Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-mwx4w Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-25T08:38:38.346Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Developmental differences in reported speech and internal state language in preschoolers’ personal narratives

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 July 2023

Megan E. WELLIVER*
Affiliation:
College of Education, University of South Florida, Tampa, FL
Alice J. DAVIDSON
Affiliation:
Psychology, Rollins College, Winter Park, FL, USA
Alexandra MCCRARY
Affiliation:
Psychology, Rollins College, Winter Park, FL, USA
*
Corresponding author: Megan E. Welliver; Email: meganwelliver@usf.edu

Abstract

The present study explored developmental differences in preschoolers’ use of reported speech and internal state language in personal narratives. Three-, four-, and five-year-olds attending a laboratory preschool shared 204 stories about ‘a time when you were happy/sad’. Stories were audio-recorded, transcribed, and coded for reported speech (direct, indirect, narrativized) and internal state language (cognitive states, total emotion terms, unique emotion terms). Personal narratives told by five-year-olds included more cognitive states and more narrativized speech than those told by three- and four-year-olds, even when accounting for children’s vocabulary skills, and that reported speech (narrativized, direct) were positively correlated with cognitive state talk. These findings highlight distinct shifts in children’s use of cognitive state talk and reported speech in personal narratives told at age five. Associations between reported speech and internal state language are both informed by and support Vygotsky’s (1978) fundamental claim that psychological processes are socially mediated by language.

