Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home

Prosody signals the emergence of intentional communication in the first year of life: evidence from Catalan-babbling infants*

  • NÚRIA ESTEVE-GIBERT (a1) and PILAR PRIETO (a2)

Abstract

There is considerable debate about whether early vocalizations mimic the target language and whether prosody signals emergent intentional communication. A longitudinal corpus of four Catalan-babbling infants was analyzed to investigate whether children use different prosodic patterns to distinguish communicative from investigative vocalizations and to express intentionality. A total of 2,701 vocalizations from 0;7 to 0;11 were coded acoustically (by marking pitch range and duration), gesturally, and pragmatically (by marking communicative status and specific pragmatic function). The results showed that communicative vocalizations were shorter and had a wider pitch range than investigative vocalizations and that these patterns in communicative vocalizations depended on the intention of the vocalizations: requests and expressions of discontent displayed wider pitch range and longer duration than responses or statements. These results support the hypothesis that babbling children can successfully use a set of prosodic patterns to signal intentional speech.

Copyright

Corresponding author

Address for correspondence: Universitat Pompeu Fabra – Departament de Traducció i Ciències del Llenguatge, Edifici Roc Boronat 138, Barcelona – 08018, Spain. tel: 935422409; e-mail: nuria.esteve@upf.edu

Footnotes

Hide All
[*]

An earlier version of this article was presented at the VI Conference on Language Acquisition (Barcelona, 8–10 September 2010). We would like to thank participants at that meeting, and especially S. López-Ornat, J. Trueswell, and L. Bosch. We are grateful to the editor, the action editor and the two reviewers for their comments, which have been very helpful to us in revising the text. We also thank M. Armstrong, J. Borràs-Comes, and S. Berends for the reliability coding, M. M. Vanrell for her help with statistics, and Paolo Roseano for his help with Praat figures, all of them members of the Grup d'Estudis de Prosòdia. Finally, we thank the children and the children's parents for voluntarily taking part in this study. This research has been funded by three research grants awarded by the Spanish Ministerio de Educación y Ciencia, FFI2009-07648/FILO ‘The role of tonal scaling and tonal alignment in distinguishing intonational categories in Catalan and Spanish’, by the Consolider-Ingenio 2010 (CSD2007-00012) Program, and by a grant awarded by the Generalitat de Catalunya to the Grup d'Estudis de Prosòdia (2009SGR-701).

Footnotes

References

Hide All
Allwood, J., Cerrato, L., Jokinen, K., Navarretta, C. & Paggio, P. (2007). The MUMIN coding scheme for the annotation of feedback, turn management and sequencing. In Martin, J. C., Paggio, P., Kuehnlein, P., Stiefelhagen, R. & Pianesi, F. (eds), Multimodal corpora for modeling human multimodal behaviour. Special issue of Language Resources and Evaluation 41(3/4), 273–87. Heidelberg: Springer.
Astruc, L., Prieto, P., Payne, E., Post, B. & Vanrell, M. M. (in press). Tonal targets in early child English, Spanish, and Catalan. Language and Speech.
Balog, H. L. & Brentari, D. (2008). The relationship between early gesture and intonation. First Language 28, 141–63.
Balog, H. L., Roberts, F. & Snow, D. (2009). Discourse and intonation development in the first-word period. Enfance 3, 293304.
Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L. & Volterra, V. (1979). The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic Press.
Bates, E., Camaioni, L. & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 21, 205226.
Blake, J. & Boysson-Bardies, B. de (1992). Patterns in babbling: A cross-linguistic study. Journal of Child Language 19, 5174.
Boersma, P. & Weenink, D. (2005). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (Version 4.3.01). University of Amsterdam 2005 [http://www.praat.org/].
Chen, L. & Kent, R. (2009). Development of prosodic patterns in Mandarin-learning infants. Journal of Child Language 36, 7395.
D'Odorico, L. & Franco, F. (1991). Selective production of vocalization types in different communication contexts. Journal of Child Language 18, 475–99.
Davis, B. L., MacNeilage, P. F., Matyear, C. L. & Powell, J. K. (2000). Prosodic correlates of stress in babbling: An acoustical study. Child Development 71, 1258–70.
Engstrand, O., Williams, K. & Lacerda, F. (2003). Does babbling sound native? Listener responses to vocalizations produced by Swedish and American 12- and 18-month-olds. Phonetica 60, 1744.
Flax, J., Lahey, M., Harris, K. & Boothroyd, A. (1991). Relations between prosodic variables and communication function. Journal of Child Language 18, 319.
Frota, S. & Vigário, M. (2008). The intonation of one-word and first two-word utterances in European Portuguese. Paper presented at the XI International Conference for the Study of Child Language (IASCL).
Furrow, D. (1984). Young children's use of prosody. Journal of Child Language 11(1), 203213.
Furrow, D., Podrouzek, W. & Moore, C. (1990). The acoustical analysis of children's use of prosody in assertive and directive contexts. First Language 10, 3749.
Galligan, R. (1987). Intonation with single words: Purposive and grammatical use. Journal of Child Language 14, 121.
Halliday, M. A. K. (1975). Learning how to mean: Explorations in the development of language. New York: Elsevier.
Iverson, J. M. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological Science 16(5), 367–71.
Karousou, A. (2003). Análisis de las vocalizaciones tempranas: su patrón evolutivo y su función determinante en la emergencia de la palabra. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Universidad Complutense de Madrid.
Kent, R. D. & Murray, A. D. (1982). Acoustic features of infant vocalic utterances at 3, 6, and 9 months. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 72, 353–65.
Levitt, A. & Utman, J. (1992). From babbling towards the sound systems of English and French: A longitudinal two-case study. Journal of Child Language 19(1), 1949.
Lieberman, P. (1967). Intonation, perception, and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Mampe, B., Friederici, A. D., Christophe, A. & Wermke, K. (2009). Newborns' cry melody is shaped by their native language. Current Biology 19(23), 1994–97.
Marcos, H. (1987). Communicative functions of pitch range and pitch direction in infants. Journal of Child Language 14, 255–68.
Özçalişkan, S. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture is at the cutting edge of early language development. Cognition 96, 101113.
Papaeliou, C., Minadakis, G. & Cavouras, D. (2002). Acoustic patterns of infant vocalizations expressing emotions and communicative functions. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 45(2), 311–17.
Papaeliou, C. F. & Trevarthen, C. (2006). Prelinguistic pitch patterns expressing ‘communication’ and ‘apprehension’. Journal of Child Language 33, 163–78.
Piaget, J. (1936). La naissance de l'intelligence chez l'enfant. Neuchâtel: Delachaux et Niestlé.
Prieto, P., Estrella, A., Thorson, J. & Vanrell, M. M. (2012). Is prosodic development correlated with grammatical and lexical development? Evidence from emerging intonation in Catalan and Spanish. Journal of Child Language 39(2), 258–83.
Rose, Y., MacWhinney, B., Byrne, R., Hedlund, G., Maddocks, K., O'Brien, P. & Warehem, T. (2006). Introducing Phon: A software solution for the study of phonological acquisition. In Bamman, D., Magnitskaia, T. & Zaller, Colleen (eds), Proceedings of the 30th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, 489500. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
Sachs, J. (1993). The emergence of intentional communication. In Gleason, J. (ed.), The development of language, 4064. New York: Macmillan.
Sansavini, B., Guarini, S. & Stefanini, C. (2010). Early development of gestures, object-related actions, word comprehension and word production, and their relationships in Italian infants. Gesture 10(1), 5285.
Sarriá, E. (1991). Observación de la comunicación intencional preverbal: un sistema de codificación basado en el concepto de la categoría natural. Psicotema 3, 359–80.
Scherer, K. R. (1986). Vocal affect expression: A review and a model for future research. Psychological Bulletin 99, 143–65.
Snow, D. (2006). Regression and reorganization of intonation between 6 and 23 months. Child Development 77, 281–96.
Snow, D. & Balog, H. L. (2002). Do children produce the melody before the words? A review of developmental intonation research. Lingua 112, 1025–58.
Tomasello, M. (1993). On the interpersonal origins of self-concept. In Neisser, U. (ed.), The perceived self. Ecological and interpersonal knowledge of the self-knowledge, 174–84. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M. & Liszkowski, U. (2007). A new look at infant pointing. Child Development 78, 705722.
Trevarthen, C. (1977). Descriptive analyses of infant communicative behaviour. In Schaffer, H. R. (ed.), Studies in mother–infant interaction, 227–70. London: Academic Press.
Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in early infancy: A description of primary intersubjectivity. In Bullowa, M. (ed.), Before speech, 321–47. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Trevarthen, C. (1982). The primary motives for cooperative understanding. In Butterworth, G. & Light, P. (eds), Social cognition: Studies of the development of understanding, 77109. Brighton: Harvester Press.
Trevarthen, C. (1990). Signs before speech. In Sebeok, T. A. & Sebeok, J. U. (eds), The semiotic web, 689755. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Vihman, M. M. & DePaolis, R. A. (1998). Perception and production in early vocal development: Evidence from the acquisition of accent. In Gruber, M. C., Higgins, D., Olson, K. S. & Wysocki, T. (eds), Chicago Linguistic Society 34, 373–86.
Vihman, M. M., DePaolis, R. A. & Davis, B. L. (1998). Is there a ‘trochaic bias’ in early word learning? Evidence from infant production in English and French. Child Development 69, 933947.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
West, B., Welch, K. B. & Galecki, A. T. (2007). Linear mixed models: A practical guide using statistical software. New York: Chapman & Hall/CRC.
Whalen, D. H., Levitt, A. G. & Wang, Q. (1991). Intonational differences between the reduplicative babbling of French- and English-learning infants. Journal of Child Language 18, 501516.

Related content

Powered by UNSILO

Prosody signals the emergence of intentional communication in the first year of life: evidence from Catalan-babbling infants*

  • NÚRIA ESTEVE-GIBERT (a1) and PILAR PRIETO (a2)

Metrics

Altmetric attention score

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between <date>. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed.