Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-xm8r8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-22T04:31:37.708Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Combining observational and experimental approaches to the development of language and communication in rural samples: Opportunities and challenges

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 March 2023

Alejandrina CRISTIA*
Affiliation:
Laboratoire de Sciences Cognitives et de Psycholinguistique, Département d’études cognitives, ENS, EHESS, CNRS, PSL University, Paris, France
Ruthe FOUSHEE
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA
Paulina ARAVENA-BRAVO
Affiliation:
Departamento de Fonoaudiología, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile Escuela de Psicología, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile
Margaret CYCHOSZ
Affiliation:
Department of Hearing and Speech Sciences, University of Maryland, College Park, Maryland, USA Department of Linguistics, University of California, Los Angeles, California, USA
Camila SCAFF
Affiliation:
Laboratoire de Sciences Cognitives et de Psycholinguistique, Département d’études cognitives, ENS, EHESS, CNRS, PSL University, Paris, France Institute of Evolutionary Medicine, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
Marisa CASILLAS
Affiliation:
Department of Comparative Human Development, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA
*
*Corresponding author. Alejandrina Cristia, Laboratoire de Sciences Cognitives et de Psycholinguistique, Département d’études cognitives, ENS, EHESS, CNRS, PSL University, Paris, France. Email: alecristia@gmail.com

Abstract

Multiple approaches – including observational and experimental – are necessary to articulate powerful theories of learning. Our field’s key questions, which rely on these varied methods, are still open. How do children perceive and produce language? What do they encounter in their linguistic input? What does the learner bring to the task of acquisition? Considerable progress has been made for the development of spoken English (especially by North American learners). Yet there is still a great deal to discover about how children in other populations proceed, especially populations in rural settings. To examine language learning in these populations, we need a multi-method approach. However, adapting and integrating methods, particularly experimental ones, to new settings can present immense challenges. In this paper, we discuss the opportunities and challenges facing researchers who aim to use a multimethodological approach in rural samples, and what the field of language acquisition can do to promote such work.

Type
Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abels, M. (2020). Triadic interaction and gestural communication: Hierarchical and child-centered interactions of rural and urban Gujarati (Indian) caregivers and 9-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 56(10), 1817.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aravena-Bravo, P., Garcia, R., Kotera, H., Laranjo, R., Nicolas, R. K., and others (2023) Towards Diversifying Early Language Development Research: Lessons from /L+/ 2021. https://osf.io/a3dy8/Google Scholar
Brown, P. (2011). The cultural organization of attention. In The handbook of language socialization (pp. 2955). Wiley-Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brownell, R., & Academic Therapy Publications. (2000). Expressive one-word picture vocabulary test.Google Scholar
Bunce, J., Soderstrom, M., Bergelson, E., Rosemberg, C., Stein, A., Migdalek, M., & Casillas, M. (2022). A cross-cultural examination of young children’s everyday language experiences. PsyArxiv.Google Scholar
Callaghan, T., Moll, H., Rakoczy, H., Warneken, F., Liszkowski, U., Behne, T., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Early social cognition in three cultural contexts. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 76(2), vii–142. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5834.2011.00603.xGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casillas, M., Brown, P., & Levinson, S. C. (2020). Early language experience in a Tseltal Mayan village. Child Development, 91(5), 18191835.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casillas, M., Brown, P., & Levinson, S. C. (2021). Early language experience in a Papuan community. Journal of Child Language, 48(4), 792814.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Childers, J. B., Vaughan, J., & Burquest, D. A. (2007). Joint attention and word learning in Ngas-speaking toddlers in Nigeria. Journal of Child Language, 34(2), 199225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cristia, A. (2022). A systematic review suggests marked differences in the prevalence of infant-directed vocalization across groups of populations. Developmental Science, XX.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cristia, A., & Casillas, M. (2022). Non-word repetition in children learning Yélî Dnye. Language Development Research, 2(1), 69104.Google Scholar
Cristia, A., Dupoux, E., Gurven, M., & Stieglitz, J. (2019). Child-directed speech is infrequent in a forager-farmer population: A time allocation study. Child Development, 90(3), 759773.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cristia, A., Farabolini, G., Scaff, C., Havron, N., & Stieglitz, J. (2020). Infant-directed input and literacy effects on phonological processing: Non-word repetition scores among the Tsimane’. PloS one, 15(9), e0237702.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cristia, A., Foushee, R., Aravena-Bravo, P., Cychosz, M., Scaff, C., & Casillas, M. (2023). Online supplementary materials to Combining observational and experimental approaches to the development of language and communication in rural samples: Opportunities and challenges. http://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000922000617CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cychosz, M. (2021). The coarticulation-duration relationship in early Quechua speech. Journal of Phonetics, 87, 101052.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cychosz, M. (2022a). Language exposure and the development of phonological working memory. Talk given at the 96th annual meeting of the Linguistic Society of America.Google Scholar
Cychosz, M. (2022b). Language exposure predicts children’s phonetic patterning: Evidence from language shift. Language, 98(3), XX-XX.Google ScholarPubMed
Cychosz, M., Cristia, A., Bergelson, E., Casillas, M., Baudet, G., Warlaumont, A. S., Scaff, C., Yankowitz, L., & Seidl, A. (2021a). Vocal development in a large-scale, crosslinguistic corpus. Developmental Science, 24(5), e13090.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cychosz, M., Villanueva, A., & Weisleder, A. (2021b). Efficient estimation of children’s language exposure in two bilingual communities. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 64(10), 38433866.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de León, L. (2019). Nacer y crecer en Mesoamérica y Los Andes: Teorías parentales y prácticas de crianza infantil contemporáneas. México: CIESAS, Centro de Investigaciones y Estudios Superiores en Antropología Social.Google Scholar
Demuth, K. (1989). Maturation and the acquisition of the Sesotho passive. Language 65(1), 5680.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Demuth, K. (1992). The acquisition of Sesotho. In The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition (pp. 557638). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Demuth, K., Machobane, M., & Moloi, F. (2003). Learning animacy hierarchy effects in Bantu double object applicative constructions. In Linguistic Typology and Representation of African Languages (pp. 2333). Africa World Press.Google Scholar
Demuth, K., Machobane, M., Moloi, F., & Odato, C. (2005). Learning animacy hierarchy effects in Sesotho double object applicatives. Language, 81(2), 421447.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Demuth, K., Moloi, F., & Machobane, M. (2010). 3-Year-olds’ comprehension, production, and generalization of Sesotho passives. Cognition, 115(2), 238251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dixon, S., Tronick, E., Keefer, C., & Brazelton, T. B. (1982). Perinatal circumstances and newborn outcome among the Gusii of Kenya: Assessment of risk. Infant Behavior and Development, 5(1), 1132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, D. M. (2007). PPVT-4: Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. Pearson Assessments.Google Scholar
Dutra, N. B. (2021). Commentary on Apicella, Norenzayan & Henrich (2020): Who is going to run the global laboratory of the future? Evolution and Human Behavior, 42(3), 271273. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2021.04.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Espinosa Ochoa, M. R. (2017). Un acercamiento a la construcción de la circunclisis en el maya yucateco infantil. Estudios de Lingüística Aplicada, 66, 41. https://doi.org/10.22201/enallt.01852647p.2017.66.682Google Scholar
Espinosa Ochoa, M. R. (2022). Germination, early development, and creativity in the acquisition of the Yucatec Maya deictic system. Journal of Child Language, 49(1), 2437. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000920000689CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, N., & Levinson, S. C. (2009). The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32(5), 429448.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fibla Reixachs, L. (2021). Relating language input to language processes early in development: using the early language processing task in UK and India (Doctoral dissertation, University of East Anglia).Google Scholar
Foushee, R., & Srinivasan, M. (2022). Evidence for early lexical knowledge in infants primarily exposed to overheard speech. https://osf.io/kcza9/?view_only=64ed45a0da2c429eaf5b9c2f0c6f799fGoogle Scholar
Frank, M. C., Braginsky, M., Yurovsky, D., & Marchman, V. A. (2017). Wordbank: An open repository for developmental vocabulary data. Journal of Child Language, 44(3), 677694. doi:10.1017/S0305000916000209CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilkerson, J., & Richards, J. A. (2020). A Guide to Understanding the Design and Purpose of the LENA® System. LENA Foundation: Boulder, CO. Available from https://www.lena.org/wp-content/uploads/2020/07/LTR-12_How_LENA_Works.pdf.Google Scholar
Gurven, M., Stieglitz, J., Trumble, B., Blackwell, A. D., Beheim, B., Davis, H., Hooper, P., & Kaplan, H. (2017). The Tsimane health and life history project: integrating anthropology and biomedicine. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 26(2), 5473. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.21515.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holcombe, A. O. (2019). Contributorship, not authorship: Use CRediT to indicate who did what. Publications, 7(3), 48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kidd, E., & Garcia, R. (2022). How diverse is child language acquisition research?First Language, XX.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kline, M. A., Shamsudheen, R., & Broesch, T. (2018). Variation is the universal: Making cultural evolution work in developmental psychology. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1743), 20170059.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lison, P., & Tiedemann, J. (2016). OpenSubtitles2016: Extracting Large Parallel Corpora from Movie and TV Subtitles. In Proceedings of the 10th International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation (LREC 2016). Database available from https://opus.nlpl.eu/OpenSubtitles-v2018.phpGoogle Scholar
Liszkowski, U. (2011). Three lines in the emergence of prelinguistic communication and social cognition. Journal of cognitive education and psychology, 10(1), 3243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liszkowski, U., Brown, P., Callaghan, T., Takada, A., & de Vos, C. (2012). A Prelinguistic Gestural Universal of Human Communication. Cognitive Science, 36(4), 698713. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1551-6709.2011.01228.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loukatou, G., Scaff, C., Demuth, K., Cristia, A., & Havron, N. (2021). Child-directed and overheard input from different speakers in two distinct cultures. Journal of Child Language, 49(6), 11731192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oakes, L. M. (2017). Sample size, statistical power, and false conclusions in infant looking-time research. Infancy, 22(4), 436469.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ochs, E., & Kremer-Sadl, T. (2020). Ethical blind spots in ethnographic and developmental approaches to the language gap debate. Langage et Societe, (2), 3967.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Neill, D. (2008). Social organization: An overview of how we create and maintain social groups. Online tutorial, available from https://www2.palomar.edu/anthro/status/default.htmGoogle Scholar
Pencil, One (2022). The Tsimane Scholarship Fund, accessible from https://www.onepencilproject.org/tsimane-scholarship-fundGoogle Scholar
Padilla-Iglesias, C., Woodward, A. L., Goldin-Meadow, S., & Shneidman, L. A. (2021). Changing language input following market integration in a Yucatec Mayan community. PloS one, 16(6), e0252926.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pye, C. (1980). The acquisition of grammatical morphemes in Quiche Mayan. PhD Thesis, University of Pittsburgh.Google Scholar
Pye, C. (2017). The comparative method of language acquisition research. The University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pye, C., & Quixtan Poz, P. (1988). Precocious passives (and antipassives) in Quiché Mayan. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 27, 7180.Google Scholar
Salomo, D., & Liszkowski, U. (2013). Sociocultural settings influence the emergence of prelinguistic deictic gestures. Child Development, 84(4), 12961307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scaff, C., Casillas, M., Stieglitz, J., & Cristia, A. (2022). Verbal input among the Tsimane’. PsyArXiV preprint. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/mt6nzCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shneidman, L. A., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2012). Language input and acquisition in a Mayan village: How important is directed speech? Developmental Science, 15(5), 659673.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singh, L., Cristia, A., Karasik, L. B., & Oakes, L. (2021). Why are we still so WEIRD? Barriers and bridges towards a diversified science of early development. PsyArxiv. https://psyarxiv.com/hgukc/Google Scholar
Sugden, N. A., & Moulson, M. C. (2015). Recruitment strategies should not be randomly selected: empirically improving recruitment success and diversity in developmental psychology research. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
UN, Secretary-General (2017). Repositioning the United Nations development system to deliver on the 2030 Agenda: ensuring a better future for all: report of the Secretary-General. https://digitallibrary.un.org/record/1298793?ln=enGoogle Scholar
United Nations Educational, Scientific and Cultural Organization (2021). UNESCO Recommendation on Open Science (Programme and Meeting Document SC-2021/SANS COTE). Retrieved from the United Nations Educational, Scientific, and Cultural Organization website: https://unesdoc.unesco.org/ark:/48223/pf0000379949.locale=enGoogle Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Cristia et al. supplementary material

Table S1

Download Cristia et al. supplementary material(File)
File 26.3 KB