Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-gvh9x Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-16T10:20:53.507Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Four early stages in the development of Li negation*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Henning Wode
Affiliation:
Kiel University

Abstract

No reasonably successful theory of the acquisition of negation seems to have yet been proposed. Most studies describe post hoc what has taken place; but they fail to go on to suggest a theory that will predict what will take place. McNeill seems to be the only one to have explicitly aimed at a theory with at least a certain amount of predictive capacity, but his views leave much to be desired. This paper outlines an alternative proposal to cover four very early stages for the acquisition of negation systems in natural languages. It emphasizes the formal linguistic devices as the major variables that determine the various language-specific developmental sequences.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1977

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

For help, criticism and comments I am indebted to various people in our research group, notably Sascha Felix, Dietrich Lange, Dieter Furkmann, Christa Meyer, Theo Schmitz, Ocke Bohn and Detlef Ufert.

References

REFERENCES

Antinucci, F. & Volterra, V. (1973). Lo sviluppo della negazione nel linguaggio infantile: uno studio pragmatico. Istituto di Psicologia, CNR, Rome.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. M. (1970). Language development: form and function in emerging grammars. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. F. (1973). Early syntactic development: a cross-linguistic study with special reference to Finnish. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Gheorgov, I. A. (1908). Ein Beitrag zur grammatischen Entwicklung der Kindersprache. Arch. ges. Psychol. 11 242432.Google Scholar
Grégoire, A. (1937). L'apprentissage du langage. I. Les deux premières années. Paris: Droz.Google Scholar
Grégoire, A. (1947). L'apprentissage du langage. II. La troisième année et les années suivantes. Paris: Droz.Google Scholar
Grimm, H. (1973). Strukturanalytische Untersuchung der Kindersprache. Bern: Huber.Google Scholar
Gvozdev, A. N. (1949). Formirovaniye u rebenka grammaticheskogo stroya russkogo yazyka. 2 parts. Moscow: Akad. Pedag. Nauk RSFSR.Google Scholar
Kaper, W. (1959). Kindersprachforschung mit Hilfe des Kindes: einige Erscheinungen der kindlichen Spracherwerbung erläutert im Lichte des vom Kinde gezeigten Interesses für Sprachliches. Groningen: Wolters.Google Scholar
Kaper, W. (1975). Negatie in de Kindertaal. Forum der Letteren 16. 1844.Google Scholar
Klima, E. S. & Bellugi, U. (1966). Syntactic regularities in the speech of children. In Lyons, J. & Wales, R. J. (eds), Psycholinguistics papers. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Lange, S. & Larsson, K. (1973). Syntactical development of a Swedish girl Embla, between 20 and 42 months of age. I: age 20–25 mo. Project Child Language Syntax, Report No. 1. Stockholm University: Institutionen för Nordiska Sprak.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. (1970). The acquisition of language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. & McNeill, N. B. (1968). What does a child mean when he says ‘no’? In Zale, E. M. (ed.), Proceedings of the conference on language and language behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Neugebauer, H. (1914). Sprachliche Eigenbildungen meines Sohnes. Z. Kinderforschung 19. 174–81, 242–6, 360–70.Google Scholar
Omar, M. K. (1970). The acquisition of Egyptian Arabic as a native language. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Park, T. Z. (1970). The acquisition of German syntax. Working Paper, Psychological Institute, University of Münster, West Germany.Google Scholar
Ramge, H. (1973). Spracherwerb: Grundziige der Sprachentwicklung des Kindes. Tubingen: Niemeyer.Google Scholar
Rūķe-Draviņa, V. (1963). Sprachentwicklung bei Kleinkindern. I. Syntax: Beitrag auf der Grundlage lettischen Sprachmaterials. Lund: Slavisk Institutionen vid Lunds Universitet.Google Scholar
Rūke-Dravina, V. (1972). The emergence of affirmation and negation in child language: some universal and language-restricted characteristics. In Ohnesorg, K. (ed.), Colloquium paedolinguisticum. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Schaerlaekens, A. M. (1973). The two-word sentence in child language development. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Scupin, E. & Scupin, G. (1907). Bubis erste Kindheit: ein Tagebuch. Leipzig: Grieben.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1966). Abstracts of Soviet studies of child language. In Smith, F. & Miller, G. A. (eds), The genesis of language. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T.Google Scholar
Stern, C. & Stern, W. (1907). Die Kindersprache. Leipzig: Barth.Google Scholar
Stickel, G. (1970). Untersuchungen zur Negation im heutigen Deutsch. Braunschweig: Vieweg.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Volterra, V. (1972). Il ‘no’. Prime fasi di sviluppo della negazione nel linguaggio infantile. Archive di Psicologia, Neurologia e Psichiatria, fasc.. 1.Google Scholar
Wode, H. (in press). Grammatical intonation in child language. In Sangster, L. W. & Schooneveld, C. H. (eds), Intonation and prosodic analysis (forthcoming).Google Scholar
Wode, H. & Rūķe-Draviņa, V. (in press). Why ‘Katherine no like celery’?Google Scholar
Wode, H. & Schmitz, T. (1974). Some developmental trends in the acquisition of negation in several languages. Arbeitspapiere zum Spracherwerb Nr. 3. Englisches Seminar der Universität Kiel.Google Scholar