Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-qxdb6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T10:33:31.860Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Mother conversational behaviour as a function of interactional intent*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Lynda McDonald
Affiliation:
Wright State University
Diana Pien
Affiliation:
Michael Reese Hospital and Medical Center

Abstract

Previous research has suggested that the syntactic and semantic characteristics of mother speech are determined by the underlying intention of mothers to control (Newport 1977) or to converse with (Snow 1977) the child. The present study attempts to delineate the structure among functionally categorized mother conversational behaviours, especially those which might reflect the mother's intention to control or converse with her child. The utterances of II mothers with their children (aged 2; 5–3; 0) were categorized according to their illocutionary force or function. Conversational parameters, such as topic change rate and talkativeness, were also measured. Intercorrelations among all these mother variables showed a polarized pattern, reflecting two predominant mother intentions for conversational interaction: the control of the child's physical actions, and the elicitation of his conversational participation. Implications of the findings, especially those indicating the incompatibility of controlling and conversation-eliciting intentions, are discussed.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1982

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

Portions of this paper were submitted by the first author in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the Ph.D. degree at the University of Oregon, and were presented at the convention of the Society for Research in Child Development, Boston, April 1981. Our thanks to Dr Solomon Fulero, who provided very helpful critical and editorial assistance, and to our undergraduate assistants, especially Vicki Hult. Address for correspondence: L. McDonald, Psychology Deparment, Wright State University, Dayton, Ohio, 45435, U.S.A.

References

REFERENCES

Austin, J. L. (1962). How to do things with words. New York: O.U.P.Google Scholar
Bateson, M. (1975). Mother–infant exchanges: the epigenesis of conversational interaction. AnnNYAcSci 263. 101–13.Google ScholarPubMed
Bellinger, D. (1979). Changes in the explicitness of mothers' directives as children age. JChLang 6. 443–58.Google Scholar
Bellinger, D. (1980). Consistency in the pattern of change in mothers' speech: some discriminant analyses. JChLang 7. 469–87.Google Scholar
Bloom, L., Rocissano, L. & Hood, L. (1976). Adult–child discourse: developmental interaction between information processing and linguistic knowledge. CogPsych 8. 521–52.Google Scholar
Bradley, R. H., Caldwell, B. M. & Elardo, R. (1977). Home environment, social status and mental test performance. JEdPsychol 69. 697701.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R. (1977). Introduction. In Snow, C. & Ferguson, C. (eds), Talking to children: language input and acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. (1977). Early social interaction and language acquisition. In Schaffer, H. R. (ed.), Studies in mother–infant interaction. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (1973). Interactions between mothers and their young children: characteristics and consequences. Monogr. Soc. Res. Ch. Devel. 38. Nos. 67.Google Scholar
Cross, T. (1977). Mothers' speech adjustments: the contribution of selected child listener variables. In Snow, C. & Ferguson, C. (eds), Talking to children: language input and acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Cross, T. (1978). Mothers' speech and its association with rate of linguistic development in young children. In Waterson, N. & Snow, C. (eds), The development of communication. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Dore, J. (1973). A developmental theory of speech act production. TNYAcSci 35. 623–30.Google Scholar
Dore, J. (1978). Variation in preschool children's conversational performances. In Nelson, K. (ed.), Children's language, Vol. 1. New York: Gardner Press.Google Scholar
Dore, J., Gearhart, M. & Newman, D. (1978). The structure of nursery school conversation. In Nelson, K. (ed.), Children's language, Vol. 1. New York: Gardner Press.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. (1976). Is Sybil there? The structures of some American English directives. LangSoc 5. 2566.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. (1977). Wait for me, roller skate! In Ervin-Tripp, S. & Mitchell-Kernan, C. (eds), Child discourse. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Freedle, R. & Lewis, M. (1977). Prelinguistic conversations. In Lewis, M. & Rosenblum, L. (eds), Interaction, conversation, and the development of language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Furrow, D., Nelson, K. & Benedict, H. (1979). Mothers' speech to children and syntactic development: some simple relationships. JChLang 6. 423–42.Google Scholar
Garvey, C. (1975). Requests and responses in children's speech. JChLang 2. 4160.Google Scholar
Garvey, C. (1977). Contingent queries. In Lewis, M. & Rosenblum, L. (eds), Interaction, conversation, and the development of language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Garvey, C. & BenDabba, M. (1974). Effects of age, sex, and partner on children's dyadic speech. ChDev 45. 1159–61.Google ScholarPubMed
Gelman, R. & Shatz, M. (1977). Appropriate speech adjustments: the operation of conversational constraints on talk to two-year-olds. In Lewis, M. & Rosenblum, L. (eds), Interaction, conversation, and the development of language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Holzman, M. (1972). The use of interrogative forms in the verbal interaction of three mothers and their children. JPsycholingRes 1. 311–36.Google Scholar
Hubbell, R. (1977). On facilitating spontaneous talking in young children. JSHD 42. 216–31.Google Scholar
Jaffee, J. & Feldstein, S. (1970). Rhythms of dialogue. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Jefferson, G. (1972). Side sequences. In Sudnow, D. (ed.), Studies in social interaction. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Kaye, K. (1977). Toward the origin of dialogue. In Schaffer, H. R. (ed.), Studies in mother–infant interaction. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Lord, C. (1975). Is talking to baby more than baby talk? Paper presented at Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development,Denver,Colorado.Google Scholar
McDonald, L. (in press). Style and stability in mother conversational behaviour: a study of individual differences. JChLang. 9, No. 3.Google Scholar
McNemar, Q. (1969). Psychological statistics. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Mitchell-Kernan, C. & Kernan, K. (1977). Pragmatics of directive choice among children. In Ervin-Tripp, S. & Mitchell-Kernan, C. (eds), Child discourse. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monogr. Soc. Res. Ch. Devel. 38. Nos. 12.Google Scholar
Newport, E. (1977). Motherese: the speech of mothers to young children. In Castellan, N. Pisoni, D. & Potts, G. (eds), Cognitive theory, Vol. 2. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Newport, E., Gleitman, H. & Gleitman, L. (1977). Mother, I'd rather do it myself: some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style. In Snow, C. & Ferguson, C. (eds), Talking to children: language input and acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Pawlby, S. (1977). Imitative interaction. In Schaffer, H. R. (ed.), Studies in mother–infant interaction. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Sacks, H. & Schegloff, E. (1974). Two preferences in the organization of references to persons in conversation and their interaction. In Avison, N. H. & Wilson, R. J. (eds), Ethnomethodology, labelling theory, and deviant behaviour. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Searle, J. (1969). Speech acts: an essay in the philosophy of language. New York: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Searle, J. (1975). Indirect speech acts. In Cole, P. & Morgan, J. (eds), Syntax and semantics. Vol. 3. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Shatz, M. (1977). How to do things by asking: form–function relations in mothers' questions to children. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development.Google Scholar
Shatz, M. & Gelman, R. (1977). Beyond syntax: the influence of conversational constraints on speech modifications. In Ferguson, C. & Snow, C. (eds), Talking to children: language input and acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Sinclair, J. & Coulthard, M. (1975). Towards an analysis of discourse: the English used by teachers and pupils. London: O.U.P.Google Scholar
Snow, C. (1972). Mothers' speech to children learning language. ChDev 43. 549–65.Google Scholar
Snow, C. (1977). The development of conversation between mothers and babies. JChLang 4. 122.Google Scholar
Stern, D. (1977). The infant's stimulus world during social interaction. In Schaffer, H. R. (ed.), Studies in mother–child interaction. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Stern, D., Jaffe, J., Beebe, B. & Bennett, S. (1975). Vocalizing in unison and in alternation: two modes of communication with the mother–infant dyad. AnnNYAcSci 263. 80100.Google ScholarPubMed
Watson, J. (1979). Perception of contingency as a determinant of social responsiveness. In Thoman, E. (ed.), Origins of the infant's social responsiveness. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar