Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-vpsfw Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-17T01:06:44.306Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Similarity neighbourhoods of words in young children's lexicons*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 February 2009

Jan Charles-Luce*
Affiliation:
State University of New York at Buffalo
Paul A. Luce*
Affiliation:
State University of New York at Buffalo
*
Department of Communicative Disorders and Sciences
Department of Psychology, State University of New York at Buffalo, Buffalo, New York 14260, USA.

Abstract

Similarity neighbourhoods for words in young children's lexicons were investigated using three computerized databases. These databases were representative of three groups of native English speakers: 5-year-olds, 7-year-olds, and adults. Computations relating to the similarity neighbourhoods of words in the children's and adult's lexicon revealed that words in the 5- and 7-year-olds' lexicons have many fewer similar neighbours than the same words analyzed in the adult lexicon. Thus, young children may employ more global recognition strategies because words are more discriminable in memory. The neighbourhood analyses provide a number of insights into the processes of auditory word recognition in children and the possible structural organization of words in the young child's mental lexicon.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1990

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

This research was supported, in part, by NIH Research grant NS-12179 and, in part, by NIH Training Grant NS-07134 to Indiana University. We would like to thank Beth Greene and Debbie Burns for their assistance with the children's lexicons and David B. Pisoni and Linda B. Smith for their helpful comments and suggestions.

References

REFERENCES

Aslin, R. N., Pisoni, D. B. & Jusczyk, P. W. (1983). Auditory development and speech perception in infancy. In Haith, M. M. & Campos, J. J. (eds), Infancy and the biology of development. (Vol. 2 of L. Carmichael (ed.), Manual of Child Psychology. Fourth edition.) New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Burke, D. (1976). The effects of noise on the identification of words in children. Ph.D. thesis, Columbia University.Google Scholar
Calfee, R. C., Chapman, R. S. & Venezky, R. (1972). How a child needs to think to learn to read. In Gregg, L. W. (ed.), Cognition in learning and memory. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Cole, R. A. (1981). Perception of fluent speech by children and adults. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 379. 92109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cole, R. A. & Perfetti, C. A. (1980). Listening for mispronunciations in a children's story: the use of context by children and adults. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 19. 297315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grosjean, F. (1980). Spoken word recognition and the gating paradigm. Perception and Psychophysics 28. 267–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huttenlocher, J. (1984). Word recognition and word production in children. In Bouma, H. & Bouwhuis, D. (eds), Attention and performance X: control of language processes. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Jusczyk, P. W. (1983). On characterizing the development of speech perception. In Mehler, J. & Fox, R. (eds), Neonate cognition: beyond the blooming, buzzing confusion. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Landauer, T. K. & Streeter, L. A. (1973). Structural differences between common and rare words: failure of equivalence assumptions for theories of word recognition. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 12. 119–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liberman, I. Y., Shankweiler, D., Fischer, F. W. & Carter, B. (1974). Explicit syllable and phoneme segmentation in the young child. Journal of Experimental Child Phonology 18. 201–12.Google Scholar
Luce, P. A. (1986). Neighborhoods of words in the mental lexicon. Unpublished Ph.D. thesis, Indiana University.Google Scholar
Salasoo, A. & Pisoni, D. B. (1985). Interaction of knowledge in spoken word identification. Journal of Memory Language 24. 210–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treiman, R. & Baron, J. (1981). Segmental analysis ability: development and relation to reading ability. In Waller, T. G. & MacKinnon, G. E. (eds), Reading research: advances in theory and practice. Vol. 3. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Treiman, R. & Breaux, A. M. (1982). Common phoneme and overall similarity relations among spoken syllables: their use by children and adults. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 11. 581610.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walley, A. C. (1984). Developmental differences in spoken word identification. Unpublished Ph.D. thesis, Indiana University.Google Scholar
Walley, A. C. (1987). Young children's detections of word-initial and -final mispronunciations in constrained and unconstrained contexts. Cognitive Development 2. 145–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walley, A. C. (1988). Spoken word recognition by young children and adults. Cognitive Development 3 137–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walley, A. C., Smith, L. B. & Jusczyk, P. W. (1986). The role of phonemes and syllables in the perceived similarity of speech sounds for children. Memory and Cognition 14. 220–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wepman, J. M. & Hass, W. (1969). A spoken word count: children – ages 5, 6, and 7. Chicago: Language Research Associates.Google Scholar