Hostname: page-component-84b7d79bbc-rnpqb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-26T19:29:39.888Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Infinitival relative clauses in spoken discourse

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Christer Geisler
Affiliation:
University of Uppsala

Abstract

This article concerns infinitival relative clauses, such as Mary is the person to ask, and their distribution in spoken English. It analyzes the correlation between the function of the antecedent in the relative clause and the function of the whole postmodified NP (the relative complex) in the matrix clause. On the basis of a quantitative analysis of a corpus of spoken British English, I show that the grammatical function of the antecedent in the infinitival relative clause depends on the function of the antecedent in the matrix clause. I argue that the distribution of antecedent functions in the matrix clause can be explained in terms of thematic properties and information structure of the clauses in which the infinitival relatives occur. A key notion is that speakers center their discourse around information that they assume to be important for the communicative event.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1998

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aniya, Sosei. (1992). The semantics and the syntax of the existential there-construction. Linguistic Analysis 22:154184.Google Scholar
Ashby, William J., & Bentivoglio, Paola. (1993). Preferred argument structure in spoken French and Spanish. Language Variation and Change 5:6176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernardo, Robert. (1979). The function and content of relative clauses in spontaneous oral narratives. Proceedings of the 5th annual meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society. 539551.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chafe, Wallace. (1976). Givenness, contrastiveness, definiteness, subjects and topics. In Li, Charles N. (Ed.), Subject and topic. New York: Academic. 2755.Google Scholar
Crystal, David. (1969). Prosodic systems and intonation in English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Crystal, David, & Quirk, Randolph. (1964). Systems of prosodic and paralinguistic features in English. The Hague: Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Villiers, Jill G., Flusberg, Helen B., Tager, Hakuta, Kenji, , & Cohen, Michael. (1979). Children's comprehension of relative clauses. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 8:499518.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Du Bois, John W. (1987). The discourse basis of ergativity. Language 63:805855.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, Barbara A. (1987). The noun phrase accessibility hierarchy: Subject primacy or the absolutive hypothesis? Language 63:856870.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, Barbara A., & Thompson, Sandra A. (1990a). A discourse explanation of the grammar of relative clauses in English conversation. Language 66:297316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, Barbara A., & Thompson, Sandra A. (1990b). On formulating reference: An interactional approach to relative clauses in English conversation. IPRA Papers in Pragmatics 4:183196.Google Scholar
Geisler, Christer. (1995). Relative infinitives in English. Doctoral dissertation, University of Uppsala. Studia Anglistica Upsaliensia 91.Google Scholar
Iversen, Gudmund. (1979). Decomposing chi-square–A forgotten technique. Sociological methods and research 8:143157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keenan, Edward L., & Comrie, Bernard. (1977). Noun phrase accessibility and Universal Grammar. Linguistic Inquiry 8:6399.Google Scholar
Ladusaw, William A., & Dowty, David R. (1988). Toward a nongrammatical account of thematic roles. In Wilkins, Wendy (Ed.), Syntax and semantics. Vol. 21: Thematic relations. New York: Academic. 6173.Google Scholar
Nishigauchi, Taisuke. (1984). Control and the thematic domain. Language 60:215250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prince, Ellen. (1979). On the given/new distinction. Papers from the fifteenth regional meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society. 267a–278.Google Scholar
Prince, Ellen. (1981). Toward a typology of given-new information. In Cole, P. (Ed.), Radical pragmatics. New York: Academic. 233255.Google Scholar
Quirk, Randolph, Greenbaum, Sidney, Leech, Geoffrey, & Svartvik, Jan. (1985). A comprehensive grammar of the English language. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Reynolds, H. T. (1984). Analysis of nominal data (2nd ed.). Newbury Park: Sage Publications.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romaine, Suzanne. (1984). Relative clauses in child language, pidgins and Creoles. Australian Journal of Linguistics 4:257281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sasaki, Miyuki. (1991). An analysis of sentences with nonreferential there in spoken American English. Word 42:157178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Svartvik, Jan, & Quirk, Randolph (Eds.). (1980). A corpus of English conversation. Lund: Lund University Press.Google Scholar
Svartvik, Jan, Eeg-Olofsson, Mats, Forsheden, Oskar, Oreström, Bengt, & Thavenius, Cecilia. (1982). Survey of spoken English. Report on research 1975–81. Lund Studies in English 63. Lund: Gleerup.Google Scholar
Tomlin, Russell S. (1985). Interaction of subject, theme, and agent. In Wirth, J. R. (Ed.), Beyond the sentence: discourse and pronominal form. Ann Arbor: Karoma Publishers. 6080.Google Scholar
Upton, Graham J. G. (1978). The analysis of cross-tabulated data. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar