Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-wq2xx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T16:17:55.751Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Patterns in babbling: a cross-linguistic study*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 February 2009

Joanna Blake*
Affiliation:
York University
Bénédicte De Boysson-Bardies
Affiliation:
C.N.R.S.
*
Department of Psychology, York University, 4700 Keele St., North York, Ontario M3J 1P3, Canada.

Abstract

Three Canadian–English infants and three Parisian–French infants were filmed bi-weekly for three to five months, from the age of 0;9 or 0;11 until 1;2, at home in naturalistic interaction with a parent. Their babbled utterances were transcribed phonetically and categorized according to consonant-type and vowel-type. The contexts for each utterance were described in terms of both specific and more general contextual categories. Observed frequencies of co-occurrences between phonetic and contextual categories were compared to expected frequencies, and deviations were considered to be patterns in babbling. Patterning increased after age 1;0 for most infants, and from 15% to 30% of each infant's babbled utterances recurred in particular contexts with a greater-than-expected frequency. Similarities in patterns were found both within and across language groups. These sound-meaning correspondences in babbling are viewed as continuous with early situation-bound meaning in words.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1992

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

Some of these data were presented at the Biennial Conference on Child Development, University of Waterloo, May 1988, and at the Tenth Biennial Meeting of the International Society for the Study of Behavioural Development, Jyväskylä, Finland, July 1989. The research was supported by grants from the Fondation Fyssen, Paris, and from the Faculty of Arts, York University, to the first author; and the equipment in Paris for filming and coding was provided by C.N.R.S. The authors would like to thank Joseline Pérusse, Catherine Durand, Marsha Cannon, Nurit Nadler and Rick Blacow for their phonetic transcriptions of the corpora and Amanda Lisus and Paula O'Rourke for assistance with the data analysis. We are particularly grateful to the participating French parents and infants for their warm and gracious co-operation with our lengthy filming schedule and to the Canadian parents for their reliable expertise in conducting the filming themselves.

References

REFERENCES

Barrett, M. D. (1986). Early semantic representations and early word-usage. In Kuczaj, S. A. II & Barrett, M. D. (eds), The development of word meaning. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Bates, E. (1976). Language and context: the acquisition of pragmatics. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E. (1979). The emergence of symbols. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Blake, J. & Fink, R. (1987). Sound-meaning correspondences in babbling. Journal of Child Language 14, 229–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boysson-Bardies, B. de, Halle, P., Sagart, L. & Durand, C. (1989). A crosslinguistic investigation of vowel formants in babbling. Journal of Child Language 16, 117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carter, A. L. (1974). The development of communication in the sensorimotor period: a case study. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California at Berkeley.Google Scholar
Carter, A. L. (1975). The transformation of sensorimotor morphemes into words: a case study of the development of ‘more’ and ‘mine’. Journal of Child Language 2, 233–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delack, J. B. & Fowlow, P. J. (1978). The ontogenesis of differential vocalization: development of prosodie contrastivity during the first year of life. In Waterson, N. & Snow, C. (eds), The development of communication. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Dore, J., Franklin, M. B., Miller, R. T. & Ramer, A. L. H. (1976). Transitional phenomena in early language acquisition. Journal of Child Language 3, 1328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferguson, C. A. (1976). Learning to pronounce: the earliest stages of phonological development in the child. Paper presented at the conference on the Early Behavioural Assessment of the Communicative and Cognitive Abilities of the Developmentally Disabled, Oreas Island.Google Scholar
Gillis, S. & DeSchutter, G. (1986). Transitional phenomena revisited: insights into the nominal insight. In Lindblom, B. & Zetterstrom, R. eds, Precursors of early speech. New York: Stockton.Google Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K. (1973). A sociosemiotic perspective on language development. Paper presented at the Fifth Child Language Research Forum, Stanford University.Google Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K. (1975 a). Learning how to mean – explorations in the development of language. London: Edward Arnold.Google Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K. (1975 b). Learning how to mean. In Lenneberg, E. & Lenneberg, E., (eds), Foundations of language development. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Harris, M., Barrett, M., Jones, D. & Brookes, S. (1988). Linguistic input and early word meaning. Journal of Child Langauge 15, 7794.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hilke, D. D. (1988). Infant vocalizations and changes in experience. Journal of Child Language 15, 115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holmgren, K., Lindblom, B., Aurelius, B. J. & Zetterstrom, R. (1986). On the phonetics of infant vocalization. In Lindblom, B. & Zetterstrom, R. (eds), Precursors of early speech. New York: Stockton.Google Scholar
Jakobson, R. (1941/1968). Child language, aphasia and phonological universals. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Kent, R. D. & Bauer, H. R. (1985). Vocalizations of one-year-olds. Journal of Child Language 12, 491526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Locke, J. L. (1983). Phonological acquisition and change. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Marcos, H. (1987). Communicative functions of pitch range and pitch direction in infants. Journal of Child Language 14, 255–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCune, L. (1989). Toward an integrative theory of early language acquisition: evidence from longitudinal trends in vocal behavior. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Kansas City, Missouri.Google Scholar
McShane, J. (1980). Learning to talk. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Menn, L. (1976). The semantics of intonation contour. Paper presented at the Meeting of the Linguistic Society of America, Oswego, New York.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1985). Making sense: the acquisition of shared meaning. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. & Lucariello, J. (1985). The development of meaning in first words. In Barrett, M. (ed.), Children's single-word speech. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Oller, D. K. (1986). Metaphonology and infant vocalizations. In Lindblom, B. & Zetterstrom, R. (eds), Precursors of early speech. New York: Stockton.Google Scholar
Oller, D. K., Wieman, L. A., Doyle, W. J. & Ross, C. (1976). Infant babbling and speech. Journal of Child Language 3, 111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1945/1951). Play, dreams and imitation in childhood (Gattegno, C. & Hodgson, F. M., trans.) London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Roug, L., Landberg, I. & Lundberg, L. J. (1989). Phonetic development in early infancy: a study of four Swedish children during the first eighteen months of life. Journal of Child Language 16, 1940.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scollon, R. (1976). Conversations with a one-year-old: a case study of the developmental foundation of syntax. Hawaii: The University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stark, R. E. (1981). Infant vocalization: a comprehensive view. Infant Mental Health Journal 2, 118–28.3.0.CO;2-5>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stoel-Gammon, C. & Cooper, J. A. (1984). Patterns of early lexical and phonological development. Journal of Child Language II, 247–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vihman, M. M., Ferguson, C. A. & Elbert, M. (1986). Phonological development from babbling to speech: common tendencies and individual differences. Applied Psycho-linguistics 7, 340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vihman, M. M. & Greenlea, M. (1987). Individual differences in phonological development: ages one and three years. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 30, 503–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vihman, M. M., Macken, M. A., Miller, R., Simmons, H. & Miller, J. (1985). From babbling to speech: a reassessment of the continuity issue. Language 61, 397445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werner, H. & Kaplan, B. (1963). Symbol formation. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Winitz, H. & Irwin, O. C. (1958). Syllabic and phonetic structure of infants' early words. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 1, 250–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed