016). Cross-modal representations of first-hand and vicarious pain, disgust and fairness in insular and cingulate cortex. Nature Communications, 7. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms10904 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dane, L. K., Jonason, P. K., & McCaffrey, M. (2018). Physiological tests of the cheater hypothesis for the dark triad traits: Testosterone, cortisol, and a social stressor. Personality and Individual Differences, 121, 227231. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.09.010 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davey, C. G., Pujol, J., & Harrison, B. J. (2016). Mapping the self in the brain’s default mode network. NeuroImage, 132, 390397. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.02.022 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 113126. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawood, S., Schroder, H. S., Donnellan, M. B., & Pincus, A. L. (2018). Pathological narcissism and nonsuicidal self-injury. Journal of Personality Disorders, 32, 87108. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi_2017_31_291 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de la Vega, A., Chang, L. J., Banich, M. T., Wager, T. D., & Yarkoni, T. (2016). Large-scale meta-analysis of human medial frontal cortex reveals tripartite functional organization. The Journal of Neuroscience, 36, 65536562. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4402-15.2016 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Vignemont, F., & Singer, T. (2006). The empathic brain: How, when and why? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 435441. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.08.008 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denecke, F. W., & Hilgenstock, B. (1989). Das Narzißmusinventar. Bern: Huber.Google Scholar
Di Sarno, M., Di Pierro, R., & Madeddu, F. (2018). The relevance of neuroscience for the investigation of narcissism: A review of current studies. Clinical Neuropsychiatry: Journal of Treatment Evaluation, 15, 242250.Google Scholar
Donnellan, M. B., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Robins, R. W. (2009). An emerging epidemic of narcissism or much ado about nothing? Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 498501. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2008.12.010 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dufner, M., Rauthmann, J. F., Czarna, A. Z., & Denissen, J. J. A. (2013). Are narcissists sexy? Zeroing in on the effect of narcissism on short-term mate appeal. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39, 870882. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167213483580 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edelstein, R. S., Yim, I. S., & Quas, J. A. (2010). Narcissism predicts heightened cortisol reactivity to a psychosocial stressor in men. Journal of Research in Personality, 44, 565572. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2010.06.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edershile, E. A., & Wright, A. G. C. (2020). Fluctuations in grandiose and vulnerable narcissistic states: A momentary perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000370 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Egan, V., Chan, S., & Shorter, G. W. (2014). The dark triad, happiness and subjective well-being. Personality and Individual Differences, 67, 1722. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.01.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenberger, N. I. (2003). Does rejection hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion. Science, 302, 290292. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1089134 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Euler, S., Stöbi, D., Sowislo, J., Ritzler, F., Huber, C. G., Lang, U. E., Wrege, J., & Walter, M. (2018). Grandiose and vulnerable narcissism in borderline personality disorder. Psychopathology, 51, 110121. https://doi.org/10.1159/000486601 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fan, Y., Wonneberger, C., Enzi, B., de Greck, M., Ulrich, C., Tempelmann, C., Bogerts, B., Doering, S., & Northoff, G. (2011). The narcissistic self and its psychological and neural correlates: An exploratory fMRI study. Psychological Medicine, 41, 16411650. https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329171000228X CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feng, C., Yuan, J., Geng, H., Gu, R., Zhou, H., Wu, X., & Luo, Y. (2018). Individualized prediction of trait narcissism from whole-brain resting-state functional connectivity. Human Brain Mapping, 39, 37013712. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24205 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fossati, A., Borroni, S., Grazioli, F., Dornetti, L., Marcassoli, I., Maffei, C., & Cheek, J. (2009). Tracking the hypersensitive dimension in narcissism: Reliability and validity of the Hypersensitive Narcissism Scale. Personality and Mental Health, 3, 235247. https://doi.org/10.1002/pmh.92 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fossati, A., Feeney, J., Pincus, A., Borroni, S., & Maffei, C. (2015). The structure of pathological narcissism and its relationships with adult attachment styles: A study of Italian nonclinical and clinical adult participants. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 32, 403431. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037175 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freud, S. (1914). On narcissism: An introduction. In Strachey, J. (Ed.), The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (p. 7, 66–102). London: Hogarth.Google Scholar
Frith, C., & Frith, U. (2005). Theory of mind. Current Biology, 15, R644R645. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2005.08.041 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gabriel, M. T., Critelli, J. W., & Ee, J. S. (1994). Narcissistic Illusions in self-evaluations of intelligence and attractiveness. Journal of Personality, 62, 143155. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1994.tb00798.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galang, A. J. R. (2010). The prosocial psychopath: Explaining the paradoxes of the creative personality. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 34, 12411248. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.03.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gebauer, J. E., Sedikides, C., Verplanken, B., & Maio, G. R. (2012). Communal narcissism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103, 854878. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029629 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
George, F. R., & Short, D. (2018). The cognitive neuroscience of narcissism. Journal of Brain, Behavior and Cognitive Sciences, 1, ID 6.Google Scholar
Geukes, K., Nestler, S., Hutteman, R., Dufner, M., Küfner, A. C. P., Egloff, B., Denissen, J. J. A., & Back, M. D. (2017). Puffed-up but shaky selves: State self-esteem level and variability in narcissists. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 112, 769786. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000093 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giammarco, E. A., & Vernon, P. A. (2014). Vengeance and the dark triad: The role of empathy and perspective taking in trait forgivingness. Personality and Individual Differences, 67, 2329. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.02.010 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glover, N., Miller, J. D., Lynam, D. R., Crego, C., & Widiger, T. A. (2012). The five-factor narcissism inventory: A five-factor measure of narcissistic personality traits. Journal of Personality Assessment, 94, 500512. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2012.670680 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gore, W. L., & Widiger, T. A. (2016). Fluctuation between grandiose and vulnerable narcissism. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 7, 363371. https://doi.org/10.1037/per0000181 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grapsas, S., Denissen, J. J. A., Lee, H. Y., Bos, P. A., & Brummelman, E. (2020). Climbing up or falling down: Narcissism predicts physiological sensitivity to social status in children and their parents. Developmental Science. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13062 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grijalva, E., & Zhang, L. (2016). Narcissism and self-insight: A review and meta-analysis of narcissists’ self-enhancement tendencies. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 42, 324. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167215611636 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grosz, M. P., Lösch, T., & Back, M. D. (2017). The narcissism-overclaiming link revisited. Journal of Research in Personality, 70, 134138. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2017.05.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gusnard, D. A., Akbudak, E., Shulman, G. L., & Raichle, M. E. (2001). Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: Relation to a default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98, 42594264. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.071043098 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hare, R. D., & Neumann, C. S. (2008). Psychopathy as a clinical and empirical construct. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 4, 217246. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091452 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hart, W., Adams, J., Burton, K. A., & Tortoriello, G. K. (2017). Narcissism and self-presentation: Profiling grandiose and vulnerable narcissists’ self-presentation tactic use. Personality and Individual Differences, 104, 4857. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.06.062 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hendin, H. M., & Cheek, J. M. (1997). Assessing hypersensitive narcissism: A reexamination of Murray’s narcism scale. Journal of Research in Personality, 31, 588599. https://doi.org/10.1006/jrpe.1997.2204 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hepper, E. G., Hart, C. M., & Sedikides, C. (2014). Moving Narcissus: Can narcissists be empathic? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40, 10791091. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167214535812 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hildebrandt, M. K., Jauk, E., Lehmann, K., Maliske, L., & Kanske, P. (2021). Brain activation during social cognition predicts everyday perspective-taking: A combined fMRI and ecological momentary assessment study of the social brain. NeuroImage, 227, 117624. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117624 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoffmann, F., Banzhaf, C., Kanske, P., Gärtner, M., Bermpohl, F., & Singer, T. (2016). Empathy in depression: Egocentric and altercentric biases and the role of alexithymia. Journal of Affective Disorders, 199, 2329. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2016.03.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holtzman, N. S., Vazire, S., & Mehl, M. R. (2010). Sounds like a narcissist: Behavioral manifestations of narcissism in everyday life. Journal of Research in Personality, 44(4), 478484. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2010.06.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Honeycutt, J. M., Pence, M. E., & Gearhart, C. G. (2014). Associations between narcissism, empathy, personality, and imagined interactions. In Besser, A. (Ed.), Psychology of emotions, motivations and actions. Handbook of the psychology of narcissism: Diverse perspectives. (pp. 333346). Hauppauge: Nova Science Publ.Google Scholar
Jacobs, S. C., Friedman, R., Parker, J. D., Tofler, G. H., Jimenez, A. H., Muller, J. E., … Stone, P. H. (1994). Use of skin conductance changes during mental stress testing as an index of autonomic arousal in cardiovascular research. American Heart Journal, 128, 11701177. https://doi.org/10.1016/0002-8703(94)90748-X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jankowiak-Siuda, K., & Zajkowski, W. (2013). A neural model of mechanisms of empathy deficits in narcissism. Medical Science Monitor, 19, 934941. https://doi.org/10.12659/MSM.889593 Google ScholarPubMed
Jauk, E., Benedek, M., Koschutnig, K., Kedia, G., & Neubauer, A. C. (2017). Self-viewing is associated with negative affect rather than reward in highly narcissistic men: An fMRI study. Scientific Reports, 7. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-03935-y CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jauk, E., Breyer, D., Kanske, P., & Wakabayashi, A. (2021). Narcissism in independent and interdependent cultures. Personality and Individual Differences, 177, 110716. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2021.110716 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jauk, E., & Dieterich, R. (2019). Addiction and the dark triad of personality. Frontiers in Psychiatry. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00662 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jauk, E., Freudenthaler, H. H., & Neubauer, A. C. (2016). The dark triad and trait versus ability emotional intelligence: emotional darkness differs between women and men. Journal of Individual Differences, 37, 112118. https://doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001/a000195 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jauk, E., & Kaufman, S. B. (2018). The higher the score, the darker the core: The nonlinear association between grandiose and vulnerable narcissism. Frontiers in Psychology, 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01305 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jauk, E., Neubauer, A. C., Mairunteregger, T., Pemp, S., Sieber, K. P., & Rauthmann, J. F. (2016). How alluring are dark personalities? The dark triad and attractiveness in speed dating. European Journal of Personality, 30, 125138. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2040 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jauk, E., Weigle, E., Lehmann, K., Benedek, M., & Neubauer, A. C. (2017). The relationship between grandiose and vulnerable (hypersensitive) narcissism. Frontiers in Psychology, 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01600 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
John, O. P., & Robins, R. W. (1994). Accuracy and bias in self-perception: Individual differences in self-enhancement and the role of narcissism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 206219. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.66.1.206 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jonason, P. K., & Webster, G. D. (2010). The dirty dozen: A concise measure of the dark triad. Psychological Assessment, 22, 420432. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019265 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kanske, P., Böckler, A., Trautwein, F.-M., Parianen Lesemann, F. H., & Singer, T. (2016). Are strong empathizers better mentalizers? Evidence for independence and interaction between the routes of social cognition. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11, 13831392. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw052 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kanske, P., Böckler, A., Trautwein, F.-M., & Singer, T. (2015). Dissecting the social brain: Introducing the EmpaToM to reveal distinct neural networks and brain–behavior relations for empathy and theory of mind. NeuroImage, 122, 619. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.082 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kanske, P., & Kotz, S. A. (2011). Emotion speeds up conflict resolution: A new role for the ventral anterior cingulate cortex? Cerebral Cortex, 21, 911919. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhq157 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kanske, P., & Kotz, S. A. (2011). Emotion triggers executive attention: Anterior cingulate cortex and amygdala responses to emotional words in a conflict task. Human Brain Mapping, 32, 198208. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21012 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kanske, P., Sharifi, M., Smallwood, J., Dziobek, I., & Singer, T. (2017). Where the narcissistic mind wanders: Increased self-related thoughts are more positive and future oriented. Journal of Personality Disorders, 31, 553566. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi_2016_30_263 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kanske, Philipp. (2018). The social mind: Disentangling affective and cognitive routes to understanding others. Interdisciplinary Science Reviews, 43, 115124. https://doi.org/10.1080/03080188.2018.1453243 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaufman, S. B., Weiss, B., Miller, J. D., & Campbell, W. K. (2020). Clinical correlates of vulnerable and grandiose narcissism: A personality perspective. Journal of Personality Disorders, 34, 107130. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi_2018_32_384 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelsey, R. M., Ornduff, S. R., McCann, C. M., & Reiff, S. (2001). Psychophysiological characteristics of narcissism during active and passive coping. Psychophysiology, 38, 292303. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-8986.3820292 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelsey, R. M., Ornduff, S. R., Reiff, S., & Arthur, C. M. (2002). Psychophysiological correlates of narcissistic traits in women during active coping. Psychophysiology, 39, 322332. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-8986.3930322 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kernberg, O. F. (1975). Borderline conditions and pathological narcissism. Lanham: Aronson.Google Scholar
Kernberg, O. F. (2007). The almost untreatable narcissistic patient. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 55, 503539. https://doi.org/10.1177/00030651070550020701 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirschbaum, C., Pirke, K.-M., & Hellhammer, D. H. (1993). The ‘trier social stress test’ – A tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology, 28, 7681. https://doi.org/10.1159/000119004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kohut, H. (1977). The restoration of the self. New York: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
Konrath, S., & Bonadonna, J. P. (2014). Physiological and health-related correlates of the narcissistic personality. In Besser, A. (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of narcissism: Diverse perspectives (pp. 175214). Hauppauge: Nova Science Publ.Google Scholar
Krizan, Z., & Herlache, A. D. (2018). The narcissism spectrum model: A synthetic view of narcissistic personality. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 22, 331. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868316685018 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krusemark, E. A. (2009). Neural correlates of self-enhancement in narcissism: An electroencephalography investigation. Unpublished doctoral dissertation.Google Scholar
Krusemark, E. A. (2012). Neurophysiological correlates of narcissism and psychopathy. In Campbell, W. K. & Miller, J. D. (Eds.), The handbook of narcissism and narcissistic personality disorder (pp. 221235). John Wiley & Sons, Inc. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118093108.ch20 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krusemark, E. A. (2018). Physiological reactivity and neural correlates of trait narcissism. In Hermann, A. D., Brunell, A. B., & Foster, J. D. (Eds.), Handbook of trait narcissism (pp. 213223). Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-92171-6_23 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuchynka, S. L., & Bosson, J. K. (2018). The psychodynamic mask model of narcissism: Where is it now? In Hermann, A. D., Brunell, A. B., & Foster, J. D. (Eds.), Handbook of trait narcissism (pp. 8995). Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-92171-6_9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Küfner, A. C. P., Dufner, M., & Back, M. D. (2015). Das dreckige Dutzend und die niederträchtigen Neun: Kurzskalen zur Erfassung von Narzissmus, Machiavellismus und Psychopathie. Diagnostica, 61, 7691. https://doi.org/10.1026/0012-1924/a000124 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lamm, C., Decety, J., & Singer, T. (2011). Meta-analytic evidence for common and distinct neural networks associated with directly experienced pain and empathy for pain. NeuroImage, 54, 24922502. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lannin, D. G., Guyll, M., Krizan, Z., Madon, S., & Cornish, M. (2014). When are grandiose and vulnerable narcissists least helpful? Personality and Individual Differences, 56, 127132. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.08.035 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LeDoux, J. E. (2000). Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual review of neuroscience, 23, 155184. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.155 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, R. J., Gozal, D., Coccaro, E. F., & Fanning, J. (2020). Narcissistic and borderline personality disorders: Relationship with oxidative stress. Journal of Personality Disorders, 34, 624. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2020.34.supp.6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lobbestael, J., Baumeister, R. F., Fiebig, T., & Eckel, L. A. (2014). The role of grandiose and vulnerable narcissism in self-reported and laboratory aggression and testosterone reactivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 69, 2227. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.05.007 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lobbestael, J., de Bruin, A., Kok, E., & Voncken, M. (2016). Through rose-coloured glasses: An empirical test of narcissistic overestimation: Empirical test of narcissistic overestimation. Personality and Mental Health, 10, 305316. https://doi.org/10.1002/pmh.1347 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luchner, A. F., Houston, J. M., Walker, C., & Alex Houston, M. (2011). Exploring the relationship between two forms of narcissism and competitiveness. Personality and Individual Differences, 51, 779782. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.06.033 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lykken, D. T. (1995). The antisocial personalities. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Mao, Y., Sang, N., Wang, Y., Hou, X., Huang, H., Wei, D., Zhang, J., & Qiu, J. (2016). Reduced frontal cortex thickness and cortical volume associated with pathological narcissism. Neuroscience, 328, 5057. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.04.025 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marcoux, L.-A., Michon, P.-E., Lemelin, S., Voisin, J. A., Vachon-Presseau, E., & Jackson, P. L. (2014). Feeling but not caring: Empathic alteration in narcissistic men with high psychopathic traits. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 224, 341348. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2014.10.002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marissen, M. A. E., Brouwer, M. E., Hiemstra, A. M. F., Deen, M. L., & Franken, I. H. A. (2016). A masked negative self-esteem? Implicit and explicit self-esteem in patients with narcissistic personality disorder. Psychiatry Research, 242, 2833. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2016.04.070 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marissen, M. A. E., Deen, M. L., & Franken, I. H. A. (2012). Disturbed emotion recognition in patients with narcissistic personality disorder. Psychiatry Research, 198, 269273. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011.12.042 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mars, R. B., Neubert, F.-X., Noonan, M. P., Sallet, J., Toni, I., & Rushworth, M. F. S. (2012). On the relationship between the “default mode network” and the “social brain.” Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00189 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mead, N. L., Baumeister, R. F., Stuppy, A., & Vohs, K. D. (2018). Power increases the socially toxic component of narcissism among individuals with high baseline testosterone. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147, 591596. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000427 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, J. D., Hoffman, B. J., Gaughan, E. T., Gentile, B., Maples, J., & Keith Campbell, W. (2011). Grandiose and vulnerable narcissism: A nomological network analysis. Journal of Personality, 79, 10131042. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2010.00711.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, J. D., Lynam, D. R., McCain, J. L., Few, L. R., Crego, C., Widiger, T. A., & Campbell, W. K. (2016). Thinking structurally about narcissism: An examination of the five-factor narcissism inventory and its components. Journal of Personality Disorders, 30, 118. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi_2015_29_177 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, J. D., Lynam, D. R., Vize, C., Crowe, M., Sleep, C., Maples-Keller, J. L., Few, L. R., & Campbell, W. K. (2018). Vulnerable narcissism is (mostly) a disorder of neuroticism. Journal of Personality, 86, 186199. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12303 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, J. D., McCain, J., Lynam, D. R., Few, L. R., Gentile, B., MacKillop, J., & Campbell, W. K. (2014). A comparison of the criterion validity of popular measures of narcissism and narcissistic personality disorder via the use of expert ratings. Psychological Assessment, 26, 958969. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036613 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, J. D., Price, J., Gentile, B., Lynam, D. R., & Campbell, W. K. (2012). Grandiose and vulnerable narcissism from the perspective of the interpersonal circumplex. Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 507512. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.04.026 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Molenberghs, P., Johnson, H., Henry, J. D., & Mattingley, J. B. (2016). Understanding the minds of others: A neuroimaging meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 65, 276291. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.03.020 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morf, C. C., & Rhodewalt, F. (2001). Unraveling the paradoxes of narcissism: A dynamic self-regulatory processing model. Psychological Inquiry, 12, 177196. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327965PLI1204_1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morf, C. C., Schürch, E., Küfner, A., Siegrist, P., Vater, A., Back, M., Mestel, R., & Schröder-Abé, M. (2017). Expanding the nomological net of the pathological narcissism inventory: German validation and extension in a clinical inpatient sample. Assessment, 24, 419443. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191115627010 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mota, S., Leckelt, M., Geukes, K., Nestler, S., Humberg, S., Schröder-Abé, M., Schmukle, S. C., & Back, M. D. (2019). A comprehensive examination of narcissists’ self-perceived and actual socioemotional cognition ability. Collabra: Psychology, 5, 6. https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.174 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mück, M., Ohmann, K., Dummel, S., Mattes, A., Thesing, U., & Stahl, J. (2020). Face perception and narcissism: Variations of event-related potential components (P1 & N170) with admiration and rivalry. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-020-00818-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mwilambwe-Tshilobo, L., & Spreng, R. N. (2021). Social exclusion reliably engages the default network: A meta-analysis of Cyberball. NeuroImage, 227, 117666. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117666 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nenadic, I., Güllmar, D., Dietzek, M., Langbein, K., Steinke, J., & Gaser, C. (2015). Brain structure in narcissistic personality disorder: A VBM and DTI pilot study. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 231, 184186. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2014.11.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neubauer, A. C., & Fink, A. (2009). Intelligence and neural efficiency. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 33, 10041023. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.04.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noser, E. (2017). Facial appearance and its association with bio-psychosocial parameters in healthy men. Unpublished doctoral dissertation.Google Scholar
Onoda, K., Okamoto, Y., Nakashima, K., Nittono, H., Yoshimura, S., Yamawaki, S., Yamaguchi, S., & Ura, M. (2010). Does low self-esteem enhance social pain? The relationship between trait self-esteem and anterior cingulate cortex activation induced by ostracism. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 5, 385391. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsq002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oosterwijk, S., Snoek, L., Rotteveel, M., Barrett, L. F., & Scholte, H. S. (2017). Shared states: Using MVPA to test neural overlap between self-focused emotion imagery and other-focused emotion understanding. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12, 10251035. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsx037 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pfattheicher, S. (2016). Testosterone, cortisol and the dark triad: narcissism (but not Machiavellianism or psychopathy) is positively related to basal testosterone and cortisol. Personality and Individual Differences, 97, 115119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.03.015 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pincus, A. L., Ansell, E. B., Pimentel, C. A., Cain, N. M., Wright, A. G. C., & Levy, K. N. (2009). Initial construction and validation of the pathological narcissism inventory. Psychological Assessment, 21, 365379. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016530 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pincus, A. L., Cain, N. M., & Wright, A. G. C. (2014). Narcissistic grandiosity and narcissistic vulnerability in psychotherapy. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 5, 439443. https://doi.org/10.1037/per0000031 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pincus, A. L., & Lukowitsky, M. R. (2010). Pathological narcissism and narcissistic personality disorder. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6, 421446. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131215 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raskin, R., & Hall, C. S. (1979). A narcissistic personality inventory. Psychological Reports, 45, 590590. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1979.45.2.590 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raskin, R., & Hall, C. S. (1981). The narcissistic personality inventory: Alternative form reliability and further evidence of construct validity. Journal of Personality Assessment, 45, 159162. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4502_10 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raskin, R., & Terry, H. (1988). A principal-components analysis of the narcissistic personality inventory and further evidence of its construct validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 890902. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.5.890 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reinhard, D. A., Konrath, S. H., Lopez, W. D., & Cameron, H. G. (2012). Expensive egos: Narcissistic males have higher cortisol. PLoS One, 7, e30858. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0030858 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rhodewalt, F., Madrian, J. C., & Cheney, S. (1998). Narcissism, self-knowledge organization, and emotional reactivity: The effect of daily experiences on self-esteem and affect. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 7587. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167298241006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ritter, K., Dziobek, I., Preißler, S., Rüter, A., Vater, A., Fydrich, T., … Roepke, S. (2011). Lack of empathy in patients with narcissistic personality disorder. Psychiatry Research, 187, 241247. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2010.09.013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roche, M. J., Pincus, A. L., Conroy, D. E., Hyde, A. L., & Ram, N. (2013). Pathological narcissism and interpersonal behavior in daily life. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 4, 315323. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030798 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roepke, S., & Vater, A. (2014). Narcissistic personality disorder: An integrative review of recent empirical data and current definitions. Current Psychiatry Reports, 16. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-014-0445-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rohmann, E., Hanke, S., & Bierhoff, H.-W. (2019). Grandiose and vulnerable narcissism in relation to life satisfaction, self-esteem, and self-construal. Journal of Individual Differences, 40, 194203. https://doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001/a000292 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rohmann, E., Neumann, E., Herner, M. J., & Bierhoff, H.-W. (2012). Grandiose and vulnerable narcissism: self-construal, attachment, and love in romantic relationships. European Psychologist, 17, 279290. https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/a000100 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ronningstam, E. (2009). Narcissistic personality disorder: Facing DSM-V. Psychiatric Annals, 39, 111121. https://doi.org/10.3928/00485713-20090301-09 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rose, P. (2002). The happy and unhappy faces of narcissism. Personality and Individual Differences, 33, 379391. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00162-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rotge, J.-Y., Lemogne, C., Hinfray, S., Huguet, P., Grynszpan, O., Tartour, E., … Fossati, P. (2015). A meta-analysis of the anterior cingulate contribution to social pain. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10, 1927. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsu110 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salim, S. (2014). Oxidative stress and psychological disorders. Current Neuropharmacology, 12, 140147. https://doi.org/10.2174/1570159X11666131120230309 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scalabrini, A., Huang, Z., Mucci, C., Perrucci, M. G., Ferretti, A., Fossati, A., … Ebisch, S. J. H. (2017). How spontaneous brain activity and narcissistic features shape social interaction. Scientific Reports, 7. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-10389-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schulze, L., Dziobek, I., Vater, A., Heekeren, H. R., Bajbouj, M., Renneberg, B., Heuser, I., & Roepke, S. (2013). Gray matter abnormalities in patients with narcissistic personality disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 47, 13631369. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.05.017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schulze, L., & Roepke, S. (2014). Structural and functional brain imaging in borderline, antisocial, and narcissistic personality disorder. In Mulert, C. & Shenton, M. E. (Eds.), MRI in psychiatry (pp. 313340). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-54542-9_17 Google Scholar
Schurz, M., Radua, J., Tholen, M. G., Maliske, L., Margulies, D. S., Mars, R. B., … Kanske, P. (2021). Toward a hierarchical model of social cognition: A neuroimaging meta-analysis and integrative review of empathy and theory of mind. Psychological Bulletin, 147(3), 293327. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000303 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sedikides, C., Rudich, E. A., Gregg, A. P., Kumashiro, M., & Rusbult, C. (2004). Are normal narcissists psychologically healthy?: Self-esteem matters. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 400416. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.87.3.400 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seeley, W. W. (2019). The salience network: A neural system for perceiving and responding to homeostatic demands. The Journal of Neuroscience, 39, 98789882. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1138-17.2019 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shackman, A. J., Salomons, T. V., Slagter, H. A., Fox, A. S., Winter, J. J., & Davidson, R. J. (2011). The integration of negative affect, pain and cognitive control in the cingulate cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12, 154167. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2994 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Somerville, L. H., Heatherton, T. F., & Kelley, W. M. (2006). Anterior cingulate cortex responds differentially to expectancy violation and social rejection. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 10071008. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1728 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sommer, K. L., Kirkland, K. L., Newman, S. R., Estrella, P., & Andreassi, J. L. (2009). Narcissism and cardiovascular reactivity to rejection imagery. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 39, 10831115. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2009.00473.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spencer, C. C., Foster, J. D., & Bedwell, J. S. (2018). Structural relationships among the revised reinforcement sensitivity theory and grandiose and vulnerable narcissism. Journal of Personality Disorders, 32, 654667. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi_2017_31_318 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stenstrom, E. P., Dinsmore, J. B., Kunstman, J. W., & Vohs, K. D. (2018). The effects of money exposure on testosterone and risk-taking, and the moderating role of narcissism. Personality and Individual Differences, 123, 110114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.10.035 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stietz, J., Jauk, E., Krach, S., & Kanske, P. (2019). Dissociating empathy from perspective-taking: Evidence from intra- and inter-individual differences research. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00126 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sylvers, P., Brubaker, N., Alden, S. A., Brennan, P. A., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2008). Differential endophenotypic markers of narcissistic and antisocial personality features: A psychophysiological investigation. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 12601270. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2008.03.010 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tanzilli, A., Colli, A., Muzi, L., & Lingiardi, V. (2015). Clinician emotional response toward narcissistic patients: A preliminary report. Research in Psychotherapy: Psychopathology, Process and Outcome, 18. https://doi.org/10.4081/ripppo.2015.174 Google Scholar
Thomaes, S., Stegge, H., Bushman, B. J., Olthof, T., & Denissen, J. (2008). Development and validation of the childhood narcissism scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 90, 382391. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223890802108162 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Twenge, J. M. (2006). Generation me: Why today’s young Americans are more confident, assertive, entitled—And more miserable than ever before. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Twenge, J. M., & Campbell, W. K. (2009). The narcissism epidemic: Living in the age of entitlement. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Twenge, J. M., Konrath, S., Foster, J. D., Keith Campbell, W., & Bushman, B. J. (2008). Egos inflating over time: A cross-temporal meta-analysis of the narcissistic personality inventory. Journal of Personality, 76, 875902. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00507.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tyrer, P., Mulder, R., Kim, Y.-R., & Crawford, M. J. (2019). The development of the ICD-11 classification of personality disorders: An amalgam of science, pragmatism, and politics. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 15, 481502. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050718-095736 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ulrich-Lai, Y. M., & Herman, J. P. (2009). Neural regulation of endocrine and autonomic stress responses. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 397409. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2647 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valdespino, A., Antezana, L., Ghane, M., & Richey, J. A. (2017). Alexithymia as a transdiagnostic precursor to empathy abnormalities: The functional role of the insula. Frontiers in Psychology, 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02234 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valk, S. L., Hoffstaedter, F., Camilleri, J. A., Kochunov, P., Thomas Yeo, B. T., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2019). Does macroscale grey matter anatomy reflect personality? BioRxiv. https://doi.org/10.1101/645945 Google Scholar
Vater, A., Moritz, S., & Roepke, S. (2018). Does a narcissism epidemic exist in modern western societies? Comparing narcissism and self-esteem in East and West Germany. PLoS One, 13, e0188287. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0188287 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vater, A., Ritter, K., Schröder-Abé, M., Schütz, A., Lammers, C.-H., Bosson, J. K., & Roepke, S. (2013). When grandiosity and vulnerability collide: Implicit and explicit self-esteem in patients with narcissistic personality disorder. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 44, 3747. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2012.07.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vonk, J., Zeigler-Hill, V., Ewing, D., Mercer, S., & Noser, A. E. (2015). Mindreading in the dark: Dark personality features and theory of mind. Personality and Individual Differences, 87, 5054. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.07.025 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, D. D., Haxby, J. V., & Heatherton, T. F. (2012). The representation of self and person knowledge in the medial prefrontal cortex: The representation of self and person knowledge. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 3, 451470. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1183 Google ScholarPubMed
Wai, M., & Tiliopoulos, N. (2012). The affective and cognitive empathic nature of the dark triad of personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 52, 794799. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.01.008 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, W., Wang, Y., Fu, X., Liu, J., He, C., Dong, Y., … Jang, K. L. (2006). Cerebral information processing in personality disorders: I. Intensity dependence of auditory evoked potentials. Psychiatry Research, 141, 173183. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2004.05.028 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wardecker, B. M., Chopik, W. J., LaBelle, O. P., & Edelstein, R. S. (2018). Is narcissism associated with baseline cortisol in men and women? Journal of Research in Personality, 72, 4449. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2016.07.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiss, B., Campbell, W. K., Lynam, D. R., & Miller, J. D. (2019). A trifurcated model of narcissism: On the pivotal role of trait antagonism. In The handbook of antagonism (pp. 221235). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-814627-9.00015-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wetzel, E., Brown, A., Hill, P. L., Chung, J. M., Robins, R. W., & Roberts, B. W. (2017). The narcissism epidemic Is dead; Long live the narcissism Epidemic. Psychological Science, 28, 18331847. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617724208 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wink, P. (1991). Two faces of narcissism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 590597. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.61.4.590 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winter, K., Spengler, S., Bermpohl, F., Singer, T., & Kanske, P. (2017). Social cognition in aggressive offenders: Impaired empathy, but intact theory of mind. Scientific Reports, 7. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-00745-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodman, T., Roberts, R., Hardy, L., Callow, N., & Rogers, C. H. (2011). There is an “I” in TEAM: Narcissism and social loafing. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 82, 285290. https://doi.org/10.1080/02701367.2011.10599756 Google Scholar
Wrege, J. S., Ruocco, A. C., Euler, S., Preller, K. H., Busmann, M., Meya, L., … Walter, M. (2019). Negative affect moderates the effect of social rejection on frontal and anterior cingulate cortex activation in borderline personality disorder. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 19, 12731285. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-019-00716-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wright, A. G., & Edershile, E. A. (2018). Issues resolved and unresolved in pathological narcissism. Current Opinion in Psychology, 21, 7479. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.10.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wright, A. G. C., Lukowitsky, M. R., Pincus, A. L., & Conroy, D. E. (2010). The higher order factor structure and gender invariance of the pathological narcissism inventory. Assessment, 17, 467483. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191110373227 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wurst, S. N., Gerlach, T. M., Dufner, M., Rauthmann, J. F., Grosz, M. P., Küfner, A. C. P., … Back, M. D. (2017). Narcissism and romantic relationships: The differential impact of narcissistic admiration and rivalry. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 112, 280306. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000113 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, W., Cun, L., Du, X., Yang, J., Wang, Y., Wei, D., … Qiu, J. (2015). Gender differences in brain structure and resting-state functional connectivity related to narcissistic personality. Scientific Reports, 5. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep10924 Google ScholarPubMed
Yang, Z., Sedikides, C., Gu, R., Luo, Y. L. L., Wang, Y., & Cai, H. (2018). Narcissism and risky decisions: A neurophysiological approach. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 13, 889897. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsy053 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, Z., Sedikides, C., Gu, R., Luo, Y. L. L., Wang, Y., Yang, Y., … Cai, H. (2018). Communal narcissism: Social decisions and neurophysiological reactions. Journal of Research in Personality, 76, 6473. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2018.07.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zajenkowski, M., Maciantowicz, O., Szymaniak, K., & Urban, P. (2018). Vulnerable and grandiose narcissism are differentially associated with ability and trait emotional intelligence. Frontiers in Psychology, 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01606 Google ScholarPubMed
Zeigler-Hill, V. (2006). Discrepancies between implicit and explicit self-esteem: Implications for narcissism and self-esteem instability. Journal of Personality, 74, 119144. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00371.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, B., Shen, C., Zhu, Q., Ma, G., & Wang, W. (2016). Processing of facial expressions of emotions in antisocial, narcissistic, and schizotypal personality disorders: An event-related potential study. Personality and Individual Differences, 99, 16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.04.066 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, H., Wang, Z., You, X., , W., & Luo, Y. (2015). Associations between narcissism and emotion regulation difficulties: Respiratory sinus arrhythmia reactivity as a moderator. Biological Psychology, 110, 111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.06.014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zuckerman, M., & O’Loughlin, R. E. (2009). Narcissism and well-being: A longitudinal perspective. European Journal of Social Psychology, 39, 957972. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.594 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Structural model of narcissism, adapted and synthesized from the Trifurcated Model (Miller et al., 2016; Weiss et al., 2019) and the Narcissism Spectrum Model (Krizan & Herlache, 2018).

Figure 1

Table 1. Dimensions of intra- and interpersonal functioning

Figure 2

Table 2. Summary of neuroscience studies on narcissism

Figure 3

Figure 2. Schematic depiction of central findings of neuroscience studies on narcissism. Dashed lines indicating the moderating influence of narcissism (see section 3) on particular effects. Activation within central nodes of the salience network (orange) and reactions in autonomic (sympathetic) or neuroendocrine markers are higher in situations involving intrapersonal focus when self-relevance is high (potential ego threat). Reactions to aversive self-related stimuli of low self-relevance can be “cool”. Situations involving interpersonal focus (empathy, social touch) lead to down-regulations. At a brain structural and baseline function level, narcissism is related to alterations in regions of the default mode network (cyan) and in the frontostriatal pathway (purple) as well as neuroendocrine markers.