Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-45l2p Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T03:32:52.969Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The linguistic maturity of 11-year-olds: some analysis of the written compositions of children in the National Child Development Study*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

K. Richardson
Affiliation:
National Children's Bureau, London
M. Calnan
Affiliation:
National Children's Bureau, London
J. Essen
Affiliation:
National Children's Bureau, London
L. Lambert
Affiliation:
National Children's Bureau, London

Abstract

The written compositions of 11-year-olds in the National Child Development Study were analysed using the T-unit length (Hunt 1965) as a measure of syntactic maturity, and composition length as a measure of productivity. Only a weak association was found between mean T-unit length (MTUL) and composition length. There were no statistically significant regional differences in either composition length or MTUL. There were statistically significant sex and social class differences in composition length but not in MTUL. Composition length was associated with Reading Comprehension, General Ability test scores, and teachers' ratings of certain scholastic aptitudes. MTUL was associated only slightly or not at all with these measures. The results are discussed in relation to cognitive and linguistic development.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1976

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

We are grateful to Harvey Goldstein for carrying Out the statistical analyses and for preparing the Appendix.

We would like to thank Dr Harold Rosen of the University of London Institute of Education, who first brought our attention to the index of linguistic maturity adopted in this study, and who has provided valuable suggestions regarding its use. We thank other members of staff of the National Children's Bureau for useful comments and criticism. This research was financed by grants from the DHSS, DES and SSRC which are gratefully acknowledged.

References

REFERENCES

Anderson, J. (1939). An evaluation of various indices of linguistic development. ChDev 8. 62–8.Google Scholar
Bernstein, B. (1961). Social structure, language and learning. EducRes 3. 163–76.Google Scholar
Butler, N. R. & Alberman, E. D. (1969). Perinatal problems. London & Edinburgh: Livingstone.Google Scholar
Butler, N. R., Peckham, C. & Sheridan, M. (1973). Speech defects in children aged 7 years; a national study. BMJ 3. 253–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Callary, R. E. (1974). Status perception through syntax. L & S 17. 187–92.Google ScholarPubMed
Cazden, C. B. (1972). Child language and education. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic structures. The Hague: Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T.Google Scholar
Christensen, F. (1968). The problem of defining a mature style. English Journal 57. 411.Google Scholar
Davie, R. (1973). Eleven years of childhood. StatNews 22 (08). 37.Google Scholar
Davie, R., Butler, N. R. & Goldstein, H. (1972). From birth to seven. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Ginsberg, H. (1970). The myth of the deprived child. New York: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Gipps, C. & Ewen, E. (1974). Scoring written work in English as a second language: the use of the T-unit. EducRes 16. 121–5.Google Scholar
Gooch, S. & Pringle, M. L. K. (1966). Four years on. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Griffin, W. J. (1968). Children's development of syntactic control. In Rosenberg, S. & Koplin, J. H. (eds), Developments in applied psycholinguistics research. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Harrel, L. E. (1957). A comparison of the development of oral and written language in school-age children. Monogr. Soc. Res. Ch. Devel. 22, No. 3.Google Scholar
Heider, F. K. & Heider, G. M. (1940). A comparison of sentence structure of deaf and hearing children. PsycholMonogr 52. 42103.Google Scholar
Hunt, K. W. (1965). Grammatical structure at 3 grade levels. Illinois: National Council of Teachers of English.Google Scholar
Hunt, K. W. (1970). Syntactic maturity in school children and adults. Monogr. Soc. Res. Ch. Devel. 35. No. 1.Google Scholar
La Brant, L. L. (1933). A study of certain language developments of children in grades four to twelve inclusive. Genet Psychol Monogr 14. 387491.Google Scholar
Labov, W. (1969). The logic of non-standard English. Georgetown Round Table on Language and Linguistics 22. 131.Google Scholar
Lawton, D. (1968). Social class, language and education. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
McCarthy, D. (1954). Language development in children. In Carmichael, L. (ed.), Manual of child psychology. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Mellon, J. C. (1967). Transformational sentence-combining: a method for enhancing the development of syntactic fluency in English composition. Cooperative research project, No. 5-8418, Harvard University, Cambridge, Mass.: Office of English Education and Laboratory for Research in Instruction.Google Scholar
Menyuk, P. (1963). Syntactic rules used by children. ChDev 35. 533–46.Google Scholar
Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. PsychRev 63. 8197.Google ScholarPubMed
Myklebust, H. R. (1965). Development and disorders of written language. Vol. I. New York & London: Grune & Stratton.Google Scholar
Myklebust, H. R. (1973). Development and disorders of written language. Vol. II. New York & London: Grune & Stratton.Google Scholar
O'Donnell, R. C., Griffin, W. J. & Norris, R. C. (1967). The syntax of kindergarten and elementary school children: a transformational analysis. Illinois: National Council of Teachers of English.Google Scholar
Pask, G. & Lewis, B. (1964). Communication in problem-solving groups. In Jones, C. (ed.), Conference on design methods. Oxford: Pergamon.Google Scholar
Peltz, F. M. (1973). The effect upon comprehension of repatterning based upon students' writing patterns. ReadingResQuart 9. 603–19.Google Scholar
Percival, E. (1966). The dimensions of ability in English composition. EducRev 18. 205–12.Google Scholar
Peckham, C. (1973). Speech defects in a national sample of children aged seven years. BjDisComm 8. 28.Google Scholar
Potter, R. R. (1967). Sentence structure and phrase quality: an exploratory study. Research in the teaching of English 1. 1728.Google Scholar
Read, C. E. (1971). The learning of language. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Robinson, W. P. (1965). The elaborated code in working class. L & S 8. 243–52.Google ScholarPubMed
Rosen, H. (1969). An investigation of the effects of differentiated writing assignments on the performance in English composition of a selected group of 15–16-year-old pupils. Ph.D. Thesis, University of London.Google Scholar
Rosen, H. (1972). Language and class: a critical look at the theories of Basil Bernstein. Bristol: Falling Wall Press.Google Scholar
Rosen, H. (1973). Written language and the sense of audience. EducRes 15. 177–87.Google Scholar
Sampson, O. C. (1964 a). Written composition at so years as an aspect of linguistic development. BJEdPsych 24. 143–50.Google Scholar
Sampson, O. C. (1964 b). A linguistic study of the written compositions of ten year old children. L & S 7. 176–82.Google Scholar
Siler, E. R. (1973). The effects of syntactic and semantic constraints on the oral reading performance of second and fourth graders. ReadingResQuart 9. 583601.Google Scholar
Smedley, D. A. (1968). Language and social class among grammar school children. BJEdPsych 39. 195–6.Google Scholar
Smith, W. L. (1974). Syntactic recoding of passages written at 3 levels of complexity. JExpEduc 43. 6672.Google Scholar
Van der Geest, T., Gerstel, R., Appel, R., & Tervoort, B. Th. (1973). The child's communicative competence. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Veness, T. (1962). School leavers. London: Methuen.Google Scholar