Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-dnltx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T08:03:46.728Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Infant-directed speech (IDS) vowel clarity and child language outcomes*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 December 2016

KELLY M. HARTMAN
Affiliation:
University of Maryland, USA
NAN BERNSTEIN RATNER*
Affiliation:
University of Maryland, USA
ROCHELLE S. NEWMAN
Affiliation:
University of Maryland, USA
*
Address for correspondence: Nan Bernstein Ratner, University of Maryland – Hearing and Speech, 0100 Lefrak Hall College Park Maryland 20742, United States. e-mail: nratner@umd.edu

Abstract

There have been many studies examining the differences between infant-directed speech (IDS) and adult-directed speech (ADS). However, investigations asking whether mothers clarify vowel articulation in IDS have reached equivocal findings. Moreover, it is unclear whether maternal speech clarification has any effect on a child's developing language skills. This study examined vowel clarification in mothers’ IDS at 0;10–11, 1;6, and 2;0, as compared to their vowel production in ADS. Relationships between vowel space, vowel duration, and vowel variability and child language outcomes at two years were also explored. Results show that vowel space and vowel duration tended to be greater in IDS than in ADS, and that one measure of vowel clarity, a mother's vowel space at 1;6, was significantly related to receptive as well as expressive child language outcomes at two years of age.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This work was supported by NSF grant BCS 0745412 to the University of Maryland, and served as part of a master's thesis by the first author. We thank Dr Tess Wood for many helpful discussions. We also thank the members of the Language Development Lab for assistance in recruiting, scheduling, and testing families, and particularly thank Devon Brunson and Giovanna Morini for project oversight, Kerry McColgan, Julie Sampson, Jenna Poland, Christina Royster, and Anna Synnestvedt for their help with analysis of acoustic variables, and Lisa Tuit, Amelie Bail, Jennifer Coon, and Sean Hendricks for testing the two-year-olds.

References

REFERENCES

Bard, E. G. & Anderson, A. H. (1983). The unintelligibility of speech to children. Journal of Child Language 10(2), 265–92.Google Scholar
Bell, A., Brenier, J. M., Gregory, M., Girand, C. & Jurafsky, D. (2009). Predictability effects on durations of content and function words in conversational English. Journal of Memory and Language 60(1), 92111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernstein Ratner, N. (1984). Patterns of vowel modification in mother–child speech. Journal of Child Language 11(3), 557–78.Google Scholar
Bernstein Ratner, N. (1986). Durational cues which mark clause boundaries in mother–child speech. Journal of Phonetics 14, 303–9.Google Scholar
Boersma, P & Weenink, D (2015). Praat: doing phonetics by computer [Computer program]. Version 5.4.17, retrieved 20 August 2015 from <http://www.praat.org/>..>Google Scholar
Bouchon, C., Floccia, C., Fux, T., Adda-Decker, M. & Nazzi, T. (2015). Call me Alix, not Elix: vowels are more important than consonants in own-name recognition at 5 months. Developmental Science 18(4), 587–98.Google Scholar
Burnham, E., Wieland, E., Kondaurova, M., McAuley, J., Bergeson, T. & Dilley, L. (2015). Phonetic modification of vowel space in storybook speech to infants up to 2 years of age. Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research 58(2), 241–53.Google Scholar
Cooper, R. P., Abraham, J., Berman, S. & Staska, M. (1997). The development of infants’ preference for motherese. Infant Behavior and Development 20(4), 477–88.Google Scholar
Cristia, A. (2010). Phonetic enhancement of sibilants in infant-directed speech. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 128(1), 424–34.Google Scholar
Cristia, A. (2013). Input to language: the phonetics and perception of infant-directed speech. Language and Linguistics Compass 7(3), 157–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cristia, A. & Seidl, A. (2014). The hyperarticulation hypothesis of infant-directed speech. Journal of Child Language 41(4), 913–34.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M. & Dunn, D. M. (2007). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test: Fourth Edition. Minneapolis, MN: Pearson Assessments.Google Scholar
Englund, K. & Behne, D. (2006). Changes in infant directed speech in the first six months. Infant and Child Development 15(2), 139–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, J., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J, Pethick, S. & Reilly, J. (1993). The MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: user's guide and technical manual. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.Google Scholar
Ferguson, S. H. & Kewley-Port, D. (2007). Talker differences in clear and conversational speech: acoustic characteristics of vowels. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 50(5), 1241–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernald, A. (1985). Four-month-old infants prefer to listen to motherese. Infant Behavior and Development 8(2), 181–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernald, A. (2000). Speech to infants as hyperspeech: knowledge-driven processes in early word recognition. Phonetica 57, 242–54.Google Scholar
Fernald, A. & Mazzie, C. (1991). Prosody and focus in speech to infants and adults. Developmental Psychology 27(2), 209–21.Google Scholar
Fernald, A. & Simon, T. (1984). Expanded intonation contours in mothers’ speech to newborns. Developmental Psychology 20(1), 104–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, C., Meakins, F. & Muawiyath, S. (2012). Learning vowel categories from maternal speech in Gurindji Kriol. Language Learning 62(4), 1052–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirchhoff, K. & Schimmel, S. (2005). Statistical properties of infant-directed versus adult-directed speech: insights from speech recognition. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 117(4), 22382246.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K., Andruski, J. E., Chistovich, I. A., Chistovich, L. A., Kozhevnikova, E. V., Ryskina, V. L., Stolyarova, E. I., Sundberg, U. & Lacerda, F. (1997). Cross-language analysis of phonetic units in language addressed to infants. Science 227(5326), 684–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lahey, M. & Ernestus, M. (2014). Pronunciation variation in infant-directed speech: phonetic reduction of two highly frequent words. Language Learning and Development 10(4), 308–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lam, C. & Kitamura, C. (2010). Maternal interactions with a hearing and hearing-impaired twin; similarities and differences in speech input, interaction quality, and word production. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 53(3), 543555.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lam, C. & Kitamura, C. (2012). Mommy, speak clearly: induced hearing loss shapes vowel hyperarticulation. Developmental Science 15(2), 212–21.Google Scholar
Lindblom, B. (1990). Explaining phonetic variation: a sketch of the H&H theory. In Hardcastle, W. & Marchal, A. (eds), Speech production and speech modeling, 403–39. Kluwer: Dordrecht.Google Scholar
Liu, H., Kuhl, P. K. & Tsao, F. (2003). An association between mothers’ speech clarity and infants’ speech discrimination skills. Developmental Science 6(3), F1F10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, H., Tsao, F. & Kuhl, P. K. (2009). Age-related changes in acoustic modifications of Mandarin maternal speech to preverbal infants and five-year-old children: a longitudinal study. Journal of Child Language 36(4), 909–22.Google Scholar
Ma, W., Golinkoff, R. M., Houston, D. M. & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2011). Word learning in infant-and adult-directed speech. Language Learning and Development 7(3), 185201.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2015). The CHILDES Project: tools for analyzing talk. CLAN manual. Retrieved from <http://childes.talkbank.org/manuals/clan.pdf>..>Google Scholar
Malsheen, B. (1980). Two hypotheses for phonetic clarification in the speech of mothers to children. In Yeni-Komshian, G., Kavanagh, J. & Ferguson, C. (eds), Child phonology, Vol. 2, 173–84. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Martin, A., Schatz, T., Versteegh, M., Miyazawa, K., Mazuka, R., Dupoux, E. & Cristia, A. (2015). Mothers speak less clearly to infants than to adults: a comprehensive test of the hyperarticulation hypothesis. Psychological Science 26(3), 341–7.Google Scholar
Martin, N. & Brownell, R. (2010). Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test–4th edition. Novato, CA: Academic Therapy Publications.Google Scholar
Mazuka, R., Igarashi, Y., Martin, A. & Utsugi, A. (2015). Infant-directed speech as a window into the dynamic nature of phonology. Laboratory Phonology 6(3/4), 281303.Google Scholar
McMurray, B., Kovack-Lesh, K. A., Goodwin, D. & McEchron, W. (2013). Infant directed speech and the development of speech perception: Enhancing development or an unintended consequence? Cognition 129(2), 362–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Multon, Karen D. (2010). Interrater reliability. In Salkind, N. J. (ed.), Encyclopedia of research design, 627–9. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Newman, R. S., Rowe, M. L. & Bernstein Ratner, N. (2016). Input and uptake at 7 months predicts toddler vocabulary: the role of child-directed speech and infant processing skills in language development. Journal of Child Language 43(5), 1158–73.Google Scholar
Pollack, I. & Pickett, J. M. (1964). Intelligibility of excerpts from fluent speech: auditory vs. structural context. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior 3, 7984.Google Scholar
R Development Core Team (2008). R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing,Vienna, Austria. Retrieved 25 March 2016 from <http://www.R-project.org>..>Google Scholar
Schmale, R., Seidl, A. & Cristia, A. (2015). Mechanisms underlying accent accommodation in early word learning: evidence for general expansion. Developmental Science 18(4), 664–70.Google Scholar
Singh, L., Nestor, S., Parikh, C. & Yull, A. (2009) Influences of infant-directed speech on early word recognition. Infancy 14(6), 654–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soderstrom, M., Blossom, M., Foygel, R. & Morgan, J. L. (2008). Acoustical cues and grammatical units in speech to two preverbal infants. Journal of Child Language 35, 869902.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Song, J. Y., Demuth, K. & Morgan, J. (2010). Effects of the acoustic properties of infant-directed speech on infant word recognition. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 128(1), 380400.Google Scholar
Stevens, K. N. & House, A. S. (1963). Perturbation of vowel articulations by consonantal context: an acoustical study. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 6(2), 111–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thiessen, E. D., Hill, E. A. & Saffran, J. R. (2005). Infant-directed speech facilitates word segmentation. Infancy 7(1), 5371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, Y., Seidl, A. & Cristia, A. (2015). Acoustic-phonetic differences between infant- and adult-directed speech: the role of stress and utterance position. Journal of Child Language 42(4), 821–42.Google Scholar
Wieland, E., Burnham, E., Kondaurova, M., Bergeson, T. & Dilley, L. (2015). Vowel space characteristics of speech directed to children with and without hearing loss. Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research 58(2), 254–67.Google Scholar
Xu, N., Burnham, D., Kitamura, C. & Vollmer-Conna, U. (2013). Vowel hyperarticulation in parrot-, dog- and infant-directed speech. Anthrozoos 26(3), 373–80.Google Scholar
Xu Rattanasone, N., Burnham, D. & Reilly, R. G. (2013). Tone and vowel enhancement in Cantonese infant-directed speech at 3, 6, 9, and 12 months of age. Journal of Phonetics 41(5), 332–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Hartman supplementary material

Appendix

Download Hartman supplementary material(File)
File 25.3 KB