Type
Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adams, S., Kuebli, J., Boyle, P. A., & Fivush, R. (1995). Gender differences in parent-child conversations about past emotions: A longitudinal investigation. Sex Roles, 33(5-6), 309323. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf01954572CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bariola, E., Gullone, E., & Hughes, E. K. (2011). Child and Adolescent Emotion Regulation: The Role of Parental Emotion Regulation and Expression. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 14(2), 198212. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-011-0092-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartsch, K., & Wellman, H. M. (1995). Children talk about the mind. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berman, R. A., & Slobin, D. I. (1996). Relating events in narrative: A cross-linguistic developmental study. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1994. Pp. xiv, 748. Language in Society, 25(1), 122125. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0047404500020479CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bird, A., & Reese, E. (2006). Emotional reminiscing and the development of an autobiographical self. Developmental Psychology, 42(4), 613626. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.4.613CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bretherton, I., & Beeghly, M. (1982). Talking about internal states: The acquisition of an explicit theory of mind. Developmental Psychology, 18(6), 906921. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.18.6.906CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bretherton, I., McNew, S., & Beeghly-Smith, M. (1981). Early person knowledge as expressed in gestural and verbal communication: When do infants acquire a “theory of mind?” In Lamb, M. E. & Sherrod, L. R. (Eds.), Infant Social Cognition: Empirical and Theoretical Orientations (pp. 333374). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1990). Acts of meaning. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Chiarella, S. S., Kristen, S., Poulin-Dubois, D., & Sodian, B. (2013). Concurrent Relations Between Perspective-Taking Skills, Desire Understanding, and Internal-State Vocabulary. Journal of Cognition and Development, 14(3), 480498. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2012.689390CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, A. J., & Welliver, M. E. (2021). Internal state language and resolution strategies in preschoolers’ narratives about sad experiences. Early Child Development and Care, 191(6), 876890. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2019.1649669CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ely, R., Gleason, J. B., & McCabe, A. (1996). “Why Didn’t You Talk to Your Mommy, Honey?”: Parents’ and children’s talk about talk. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 29 ( 1), 725. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327973rlsi2901_2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ely, R., Gleason, J. B., Narasimhan, B., & McCabe, A. (1995). Family talk about talk: Mothers lead the way. Discourse Processes, 19(2), 201218. https://doi.org/10.1080/01638539509544914CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ely, R., & McCabe, A. (1993). Remembered voices. Journal of Child Language, 20(3), 671696. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0305000900008539CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fabes, R. A., Eisenberg, N., Hanish, L. D., & Spinrad, T. L. (2001). Preschoolers’ Spontaneous Emotion Vocabulary: Relations to Likability. Early Education & Development, 12(1), 1127. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15566935eed1201_2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fivush, R., Berlin, L., McDermott Sales, J., Mennuti-Washburn, J., & Cassidy, J. (2003). Functions of parent-child reminiscing about emotionally negative events. Memory, 11(2), 179192. https://doi.org/10.1080/741938209CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fivush, R., Haden, C. A., & Reese, E. (2006). Elaborating on Elaborations: Role of Maternal Reminiscing Style in Cognitive and Socioemotional Development. Child Development, 77(6), 15681588. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00960.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fivush, R., Hazzard, A., McDermott Sales, J., Sarfati, D., & Brown, T. (2002). Creating coherence out of chaos? Children’s narratives of emotionally positive and negative events. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17(1), 119. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.854CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fivush, R., McDermott Sales, J., & Bohanek, J. G. (2008). Meaning making in mothers’ and children’s narratives of emotional events. Memory, 16(6), 579594. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210802150681CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flavell, J. H. (1999). Cognitive development: Children’s knowledge about the mind. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 2145. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.21CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Furrow, D., Moore, C., Davidge, J., & Chiasson, L. (1992). Mental terms in mothers’ and children’s speech: similarities and relationships. Journal of Child Language, 19(3), 617631. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0305000900011594CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gobbo, C., & Raccanello, D. (2010). Personal narratives about states of suffering and wellbeing: Children’s conceptualization in terms of physical and psychological domain. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25(3), 386394. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1703CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gottman, J. M., Katz, L. F., & Hooven, C. (1997). Meta-emotion: how families communicate emotionally. Choice Reviews Online, 35(01), 35-060735-0607. https://doi.org/10.5860/choice.35-0607Google Scholar
Granberg, D., & Brent, E. (1983). When prophecy bends: The preference-expectation link in U.S. presidential elections, 1952-1980. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45(3), 477491. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.45.3.477CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grueneisen, S., Wyman, E., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Conforming to coordinate: Children use majority information for peer coordination. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 33(1), 136147. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12078CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haden, C. A., Haine, R. A., & Fivush, R. (1997). Developing narrative structure in parent–child reminiscing across the preschool years. Developmental Psychology, 33(2), 295307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, C., Lecce, S., & Wilson, C. (2007). “Do you know what I want?” Preschoolers’ talk about desires, thoughts and feelings in their conversations with sibs and friends. Cognition & Emotion, 21(2), 330350. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930600551691CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Köder, F., & Maier, E. (2016). Children mix direct and indirect speech: evidence from pronoun comprehension. Journal of Child Language, 43(4), 843866. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0305000915000318CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leach, J., Howe, N., & DeHart, G. (2017). “I wish my people can be like the ducks”: Children’s references to internal states with siblings and friends from early to middle childhood. Infant and Child Development, 26(5), e2015. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2015CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leyva, D., & Nolivos, V. (2015). Chilean Family Reminiscing About Emotions and Its Relation to Children’s Self-Regulation Skills. Early Education and Development, 26(5-6), 770791. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2015.1037625CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luria, A. R. (1999). Speech development and the formation of mental processes. In Lloyd, P. & Fernyhough, C. (Eds.), Lev Vygotsky: Critical assessments: Thought and language (Vol. II, pp. 84122). Florence, KY: Taylor & Frances/Routledge. (Reprinted from A handbook of contemporary Soviet psychology, by M. Cole & I. Maltzman, Eds., 1969, New York: Basic Books).Google Scholar
Miller, S. A. (2009). Children’s understanding of second-order mental states. Psychological Bulletin, 135(5), 749773. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016854CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milligan, K., Astington, J. W., & Dack, L. A. (2007). Language and Theory of Mind: Meta-Analysis of the Relation Between Language Ability and False-belief Understanding. Child Development, 78(2), 622646. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01018.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, C., & Davidge, J. (1989). The development of mental terms: pragmatics or semantics? Journal of Child Language, 16(3), 633641. https://doi.org/10.1017/s030500090001076xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, K. (2010). Developmental Narratives of the Experiencing Child. Child Development Perspectives, 4(1), 4247. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2009.00116.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicolopoulou, A., & Richner, E. S. (2007). From Actors to Agents to Persons: The Development of Character Representation in Young Children’s Narratives. Child Development, 78(2), 412429. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01006.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nolivos, V., & Leyva, D. (2013). Fun and frustrations: Low-income Chilean parents reminiscing with their children about past emotional experiences. Actualidades En Psicología, 27(115), 3148. https://doi.org/10.15517/ap.v27i115.9278CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pascual, B., Aguado, G., Sotillo, M., & Masdeu, J. C. (2008). Acquisition of mental state language in Spanish children: a longitudinal study of the relationship between the production of mental verbs and linguistic development. Developmental Science, 11(4), 454466. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00691.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perner, J., & Wimmer, H. (1985). “John thinks that Mary thinks that…” attribution of second-order beliefs by 5- to 10-year-old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 39(3), 437471. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(85)90051-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, C., & Biggs, M. (2001). “I was really, really, really mad!” Children’s use of evaluative devices in narratives about emotional events. Sex Roles, 45(11-12), 801825. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1015692403932CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Recchia, H. E., & Howe, N. (2008). Family Talk about Internal States and Children’s Relative Appraisals of Self and Sibling. Social Development (Oxford, England), 17(4), 776794. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00451.xGoogle Scholar
Rumpf, A.-L., Kamp-Becker, I., Becker, K., & Kauschke, C. (2012). Narrative competence and internal state language of children with Asperger Syndrome and ADHD. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33(5), 13951407. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2012.03.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stein, N. L. (1988). The development of children’s story-telling skill. In Franklin, M. B. & Barten, S. S. (Eds.), Child language: A reader (pp. 282297). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2018). How children come to understand false beliefs: A shared intentionality account. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - PNAS, 115(34), 84918498. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1804761115CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M. (2019). Becoming human: A theory of ontogeny. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Vouloumanos, A., & Werker, J. F. (2007). Listening to language at birth: Evidence for a bias for speech in neonates. Developmental Science, 10(2), 159164. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00549.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Walton, M. D., & Davidson, A. J. (2017). Conflict narratives in middle childhood: the social, emotional, and moral significance of story-sharing. Routledge.Google Scholar
Walton, M. D., & Walton-Wetzel, J. (2013). Reported speech and the development of authorial voice in middle childhood. Narrative Inquiry, 23(2), 388404. https://doi.org/10.1075/ni.23.2.09walCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wellman, H. M. (2018). Theory of mind: The state of the art. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15(6), 728755. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2018.1435413CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72(3), 655684. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00304CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of Theory-of-Mind Tasks. Child Development, 75(2), 523541. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00691.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, K. T. (2007). Expressive Vocabulary Test, 2nd Ed. Circle Pines, MN: AGS Publishing.Google Scholar
Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13(1), 103128. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zaman, W., & Fivush, R. (2011). When My Mom Was a Little Girl … : Gender Differences in Adolescents’ Intergenerational and Personal Stories. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 21(3), 703716. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00709.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, F., McCabe, A., Ye, J., Wang, Y., & Li, X. (2018). A Developmental Study of the Narrative Components and Patterns of Chinese Children Aged 3–6 Years. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 48(2), 477500. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-018-9614-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar