Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-nmvwc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-07T08:11:14.220Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Form and function in the acquisition of Korean wh-questions*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 February 2009

Patricia M. Clancy*
Affiliation:
University of Southern California
*
Department of Linguistics, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA 90089–1693, USA.

Abstract

In this paper the order in which wh-questions are acquired in the production and comprehension of two Korean children, aged 1;8–2;8 and 1;10–2; 10, is analysed and compared with the available crosslinguistic data. Consistencies in acquisition order are hypothesized to be based on universals of cognitive development, which constrain the comprehension and production of wh-forms and influence the order in which mothers introduce them, and on functionally based similarities in the input of form/function pairs across children and languages. Discrepancies in acquisition order are attributed to differences in interactive style across caregivers and children, leading to different input frequencies of particular forms and individual children's selection of different forms for use.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1989

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

This research was supported by a grant from the Social Science Research Council, Korea program, and by a Sloan Foundation postdoctoral fellowship from the Center for Cognitive Science at Brown University. I also gratefully acknowledge the mothers and children who participated in this study, Keumjin Lee for her invaluable assistance collecting and transcribing the data, Pamela Downing and Murvet Enc for their comments on earlier drafts, Douglas Biber and Steven Krashen for their assistance with statistical analyses, Tae-Hyun Back for assisting in the data collection, and Doyung Choi for help with data coding.

References

REFERENCES

Bates, E., Carnaioni, L. & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 21. 205–24.Google Scholar
Beringer, G. & Garvey, C. (1981). Relevant replies to questions: answers versus evasions. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 10. 403–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berman, R. (1985). The acquisition of Hebrew. In Slobin, D. I. (ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition. Vol. 1. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Blank, M. & Allen, D. A. (1976). Understanding ‘why’: its significance in early intelligence. In Lewis, M. (ed.), Origins of intelligence: infancy and early childhood. New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Bloom, L., Merken, S. & Wooten, J. (1982). Wh-questions: linguistic factors that contribute to the sequence of acquisition. Child Development 53. 1084–92.Google Scholar
Cairns, H. S. & Hsu, J. R. (1978). Who, why, when and how: a developmental study. Journal of Child Language 5. 477–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clancy, P. M. (1985). The acquisition of Japanese. In Slobin, D. I. (ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition. Vol. 1. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Cross, T. G. (1977). Mothers' speech adjustments: the contribution of selected child listener variables. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. (eds), Talking to children: language input and acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Dore, J. (1977). ‘Oh them sherriff’: a pragmatic analysis of children's responses to questions. In Ervin-Tripp, S. & Mitchell-Kernan, C. (eds), Child discourse. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ervin, S. & Miller, W. (1977). Early discourse: some questions about questions. In Lewis, M. & Rosenblum, L. (eds), Interaction, conversation and the development of language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. (1970). Discourse agreement: how children answer questions. In Hayes, J. R. (ed.), Cognition and the development of language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. (1976). Is Sybil there? The structure of American English directives. Language in Society 5. 2566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. (1977). Wait for me, roller skate! In Ervin-Tripp, S. & Mitchell-Kernan, C. (eds), Child discourse. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Forner, M. (1977). The mother as LAD: interaction between order and frequency of parental input and child production. In Eckman, F. R. & Hastings, A. J. (eds), Studies in first and second language acquisition. Rowley MA: Newbury.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A. & Meltzoff, A. N. (in press). Language and thought in the young child: early semantic developments and their relationship to object permanence, means-end understanding, and categorization. In Nelson, K. E. (ed.), Children's language. Vol. 6. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Heath, S. B. (1981). Questioning at home and at school: a comparative study. In Spindler, G. (ed.), Doing ethnography: educational anthropology in action. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Heath, S. B. (1983). Ways with words: language, life and work in communities and classrooms. Cambridge: C.U.P.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holzman, M. (1972). The use of interrogative forms in the verbal interaction of three mothers and their children. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 1. 311–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holzman, M. (1974). The verbal environment provided by mothers for their very young children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 20. 3142.Google Scholar
Hymes, D. (1972). On communicative competence. In Pride, J. B. & Holmes, J. (eds), Sociolinguistics. Harmondsworth: Penguin.Google Scholar
Ingram, D. (1972). The acquisition of questions and its relation to cognitive development in normal and linguistically deviant children: a pilot study. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development 4. 1318.Google Scholar
Johnson, C. E. (1981). Children's questions and the discovery of interrogative syntax. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Stanford University.Google Scholar
Miller, M. & Weissenborn, J. (1978). Pragmatic conditions on learning how to refer to localities. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development 15. 6877.Google Scholar
Mills, A. E. (1985). The acquisition of German. In Slobin, D. I. (ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition. Vol. 1. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Moerk, E. (1980). Relationships between parental input frequencies and children's language acquisition: a reanalysis of Browns data. Journal of Child Language 7. 105–18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newport, E., Gleitman, H. & Gleitman, L. (1977). Mother, I'd rather do it myself: some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style. In Snow, C. & Ferguson, C. (eds), Talking to children: language input and acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Olsen-Fulero, L. & Conforti, J. (1983). Child responsiveness to mother questions of varying type and presentation. Journal of Child Language 10. 495520.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Okubo, A. (1967). Yooji gengo no hattatsu (Children's language development). Tokyo: Tokyodoo.Google Scholar
Savić, S. (1975). Aspects of adult-child communication: the problem of question acquisition. Journal of Child Language 2. 251–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Savić, S. (1978). Strategies children use to answer questions posed by adults (Serbocroatianspeaking children from 1 to 3). In Waterson, N. & Snow, C. (eds), The development of communication. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Schieffelin, B. B. & Eisenberg, A. R. (1984). Cultural variation in children's conversations. In Schiefelbusch, R. & Pickar, J. (eds), The acquisition of communicative competence. Baltimore MD: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Schieffelin, B. B. & Ochs, E. (1986). Language socialization. Annual Review of Anthropology 15. 163–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shatz, M. (1978). Children's comprehension of their mothers' question-directives. Journal of Child Language 5. 3946.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shatz, M. (1979). How to do things by asking: form-function pairings in mothers' questions and their relation to children's responses. Child Development 50. 1093–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1973). Cognitive prerequisites for the development of grammar. In Ferguson, C. & Slobin, D. I. (eds), Studies of child language development. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Snow, C. (1978). The conversational context of language acquisition. In Campbell, R. & Smith, P. (eds), Recent advances in the psychology of language: language development and mother-child interaction. New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Tyack, D. & Ingram, D. (1977). Children's production and comprehension of questions. Journal of Child Language 4. 211–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wanner, E. & Gleitman, L. R. (1982). Language acquisition: the state of the state of the art. In Wanner, E. & Gleitman, L. (eds), Language acquisition: the state of the art. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Wode, H. (1974). Some stages in the acquisition of questions by monolingual children. In W. von Raffler-Engel (ed.), Child language1975. Special issue of Word 27. 261310.Google Scholar