Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-8bljj Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-27T12:02:02.479Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The effect of conversational setting on topic continuation in mother–child dyads*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Marilyn K. Kertoy*
Affiliation:
University of Western Ontario
Dolores Kluppel Vetter
Affiliation:
University of Wisconsin-Madison
*
University of Western Ontario, Department of Communicative Disorders, 1510 Elborn College, London, Ontario, Canada, N6G 1H1.

Abstract

Twelve children (five boys and seven girls, mean age 4;1) interacted with their mothers in two conversational settings: playing with a favourite toy and helping to prepare lunch or muffins. The percentage of two topic continuation processes – topic incorporation and collaboration – used by mothers and children was analysed. It was hypothesized that the help setting with a predetermined structure would elicit a greater percentage of topic incorporation by the dyads than the play setting. In fact, the play setting elicited a greater percentage of topic incorporation from mothers than the help setting. Children did not differ significantly in the percentage of use of topic incorporation between the two settings, but appeared to be equally proficient in incorporating topics in settings with and without a predetermined structure. The play setting also elicited more topic collaboration from the children than the help setting. While the help setting appeared to support collaborative action, the play setting permitted the dyads to be informative about the toys as well as related topics. Therefore, the two settings provided different opportunities for the dyads to continue topics.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1995

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

The authors would like to thank Gerilyn Timler for her diligent coding of the topic behaviours for this study. Thanks also to the families who kindly gave their time and shared their homes to make this research project possible.

References

REFERENCES

Brinton, B. & Fujiki, M. (1984). Development of topic manipulation skills in discourse. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 27, 350–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, G. & Yule, G. (1983). Information structure. In Brown, G. & Yule, G. (eds), Discourse processes. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: the early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. (1978). The role of dialogue in language acquisition. In Sinclair, A., Jarvella, R. & Levelt, W. (eds), The child's conception of language. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. (1983). Child's talk. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Cohen, J. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educational and Psychological Measurement 20, 3746.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cook-Gumperz, J. & Gumperz, J. (1978). Context in children's speech. In Waterson, N. & Snow, C. (eds), The development of communication. New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Corsaro, W. A. (1974). Sociolinguistic patterns in adult–child interaction. Unpublished manuscript, Indiana University, Bloomington.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. M. (1986). Activity structure as scaffolding for children's second language learning. In Cook-Gumperz, J., Corsaro, W. A. & Streeck, J. (eds), Children's worlds and children's language. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Fey, M. (1986). Language intervention with young children. San Diego, CA: College-Hill Press.Google Scholar
Fey, M. (1988). Generalization issues facing language interventionists: an introduction. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools 19, 272–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Folger, J. & Puck, S. (1976). Coding relational communication. A question approach. Paper presented at the meeting of the International Communication Association, Portland, Oregon.Google Scholar
Foster, S. (1986). Learning discourse topic management in the preschool years. Journal of Child Language 13, 231–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, S. E. & Zidonis, F. (1975). Protocols of children's language. Theory into Practice 14, 312–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallagher, T. (1983). Pre-assessment: a procedure for accommodating language use variability. In Gallagher, T. M. & Prutting, C. A. (eds), Pragmatic assessment and intervention issues in language. San Diego, CA: College-Hill Press.Google Scholar
Gill, G. (1979). Piagetian cognitive assessment: procedures from a variety of sources arranged for convenient clinical use. Unpublished manuscript, University of Wisconsin–Madison, Waisman Center on Mental Retardation and Human Development, Madison, WI.Google Scholar
Kaye, K. & Charney, R. (1981). Conversational asymmetry between mothers and children. Journal of Child Language 8, 3549.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keenan, E. O. & Schieffelin, B. B. (1976). Topic as a discourse notion: a study of topic in the conversations of children and adults. In Li, C. W. (ed.), Subject and topic. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Longhurst, T. & File, J. A. (1977). A comparison of developmental sentence scores from head-start children collected in four conditions. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools 8, 5464.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lucariello, J. (1990). Freeing talk from the here-and-now: the role of event knowledge and maternal scaffolds. Topics in Language Disorders 10, 3, 1429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McTear, M. (1985). The development of coherent dialogue. In McTear, M. (ed.), Children's conversations. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
McTear, M. & Conti-Ramsden, G. (1992). Explaining pragmatic disability: linguistic and pragmatic factors. In McTear, M. & Conti-Ramsden, G. (eds). Pragmatic disability in children. San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing.Google Scholar
Miller, J. (1981). Assessing language production in children: experimental procedures. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Miller, J. F. & Chapman, R. S. (1981). The relation between age and mean length of utterance in morphemes. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 24, 2, 154–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, J. F. & Chapman, R. S. (1985). Systematic analysis of language transcripts. Language Analysis Laboratory, Waisman Center on Mental Retardation and Human Development, The University of Wisconsin–Madison, Madison, Wisconsin.Google Scholar
Miller, J. F. & Yoder, D. (1983). The Miller-Yoder Test of Linguistic Comprehension. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Ochs, E. (1979). Introduction: what child language can contribute to pragmatics. In Ochs, E. & Schieffelin, B. (eds), Developmental pragmatics. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Paul, R. (1981). Analyzing complex sentence development. In Miller, Jon F. (ed.), Assessing language production in children: experimental procedures. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Ripich, D. & Spinelli, F. (1985). School discourse problems. San Diego, CA: College-Hill Press.Google Scholar
Staab, C. (1983). Language functions elicited by meaningful activities: a new dimension in language programs. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools 14, 164–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wanska, S. K. & Bedrosian, J. L. (1985). Conversational structure and topic performance in mother–child interaction. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 24, 579–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wanska, S. K. & Bedrosian, J. L. (1986). Topic and communicative intent in mother–child discourse. Journal of Child Language 13, 523–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wanska, S. K., Bedrosian, J. L. & Pohlman, J. C. (1986). Effects of play materials on the topic performance of preschool children. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools 17, 152–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uebersax, J. S. (1987). Diversity of decision-making models and the measurement of agreement. Psychological Bulletin 101, 1, 140–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar